throbber
FUNDAMENTALS
`op -·
`MUSICAL
`ACOUSTICS
`
`Second, Revised Edition
`
`I
`
`•
`
`•
`
`... ,
`\ ,
`
`. .
`.
`. .
`.
`,- .
`I .
`.
`.
`,
`. .
`,
`...
`I
`•
`
`I
`
`.
`
`\
`
`.
`• .
`.
`,_
`\ ·-
`. . . .
`--.,
`.
`.
`' ··,
`.. .
`
`· Arthur H, Benade
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 1 of 9
`
`

`

`Copyright© 1976, 1990 by Virginia Benade.
`All rights reserved.
`
`.
`.
`.
`ThisDo~r edi~ion, fir~t published in 1990, is an unabridged,
`extensively comcted republication of the edition originally pub(cid:173)
`lished by Oxford University Press in 1976. A Note to the Dover
`Edition luis been added ,
`
`· Library tf Congrtss Cmaloging-in-l'flbli~ion O.au
`Bena,lci, Arthur H:
`Fundamentals of musical acoustics i Arthur H. Benade.
`.
`..
`.
`p. ·
`.cm.
`Reprint. Originally published: Nev,, York : Oxfqrd University
`Press, 1976.




`· Includes index.
`ISBN-13: 978-0-486-26484-4
`ISBN-10: o:486-26484-X
`. 1. Mus"ic...:._Acoustics and physics.
`-Ml,.3805.B328 1990
`78t2...;...:ac20
`· ·
`
`I. Tide.
`
`9040159
`. OP
`MN
`
`Manufactured in the United States by LSC Communications Book LLC
`.·
`26484X17
`. 2021
`www.doverpublications.com
`
`i
`
`i I
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 2 of 9
`
`

`

`le
`le
`es
`,t-
`.ie
`
`.a(cid:173)
`ed·
`n.g:
`.ve
`'.2)
`tly
`JW
`
`ni~
`:ed
`ea ..
`rd·,
`R
`'
`in-
`wo
`the
`
`·are
`1&er .·
`
`.ole-
`
`be-
`
`)
`i
`
`Pitch: The Simplest Musical Implication of Characteristic Oscillations
`
`t
`notice some of the rema~kabJe implica(cid:173)
`tions of the first of these observations.
`
`59
`
`fn =rif1
`One reads thiJ mathematical s~nte11ce thus: "I 1ub-11
`IJ equal to n times I sub-I," meaning that the nth
`frequency is n times a.r large as the first orie in the
`Jet.
`
`5. 5. Sounds Having Whole~Number
`Frequency Ratios
`
`Let us imagine ~hat we have available to
`· us a hypothetical stri11g which, when
`plucked or struck, vibrates ii} a family of
`characteristic damped sinusoidal oscilla(cid:173)
`tions whose frequencies are arranged in an
`exact whole-number relation; that is, Q is
`exactly 2P, R = 3P, S =4P, and so on.
`
`In the language of chapter 2, we can say
`that the repetition rate for any one of our
`idealized string's sinusoidal oscillations is
`a whole .number times the repetition rate
`associated with its lowest frequency os(cid:173)
`cillation. Let us look into what happens
`when acco1.1t1t is taken of the fact that the
`string is actually vibrating wfrh a. whole
`set of integrally :related frequencief
`Suppose that for conceptual simplicity
`we assign an imaginary drummer to each
`char_actei-isck . oscillation of our . string,
`giving him the job of tapping with a rep~
`etition rate equal to that measured for his
`"own" string oscilla_tion. The whole(cid:173)
`ntimber · relation . between
`the
`string
`frequendes then requires that the drum(cid:173)
`mer .assigned • to keep time with the_ sec(cid:173)
`ond characteristic osdllation should beat
`twice as fast a,s drwnmer number 1. Simi-
`larly drummer · 3 taps three drries as fast
`
`.
`
`.
`
`.
`
`.
`
`.
`
`.
`
`.
`
`. Digression on the Numerical Labeling of
`Natural Frequencies.
`· We can exprm this iuhole-number relationship be(cid:173)
`tween oscillation frequmcies very compactly as fol(cid:173)
`lows. If we use the -letter n to stand for any one of
`the. integers--that is, n = 1 , or 2, or 3, etc~-.
`. and if the char~cteriJtk frequendes are given the
`serially numbered names f 1, f 2, / 3 imtead of our
`alphabetical names, then the nth one of these
`frequencks can be referred to as f.,,, The desired int-
`eger relation between the suctessive string freq lien cies
`ca11 be written i11 a mathematically tidy fashion a.s
`._follows:
`
`)-----0,-----1 .,
`
`0-
`
`Fig. 5.2. Pattern Made by Tapping Rates Having a Whole-Number Relation
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 3 of 9
`
`

