throbber
1
`
`APPLE 1032
`
`APPLE 1032
`
`1
`
`

`

`THE
`
`PSYCHOLOGY
`
`OF EVERYDAY
`
`THINGS
`
`Donald A. Norman
`
`# BasicBooks
`A Division of HarperCollinsPublishers
`
`2
`
`

`

`
`
`3
`
`

`

`CHAPTER ONE
`
`THE
`
`PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
`
`OF EVERYDAY
`
`THINGS
`
`nak
`
`e
`
`“Kenneth Olsen, the engineer who founded and
`still runs Digital Equipment Corp., confessed at
`the annual meeting that he can’t figure out how to
`heata cup of coffee in the company’s microwave
`oven, “1
`
`You Would Need an Engineering Degree
`to Figure This Out
`
`“You would need an engineering degree from MIT to workthis,”
`someoneonce told me, shaking his head in puzzlement over his brand
`new digital watch. Well,
`I have an engineering degree from MIT.
`(Kenneth Olsen has two of them,and hecan’t figure out a microwave
`oven.) Give me a few hours and I can figure out the watch. But why ,
`should it take hours? I have talked with many people whocan’tuse all
`the features of their washing machines or cameras, whocan’tfigure out
`how to work a sewing machine or a video cassette recorder, who
`habitually turn on the wrong stove burner.
`Whydo weput up with the frustrations of everyday objects, with
`objects that we can’t figure out how to use, with those neatplastic-
`wrapped packages that seem impossible to open, with doors that trap
`people, with washing machines and dryers that have become too con-
`
`4
`
`

`

`*
`
`sion of theartist.
`
`1.1 Carelman’s Coffeepot for Maso-
`chists. The French artist Jacques Carel-
`man in his series of books Catalogue
`d objets introuvables (Catalog of unfindable ob-
`jects) provides delightful examples of
`everyday things that are deliberately
`unworkable, outrageous, or otherwise
`ill-formed. Jacques Carelman: “Cof-
`feepot for Masochists.” Copyright ©
`1969-76-80 by Jacques Carelman and
`A.D.A.G.P.Paris. From Jacques Carel-
`man, Catalog of Unfindable Objects, Balland,
`éditeur, Paris-France. Used by permis-
`
`fusing to use, with audio-stereo-television-video-cassette-recorders
`that claim in their advertisements to do everything, but that make it
`almost impossible to do anything?
`The human mindis exquisitely tailored to make sense of the world.
`Giveit the slightest clue and off it goes, providing explanation,ration-
`alization, understanding. Consider the objects—books, radios, kitchen
`appliances, office machines, andlight switches—that make up our ev-
`eryday lives. Well-designed objects are easy to interpret and under-
`stand. They contain visible clues to their operation. Poorly designed
`objects can be difficult and frustrating to use. They provide no clues—
`or sometimes false clues. They trap the user and thwart the normal
`process of
`interpretation and understanding. Alas, poor design
`predominates. Theresult is a world filled with frustration, with objects
`that cannot be understood, with devices that lead to error. This book
`is an attempt to changethings.
`
`The Frustrations
`
`of Everyday Life opening doors?” Yes. I push doors that are meant to be pulled, pull
`
`doors that should be pushed, and walk into doors that shouldbe slid.
`Moreover, I see others having the same troubles—unnecessary trou-
`bles. There are psychological principles that can be followed to make
`these things understandable and usable.
`Consider the door. There is not much you can doto a door: you can
`openit or shut it. Suppose youare in an office building, walking down
`a corridor. You come to a door.In which direction does it open? Should
`you pull or push, on the left or the right? Maybe the doorslides. If so,
`in which direction? I have seen doors thatslide up into theceiling. A
`door poses only two essential questions: In which direction does it
`move? On which side should one workit? The answers should be given
`by the design, without any need for words or symbols, certainly with-
`out any need fortrial anderror.
`K Gu-}evetabal
`oy apt
`A friend told meofthe time he got trapped in the doorway ofa post
`office in a European city. The entrance was an imposing row ofperhaps »
`six glass swinging doors, followed immediately by a second, identical