`

`Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics
`60
`
`Fig. 5.4, Combination of Sinusoids Having
`a· Whole-Number Frequency Relation
`
`fer by a factor of two. If our simplified
`string could be excited by some means
`that sets into motion only the first two of
`its characteristic oscillations, then the os(cid:173)
`cilloscope picture produced from a micro~
`phone in its neighborhood woul<i look
`
`as drummer 1, and so on. The upper fuur
`lines of figure 5. 2 show the timing of the
`successive taps produced by the first four
`of out set of drummers . The bottom line
`of the diagram shows the resulting rhyth(cid:173)
`mic pattern that one would hear. Every
`drummer strikes in unison with the blows
`of drummer 1, giving a strongly marked .
`beat, and drwmners 2, 4, . . . strike at
`the midpoints between
`these accented
`taps, giving a somewhat less accented
`tap. The important thing to notice is that
`the repetition rate of the complete rhyth(cid:173)
`mic pattern produced by the composite
`set of tappings is exactly the same as that
`of the lowest frequency member (see sec.
`2. 3, "Repetition Rates of Rhythmic Pat(cid:173)
`terns"). Musiciims should not find this
`idea hard to understand if they compare
`my explanation above with what they
`would expect from a rhythmic pattern
`written out as in figure 5. 3.
`Let us look now at some examples
`using sinusoidal disturbances instead of
`drum~ats . . · The top . two parts of figure
`SA show sinusoids whose frequencies dif-
`' .
`...
`" ,,
`
`• -- - -- -- -
`.
`- - -
`- - -
`
`3
`
`.1
`
`-
`
`.,.
`--I(([:._
`j-'l1,i:! ·
`
`~1,~
`
`,,Ji{(
`
`1
`
`.
`. , .
`
`U
`
`I
`
`t..
`
`~
`
`I
`
`'I.'
`t..
`
`-
`
`. , I
`IL
`
`--- - - - -
`-· -
`-
`-
`
`3
`
`-
`.
`
`3
`- - -
`
`-
`•
`
`-
`.-
`
`-
`
`ETC
`
`ETC-
`
`.
`
`ETC~
`
`El~-
`
`-
`
`-
`
`'
`-
`
`-
`
`-
`
`-
`
`-
`
`I
`
`t
`Fig. 5,3.
`
`I
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 4 of 9
`
`