`row. That’s a standard design: it helps reduce the airflow and thus
`maintain the indoor temperature of the building.
`Myfriend pushed on theside of one of the leftmost pair of outer
`doors. It swung inward, and heentered the building. Then, before he
`couldget to the next row ofdoors, he was distracted and turned around
`for aninstant. He didn’t realizeit at the time, but he had moved slightly
`to the right. So when he cameto the next door and pushed it, nothing
`happened. “Hmm,”he thought, “must be locked.” So he pushed the
`side of the adjacent door. Nothing. Puzzled, my friend decided to go
`outside again. He turned around andpushed against the side ofa door.
`Nothing. He pushed the adjacent door. Nothing. The door he hadjust
`entered no longer worked. He turned around once more andtried the
`inside doors again. Nothing. Concern, then mildpanic. He was trapped!
`Just then, a group ofpeople on the other side of the entranceway(to
`my friend’s right) passed easily through both sets of doors. My friend
`hurried over to follow their path.
`Howcould such a thing happen? A swinging door has twosides.
`Onecontains the supporting pillar and the hinge, the other is unsup-
`ported. To open the door, you must push on the unsupported edge. If
`you push on the hinge side, nothing happens. In this case, the designer
`aimed for beauty, notutility. No distracting lines, no visible pillars, no
`visible hinges. So how can the ordinary user know which side to push
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`+
`
`go"
`os
`
`2
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`“3 If I were placed in the cockpit of a modern jet airliner, myinability to
`perform gracefully and smoothly wouldneither surprise nor bother me.
`But I shouldn’t have trouble with doors and switches, water faucets
`and stoves. “Doors?”I can hear the reader saying, “you have trouble
`
`5
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`1.2 A Row of Swinging Glass Doors in a Boston Hotel. A similar problem to
`the doors from that European post office. On which side of the door should you
`push? WhenI asked people whohad just used the doors, most couldn’t say. Yet
`only a few of the people I watched hadtrouble with the doors. The designers had
`incorporated a subtle clue into the design. Note that the horizontal bars are not
`centered: theyarea bit closer together on the sides you should push on. The design
`almost works—butnotentirely, for not everyone used the doorsright onthefirst
`try.
`
`on? While distracted, my friend had moved toward the (invisible)
`supportingpillar, so he was pushing the doors on the hinged side. No
`wonder nothing happened. Pretty doors. Elegant. Probably won a de-
`sign prize.
`
`The doorstory illustrates one of the most importantprinciples of
`design:isibility. The correct parts must be visible, and they must con-
`vey the correct message. With doors that push, the designer must
`provide signals that naturally indicate where to push. These need not
`destroy the aesthetics. Put a vertical plate on the side to be pushed,
`nothing on the other. Or make the supporting pillars visible. The
`vertical plate and supportingpillars are natural signals, naturally inter-
`preted, without any need to be conscious of them.I call the use of
`natural signals naturaldesign and elaborate on the approach throughout
`this book.
`
`Visibility problems come in many forms. Myfriend, trapped be-
`tween theglass doors, suffered from a lack of clues that would indicate
`whatpart of a door should be operated. Other problems concern the
`ings
`between what you wantto do and whatappears to be possible,
`“another topic that will be expanded upon throughout the book. Con-
`sider one type ofslide projector. This projector has a single button to
`control whethertheslide tray moves forward or backward. Onebutton
`to do two things? Whatis the mapping? Howcan youfigure out how
`to control the slides? You can’t. Nothingis visible to give the slightest
`hint. Here is what happenedto mein one of the many unfamiliar places
`I’ve lectured in during my travels as a professor:
`
`TheLeitz slide projectorillustrated in figure 1.3 has shown up sev-
`eral times in my travels. The first time, it led to a rather dramatic
`incident. A conscientious student was in charge of showing myslides.
`I started my talk and showed the first slide. When I finished with the
`first slide and asked for the next, the student carefully pushed the
`control button and watched in dismay as the tray backed up, slid out
`of the projector and plopped off the table onto the floor, spilling its
`entire contents. We had to delay the lecture fifteen minutes while I
`struggled to reorganize theslides. It wasn’t the students fault. It was
`the fault of the elegant projector. With only one button to control the
`slide advance, how could one switch from forward to reverse? Neither
`of us could figure out how to make the control work.
`All during the lecture the slides would sometimes go forward, some-
`times backward. Afterward, we found the local technician, who ex-
`plained it to us. A brief push of the button and the slide would go
`
`yw
`
`i
`
`\ick
`Sotclco
`
`Taste (7) fiir Diawechsel am Gerat
`Diawechsel vorwarts = kurz driicken,
`Diawechsel riickwartz = langer driicken.
`
`
`
`Button (7) for changing the slides
`Slide change forward = short press,
`Slide change backward = longerpress.
`
`Slide
`Pravodit
`1.3 Leitz
`I
`finally tracked
`Projector.
`down the instruction manual
`for that projector. A photo-
`graph of the projector hasits
`parts numbered. The button
`for changing slides is number
`7. The buttonitself has no la-
`bels. Who could discover this
`operation without the aid of
`the manual? Hereis the entire
`text related to the button,
`in
`the original Germanand in my
`English translation:
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`6
`
`