`

`Pitch: The Simplest Musical Implication of Characteristic Oscillations
`
`61
`
`som~thing like the curve sqown· in the
`third part of the figure. This curve is
`two
`produced by the addition of the
`curves immediately abo~e it. Notice that
`the repetition time of tht somewnat spiky
`composite curve (and hence its repetition
`rate) is exactly that of th~ f1 component at
`the. top of the diagram. The bottom part
`of the figure shows the result of combin(cid:173)
`ing additional sinusoids, so chat ~he curve
`is that belonging to the sum of the first
`six oscillations in our specially chosen set.
`
`shown us something that will prove to be
`very important to our undt1rstanding not
`only of the physical basis of tone color
`but also of the special relationships be(cid:173)
`tween notes which underlie formal music
`all over the world: Let us set down some
`of the properties of the tlass of sounds
`that would be made by our hypothetical
`strings.
`
`1. No matter what the strength of excita(cid:173)
`tion of the various oscillations, the repetition
`race for the whole signal as it reaches a micro(cid:173)
`phone (or our ears) would be exactly that of
`the lowest frequency sinusoidal compon~nt
`that is characteristic of the string.
`.
`;.
`2. Because the net repetition rate of the
`vibration is independent of how cir where the
`string • is struck, one would always · get the
`same perceived pitch sensation for the string
`sound. This means that the pitch is ltnam•
`big11011s.
`
`Fig; 5. 5. This figure is identical with the
`Lipper three sections of figure 5 .4 except that
`the second component has been displaced.
`Note that the repetition time is unaffected by
`this change.
`
`Figur~ 5. 5 shows ·~ slightly modified
`version of the upper three sections of fig(cid:173)
`ure 5.4. This time the f2 component is
`"slid ovet" in time 'so that it no longer
`has every second upward excursion coind(cid:173)
`dent with every upward excursion of the
`f1 component. We . notice that the suin(cid:173)
`tnation · of these two oscillations gives a
`resultant pattern whose shape is different
`from the one obtained before, but once
`again we see that the repetition time is
`equal to that of the · lowest frequency (f1)
`oscillation.
`Adding components whose frequencies
`.are
`in whole-number relationships has
`
`Digression:
`Sounds with Only . Even
`.
`.. . , ·.
`H;rmonics.
`In the strictest of logic, one might ask about a po1.,
`sible inadequacy of iiem 1 above. Imagine an
`ingenioui excitation method that fails to excite the
`odd-numbered oscillatiom, so that only /2, f,i, f e,
`. , . are pre,ent. The.re may be written out as
`follws:
`f2 = 2f1 = 1 X (2f1)
`f4 = 4f1 == 2 X (2ft)
`fa == 6f1 = 3 X (2ft) . .
`etc.
`
`This 1hows that our new set of frequency compo(cid:173)
`nents is itself constructed out of integer multiples of
`a new basic frequenry whose value i.r (2/1). The
`repetition rate is thmfore doubled, . and the whole
`game begins again; We would perceive thts altered ·
`sound as having a pitch one octa11(! higher ihan the
`normally excited one. ·
`. As a practical matter, it is not particularly dif(cid:173)
`Jicult to arrange peculiar exdtation.r of the iort de(cid:173)
`scribed in the precedingpariigraph, and if one were
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 5 of 9
`
`

`

`to meet such a situation it could ea.rHy be recog- _
`nized as such with· the· help of simple auxiliary ex(cid:173)
`periments. One would need only to pluck or strike
`the string at random spots once or twice in ortkr to
`.
`find out the true nature. of the string.
`
`There is something intellect1.U1.Ily very
`attractive about the apparent simplicity of
`sounds made up of components having
`integer frequency ratios, and it ·is easy to
`devi~
`lengthy · numerological· games
`based • on their presumed properties, Be(cid:173)
`fore we· fall into this trap, however, _it
`would be advisable to find out whether
`such sounds can •in fact be generated. If
`such sounds can be · generated, we then
`-must ask whether om- ears and nervous
`system deal with them· in a. way that cor(cid:173)
`re~ponds at all with experiencing the
`,sounds from real sttfr1gs. The first ques(cid:173)
`. tion can be answe·red affirmatively in two
`.ways:
`i. A truly uniform slender string of suit(cid:173)
`able material, stretched tightly enough be(cid:173)
`tween sufficiently rigid supports, will.produce.
`sounds whose components have frequencies
`that are in very nearly perfect integer. relation.
`The sounds · from such a string differ only
`. subtly from those produced l:>y. a string vibrat(cid:173)
`ing under less formalistic conditions. That is,
`nothing. drastic happens to the perceived
`sound as long as the string has. nearly integer
`frequency relations.

`2. we· 4od that th~re is a large class of fa(cid:173)
`miliar sound• sources that Oormally produce
`sounds whbse frequenc:y components are found
`to be related in the precisely Whqle-number
`manner that we pos~ulated for our hypotheti(cid:173)
`cal iitrings. E.xamples of sources of this kind
`are very .· common. The human voice is the
`most familiar one, while the woodwind and
`brass instruments join with the violin family
`to provide · orchestral examples. Th~se · diverse
`sound sources have one common demerit in
`their na.ture that sets them apart from the
`bells, chimes, and strings we have considered
`
`Fundamentals of Musical. Acoustics
`62
`so far. Instead of simply ringing (and decay~
`-.ing away) in response to an impulsive stimu(cid:173)
`lus, all of these instruments are capable of
`producing sustained sounds. They are devices
`that are capable of converting the steady Bow
`of air from a man's lungs, or the steady rµo(cid:173)
`tion of the bow in his hand, into the osciUa(cid:173)
`tory · vibrations which give rise to the sound
`we hear. We shall see in a later chapter tpat
`only under vety special circumstances can
`such devices be persuaded to maintain steady
`oscillations whose frequency components are
`not in an ei(act whole-number relation to the
`basic repetitio~ rate.
`
`It turns out that the vast majority of
`our musical listening experiences are with
`sounds whose frequency components are .
`in exact whole-number re.lation, or very
`nearly so. It i,s not surprising, then, that
`the formal structure of music (wherever it
`has developed over the world) is, strongly
`influen~ed by the properties _ of sounds
`each ofwhich has whole-number relations
`among its components. We also find that
`many subtleties in music arise through
`the , slight
`inharmonicities which a~
`present in the tones of some. instruments. ·
`This book has opened with an inves(cid:173)
`tigatio~ of iCQpulsive and heterogeneous
`sounds from struck objects, not only ne(cid:173)
`caus~ of' the 'simplicity of initial expo~i(cid:173)
`tiori •but also' as a means for underlining
`the special nature of the sound~producers
`that man· has selected for his musical ac~
`dvities. ,It is time therefore to return to
`the ;;ounds of bells and chimes in o~der to
`coi.n'pare them with the sounds of plucked
`or •~}ruck strings.


`
`5. 6. Th~ Pitch of Chimes and Bells:
`Hints .of Pattern Recognition
`
`that the characteristic
`We have founc:I
`frequencies that make up any one sound
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 6 of 9
`
`