`

`
`
`
`we
`edoT
`vot
`
`AS
`
`AF
`Kor
`
`forward, a long push and it would reverse. (Pity the conscientious
`student who kept pushing it hard—and long—to makesure that the
`switch was making contact.) What an elegant design. Why, it managed
`to do two functions with only one button! But Row was a first-time
`user of the projector to know this?
`,
`
`As another example, consider the beautiful Amphithéatre Louis-
`Laird in the Paris Sorbonne, which is filled with magnificentpaintings
`ofgreat figures in French intellectual history. (The mural on the ceiling
`shows lots of naked women floating about a man whois valiantly
`trying to read a book. The painting is right side up only for the lec-
`turer—it is upside down forall the people in the audience.) The room
`is a delightto lecture in, at least until you ask for the projection screen
`to be lowered. “Ah,”says the professor in charge, who gestures to the
`technician, who runs out of the room, up a short flightof stairs, and
`out of sight behind a solid wall. The screen comes down andstops.
`“No, no,” shouts the professor, “a little bit more.” The screen comes
`down again, this time too much. “No, no, no!”the professor jumps up
`and down andgestures wildly. It’s a lovelyroom, with lovelypaintings.
`But why can’t the person whois trying to lower orraise the screen see
`whathe is doing?
`
`New telephone systems have proven to be another excellent exam-
`ple of incomprehensible design. No matter whereI travel, I can count
`upon finding a particularly bad example.
`
`WhenI visited Basic Books, the publishers of this book, I noticed a
`new telephonesystem. I askedpeople howthey liked it. The question
`unleashed a torrent of abuse. “It doesn’t have a hold function,” one
`woman complained bitterly—the same complaintpeople at my univer-
`sity made abouttheir rather different system. In older days, business
`phones always had a button labeled “hold.” You couldpush the button
`and hang up the phone withoutlosing the call on your line. Then you
`could talk to a colleague, orpick up another telephonecall, or even pick
`up the call at anotherphone with the same telephone number. A light
`on the hold button indicated when the function wasin use. It was an
`invaluable tool for business. Why didn’t the newphonesat Basic Books
`or in my university have a hold function,ifit is so essential? Well, they
`did, even the very instrument the woman was complaining about. But
`there was no easy way to discover the fact, nor to learn howto use it.
`I wasvisiting the University ofMichigan and I asked about the new
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things 1.4 Plate Mounted Over the
`
`Dial of the Telephones at
`the University of Michigan.
`These inadequate instructions
`are all
`that most users see.
`(The button labeled “TAP”at
`the lower
`right
`is used, to
`transfer or pick up calls—it is
`pressed wheneverthe instruc-
`tion plate says “TAP.” The
`light on the lower left comes
`on whenever the telephone
`rings.)
`
`system there. “Yech!” was the response, “and it doesn’t even have a
`hold function!” Here we go again. Whatis going on? The answeris
`simple: first, look at the instructions for hold. At the University of
`Michigan the phone company provided a little plate that fits over the
`keypad and reminds users of the functions and how to use them. I
`carefully unhooked one ofthe plates from the telephone and made a
`Photocopy (figure 1.4). Can you understand how to use it? I can’t.
`There is a “call hold” operation, but it doesn’t make sense to me, not
`for the application that I just described.
`
`Thetelephone holdsituationillustrates a numberof different prob-
`lems. One of them is simply poor instructions, especially a failure to
`relate the new functions to the similarly named functions that people
`already know about. Second, and moreserious,is the lack of visibility of
`the operation of the system. The new telephones,for all their added
`sophistication,lack both the hold button and theflashinglight of the old
`ones. Theholdis signified by an arbitrary action: dialing an arbitrary
`sequence of digits (*8, or *99, or what have you:it varies from one
`phone system to another). Third, there is no visible outcome of the
`operation.
`Devices in the home have developed somerelated problems: func-
`tions and more functions, controls and morecontrols. I do not think
`that simple home appliances—stoves, washing machines, audio and
`television sets—should look like Hollywood's idea of a spaceship con-
`trol room. They already do, muchto the consternation of the consumer
`who, often as not, has lost (or cannot understand) the instruction
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`7
`
`

`

`manual, so—faced with the bewildering array of controls and dis-
`plays—simply memorizes one or twofixed settings to approximate
`whatis desired. The whole purpose of the design is lost.
`*
`
`In EnglandI visited a home with a fancy new Italian washer-drier
`combination, with super-duper multi-symbolcontrols, all to do every-
`thing you ever wanted to do with the washing and drying of clothes.
`The husband(an engineering psychologist) said he refused to go near
`it. The wife (a physician) said she had simply memorized onesetting
`andtried to ignore therest.
`Someone wentto a lot of trouble to create that design. I read the
`instruction manual. That machine took into account everything about
`today’s wide variety of synthetic and natural fabrics. The designers
`worked hard; they really cared. But obviously they had never thought
`of trying it out, or of watching anyoneuse it.
`If the design was so bad, if the controls were so unusable, why did
`the couple purchase it? If people keep buying poorly designed pro-
`ducts, manufacturers and designers will think they are doing the right
`thing and continue as usual.
`
`of visibility, appropriate clues, and feedback of one’s actions. These
`principles constitute a form of psychology—the psychology of how
`people interact with things. A British designer once noted that the
`kinds of materials used in the construction of passenger shelters af-
`fected the way vandals responded. He suggested that there might be
`a psychology of materials.
`
`geome Cs)
`
`“In one case, the reinforced glass used to panel shelters (for railroad
`passengers) erected by British Rail was smashed by vandals as fast as
`it was renewed. When the reinforced glass was replaced by plywood
`boarding, however, little further damage occurred, although no extra
`force would have been required to produce it. Thus British Rail
`managed to elevate the desire for defacementto those who could write,
`albeit in somewhat limited terms. Nobody has, as yet, considered
`whether thereis a kind ofpsychology ofmaterials. But on the evidence,
`there could well be!”
`
`
`
`
`
`theinAiliassothepecieandtenreperseFhe
`
`There already exists the start of a psychology of materials and of
`things, the study of affordances of objects. When used in this sense,
`
`The user needs help. Just the right things have to be visible: to
`indicate what parts operate and how,to indicate how the user is to
`interact with the device. Visibility indicates the mapping between in-
`tended actions and actual operations. Visibility indicates crucial dis-
`tinctions—so that youcan tell salt and pepper shakers apart, for exam-
`ple. Andvisibility of the effects of the operationstells you if the lights
`have turned on properly, if the projection screen has lowered to the
`correct height, or if the refrigerator temperature is adjusted correctly.
`It is lack of visibility that makes so many computer-controlled devices
`so difficult to operate. Andit is an excess of visibility that makes the
`gadget-ridden, feature-laden modern audio set or video cassette re-
`corder (VCR) so intimidating.
`
`(see figures 1.5 and 1.6). A chair
`tl
`affords (“is for”) support and, therefore, affordssitting. A chair can also
`be carried. Glass is for seeing through, and for breaking. Wood is
`normally used for solidity, opacity, support, or carving. Flat, porous,
`smooth surfaces are for writing on. So woodis also for writing on.
`Hence the problem for British Rail: when the shelters had glass, van-
`dals smashedit; when they had plywood,vandals wrote on and carved
`it. The planners were trapped by the affordances of their materials.?
`Affordances provide strong clues to the operations of things. Plates
`are for pushing. Knobsare for turning. Slots are for inserting things
`into. Balls are for throwing or bouncing. Whenaffordancesare taken
`advantage of, the user knows what to do just by looking: no picture,
`label, or instruction is required, Complex things may require explana-
`tion, but simple things should not. When simple things needpictures,
`labels, or instructions, the design hasfailed.
`A psychology ofcausality is also at work as we use everydaythings.
`
`The Psychology
`of Everyday Things
`
`This book is about the psychology of everyday things. POET empha-
`sizes the understanding of everyday things, things with knobs and
`dials, controls and switches, lights and meters. The instances we have
`just examined demonstrateseveral principles, including the importance
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`8
`
`