`

`Pitch: The Simplest M!-15ical Implication of Characteristic . Oscillations
`
`63
`
`from any one of the commoner orchestral
`1nsttuments are arrariged as exact (or ~~ry
`nearly exact) integer multiples of a certain
`basic frequency. It is i:his bask frequJ~cy
`component that determines the repetition
`rate of the sound we hear and· also, as we
`have learned, its musical pi~ch .. Let us use
`this -knowledge tci help ourselves gain
`some understanding of the' way in which
`'we assign pitches to chimes ind bells,
`whosecharacteristic trequehcies do not ar(cid:173)
`range the~selves in whoJe-number rela(cid:173)
`tionships.
`
`Digression on Terminology:
`Some Partials Are Harmonic.
`It will save a great deal of circum/oc11tion if we
`provide ourselves with some terminology carefully
`chosen for the description of the various compon"ents
`making up the sound we are ikq/ing with. First of
`all, in atJy sound mafk up of {intisoidal compo~ ·
`mnts, we will continue to assign identifying letters
`from the latter part of the alpha/Jet, or serial num:
`bet's, a.11igning them according to their order,
`beginning at ihe lowest one. That is, we will call
`· these /requenciei P, Q, R, . . , or f1, f2, fa,
`, . . SometimeJ it will be _useful to refer to these
`components -as the partials of the sound in que!fion.
`When this word is used, tue °will unikrstand th4t
`no particular relationship is to be "a.rsumed between
`the frequencies of theJe partials,· their fmJ.uencies
`may or may not have a whole-number. relationship.
`The1e components will still be referred to hy their
`serial numb~s a.r first partial ( referring to the
`componenOabeled P or f 1 ), second partial (also
`known asQ or /J.), etc.
`__ ·We tum now to the special ca.re of sounds in
`which the frequenciei of the t1arious sin11Joidal par(cid:173)
`tials are whole-n_11mber multiples . of some ba1ic .repe(cid:173)
`rate. Tin sinusoidal component . "whose
`tition
`frequenry matchei that of the ;repetition rate will be
`referred to _as the funda.piental component, and its
`frequency as the fundamental frequency : It is
`-0/ten referred to also as the first harmonic. The
`Pani;./ ,w~ie·fre,quency is exactly double that of
`the fun<k,mental will . be said to have a frequency
`whic/, is the second harmonic of the fundamental
`.
`. .
`
`'
`
`frequency. Similarly we will say that sim1Joidal
`oscitlaiions running at three times the fundamental
`frequency are vibrating at the third harmonic of
`the fundamental frequency . -
`We will have_ to be t1ery-,trict i_n o~r terminoiogy
`or endless . confusion can result. The word har(cid:173)
`monic is to be used only .when ,;;e m~an to imply
`an exact whole-number frequency relationihip. To
`help make things clear, we may notice that the par(cid:173)
`tials pf a guitar string have frequencies which are
`very nearly, but not exactly, harmonic1 of the
`.
`frequenry of the fint (lowest) partial.
`.
`
`We learned earlier in this chapter that
`musically experienced people won't neces(cid:173)
`sarily ag)'.'ee on wh.at pitch to assign co the
`sound of a grandfather clock chime . Iri'
`the context of out present understanding '
`of musical soun_d~; we may wonder
`whether the frequencies of the chimd'
`partials can be recognized by our nervous
`system as · belonging to two differently
`organized sequences. of harmonics. _That
`is, can we find hints of a series of har(cid:173)
`monics whose fundamental . corresponds to
`the approximate F3 that some listeners
`hear? Similarly, can we detect signs of a
`har_monic series .· whose fundamental im(cid:173)
`plies the pitch just ·above the (: 5 perceived
`by others? In our earlier investigation of
`this sound we recognized that the .second
`partial has a frequency consistent · with
`one of these pitch assignments while the
`two dosely spaced . partials (which were
`labeled Ra and Rb) are associated with the
`othe.r oi;:ie. _ Our ·earlier difficulty stemmed
`from -our inability to dispose of all the
`other partials making . up the tone; co_uld
`these be members of harmonic se.des
`based on the assigned pitches?
`:Figure 5 .6 shows the frequencies of aU
`the partials up through £4 (S) laid out as
`dots along a frequency scale . Above the
`frequency, axis of this diagram . we see · a
`pa,ir of arrows located at frequencies corre-
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 7 of 9
`
`

`

`·:.' 1,i!l:i:l~Ji..f1 -
`
`i1i!t
`]l~;
`
`200
`
`~
`
`Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics
`
`64
`
`i
`
`APPROX .
`
`. c5
`
`f
`
`f~
`1000
`
`1ioo
`
`f
`
`f
`
`600
`400
`FREQUENCY
`
`800
`
`f ± ±
`
`SHARP
`
`,=:
`3
`Fig. 5.6: Assignment of Pitches to Sound from a Cock Cbime
`
`sponding to a fundamental, belongirtg to
`the note C 5, and its second harmonic.
`The fundamental arrow is pointing at the
`pair of Rs, .while the arrow for the second
`
`harmonic points almost exactly at the
`
`me~ured S. It seems possible, then, that
`o,ur. ears can ,seize on the reiacionship of
`these two strong components a:nd accept
`,tl)em jointly as the two lowest members
`· (fundamental and second harmonic) of a
`. set of partials beJonging:to a sound whose
`pitch is near C5 •
`.
`.· Below the frequency axis we fin~ in
`similar fashion a sec of arrows indicating
`the frequencies making up .the set (fun~
`damental and its harmonics) belonging to
`the sharp-pitched F 3 which we associated
`with t.he measured f2 (which was labeled
`Q earlier). This time we find that the
`arrows corresponding to the fundamental,
`the third,· !lnd the sixth harmonics point
`very nearly ·at the <dots indicating the
`measured components Q; R; and S.
`Our search for integer relations among
`the frequency components of a struck
`chime rod has been reasonably successful, ·
`in . that it gives results that seem consis(cid:173)
`tent with the hypothesis that our ears as(cid:173)
`sign pitch (when possible) on the basis of
`
`they can .
`
`any · whole-number sequences
`.find.
`.
`.
`We turn our attention next to the bell
`sounds, to see whether they give any sup(cid:173)
`port to our hypothesis . that pitch is as~
`the- basis of approximately
`signed on
`w hole-nuinber frequency
`relationships.
`The individual lines of · figure 5. 7 show .
`the frequencies of the .first. five partials for
`the first five bells in the Terling Peal, laid
`out by means of dots on a frequency axis
`in exactly the same way as was done for
`the chime rod. The dashed vertical lines
`appeJing on the diagram . indicate the
`fundamental repetition frequency aqd its
`h~monics belonging to a reference sound
`whose pitch matches that of the bells as
`made; uniform. by a variable-speed record(cid:173)
`ing device_.
`•
`Inspection of the line corresponding to
`bell 1 shows that the first partial (marked
`P) has a'frequency quite close to that as(cid:173)
`swped for the fundamental. Furthermore;
`we,-, ~ee t~at partials 4 and 5 agree ex(cid:173)
`treri:iely well with harmonics 3 and 4 of
`the pitch reference tone. We note that
`partials · 2 and 3 do not seem to agree
`with any ,member of the reference har(cid:173)
`monic series.
`
`~:/t;(t
`··.){:;\
`
`,!,_,,.
`ItP
`' /],:)iii,
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 8 of 9
`
`