`

`
`pulling.
`
`1.5 Affordances of Doors. Door hardware can signal whether to push or pull
`withoutsigns. Theflat horizontalbar of A (aboveleft) affords no operations except
`pushing:it is excellent hardware for a door that must be pushed to be opened. The
`doorin B (aboveright) has a different kind of bar on eachside, one relatively small
`andvertical to signify a pull, the other relatively large and horizontal to signify a
`push, Both bars support the affordance of grasping: size and position specify
`whetherthe grasp is used to push or pull—though ambiguously.
`
`1.6 WhenAffordances Fail. I hadto tie a string around mycabinet doorto afford
`
`Something that happensright after an action appears to be caused by
`that action. Touch a computer terminal just whenit fails, and you are
`apt to believe that you causedthe failure, even though the failure and
`your action were related only by coincidence. Suchfalse causality is the
`basis for much superstition. Many of the peculiar behaviors of people
`using computer systems or complex household appliances result from
`such false coincidences. When an action has no apparent result, you
`mayconcludethatthe action wasineffective. So yourepeatit. In earlier
`days, when computer word processors did not always showtheresults
`of their operations, people would sometimes attempt to change their
`manuscript, but the lack ofvisible effect from each action would make
`them think that their commands had not been executed, so they would
`repeat the commands, sometimes over andover, to their later astonish-
`mentand regret. It is a poor design that allows either kind of false
`causality to occur.
`
`TWENTY THOUSAND EVERYDAY THINGS
`
`There are an amazing number of everyday things, perhaps twenty
`thousand of them. Are there really that many? Start by looking about
`you. There arelightfixtures, bulbs, and sockets; wall plates and screws;
`clocks, watches, and watchbands. There are writing devices (I count
`twelve in front of me, each different in function, color, or style). There
`are clothes, with different functions, openings, and flaps. Notice the
`variety of materials and pieces. Notice the variety of fasteners—but-
`tons, zippers, snaps, laces. Lookatall the furniture and food utensils:
`all those details, each serving some function for manufacturability,
`usage, or appearance. Consider the workarea: paperclips, scissors, pads
`of paper, magazines, books, bookmarks. In the room I’m workingin,
`I counted more than a hundred specialized objects before I tired. Each
`is simple, but each requires its own method of operation, each has to
`be learned, each does its own specialized task, and each has to be
`designed separately. Furthermore, many of the objects are made of
`manyparts. A desk stapler has sixteen parts, a household ironfifteen,
`the simple bathtub-shower combination twenty-three. You can’t be-
`lieve these simple objects have so manyparts? Hereare the eleven basic
`parts to a sink:drain, flange (around the drain), pop-up stopper, basin,
`soap dish, overflow vent, spout,lift rod,fittings, hot-water handle, and
`cold-water handle. We can count even more if westart taking the
`faucets, fittings, and lift rods apart.
`
`10
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`11
`
`9
`
`

`

`
`
`The book What's What: A Visual Glossary of the Physical World has more
`than fifteen hundred drawings and pictures andillustrates twenty-
`three thousanditemsorparts of items.* Irving Biederman, a psycholo-
`gist who studies visual perception, estimates that there are probably
`“30,000 readily discriminable objects for the adult.’’> Whatever the
`exact number,it is clear that the difficulties of everyday life are ampli-
`fied by the sheer profusion of items. Suppose that each everyday thing
`takes only one minute to learn; learning 20,000 of them occupies
`20,000 minutes—333 hours or about 8 forty-hour work weeks. Fur-
`thermore, we often encounter new objects unexpectedly, when weare
`really concerned with something else. We are confused and distracted,
`and whatoughtto be a simple, effortless, everyday thing interferes
`with the important task of the moment.
`How do people cope? Part of the answerlies in the way the mind
`works—in the psychology of human thought and cognition. Part lies
`in the information available from the appearance of the objects—the
`psychology of everyday things. And part comes from the ability of the
`designer to make the operation clear, to project a good image of the
`operation, and to take advantage of other things people might be ex-
`pected to know. Here is where the designer’s knowledge of the psy-
`chology of people coupled with knowledge of how things work
`becomescrucial.
`
`
`
`1.7 Carelman’s Tandem “Convergent Bicycle (Model for Fiancés).” Jacques
`Carelman: “Convergent Bicycle” Copyright © 1969-76-80 by Jacques Carelman
`and A.D. A. G.P. Paris. From Jacques Carelman,Catalog ofLinfindable Objects, Balland,
`éditeur, Paris-France. Used by permission oftheartist.
`
`the scissors still work. You can figure out the scissors because their
`operating parts are visible and the implications clear. The conceptual
`model is made obvious, and thereis effective use of affordances and
`constraints.
`
`As a counterexample, consider the digital watch, one with two to
`four push buttons on the front or side. What are those push buttons
`for? How would youset the time? There is no wayto tell—no evident
`relationship between the operating controls and the functions, no con-
`straints, no apparent mappings. With the scissors, moving the handle
`makes the blades move. Thewatch and theLeitz slide projector provide
`no visible relationship between the buttons and the possible actions,
`no discernible relationship between the actions and the endresult.
`
`Consider the rather strange bicycle illustrated in figure 1.7. You know
`Principles of Design
`
`it won’t work because you formassontephin!podemotathendevicerands.
`for Understandability and Usability
`
`mentallysimulateitsioperation®You can do the simulation because the
`parts are visible and the implications clear.
`We have now encountered the fundamental principles of designing
`Otherclues to how things work comefrom their visible structure—
`for people: (1) provide a good conceptual model and (2) make things
`visible.
`in particular from affordances, constraints, and mappings. Consider a pair of
`scissors: even if you have never seen