`

`Pitch: The Simplest Musical Implication of Characteristic Oscillations
`
`n REPETITION FREQUENCY
`t CORRESPO~DING TO P~RCEIVED P!TCH
`
`65
`
`.J
`-~ CL2
`, ' .
`!
`.J .J! ....,_ _____________ .,_~._-.... _ _ _ _ _ -MIII - - - -
`LtJ
`a)
`~4 1-----il..,_ _______ __,...,_ ______ ....+ __ _
`a:
`LtJ m
`~51------------1411~ --~--H..----------
`~
`z
`
`I
`
`.
`
`I
`'· I
`
`I
`
`800
`600
`400"
`200
`1000
`FREQUENCY
`· Fig. 5. 7. Frequency Components of Bells Adjusted to tl:ie' Same Pitch
`
`1200·
`
`Sl<ipping now to bells 3, 4, and 5, we
`find that partials 1, 2; 4, and 5 agree
`quite weil · with the fundamental and hat:
`monies 2, 3, · and 4 belonging to our
`pitch reference. Partial 3 never seems to
`fit in. Bell 2 does not show such a clear(cid:173)
`cut relation, although the frequencies of
`partials 1 and 4 are roughly equal to
`those. of the fundamental and · third har~
`nionic of our reference sound. Interest(cid:173)
`ingly enough, most listeners feel quite
`une~y about assigning pit~h to this bell,
`even though they find · no difficulty with
`the other ones.
`Looking o~er the data we come co real(cid:173)
`ize that for a bell to have a reasonably
`it can be .
`well~defined pitch (so -. that
`matched with a normal sort of tone · hav(cid:173)
`ing harmonic partials), our eats do not
`demand any particular set of component
`
`frequencies frotn it. · That is, our ears . do
`not demand that the same. (only approid.:
`iden(cid:173)
`mately harmonic) partials serve
`tically as the· ''pointers" in _the sounds for
`all the bells .. All ·chat is required is a suf•
`ficient · ~umber of sufficie11dy consisteni:
`clues. The frequencies of the skillet clang
`listed on p. 43 are similar to these bell
`sounds in chat they are not harn,1onically
`related.
`
`5. 7. Another Pitch Assigninept
`Phenomenon: The Effect of Suppressing

`· ·
`Upper or u;wer Partials
`
`In the previous section of this chapter we
`found ourselves thinking about the ways
`i~ which our ears respond to sounds fed
`to them from bells and chimes. We no(cid:173)
`deed that the ace of assigning pitch to
`
`MZ Audio, Ex. 2004, Page 9 of 9
`
`

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket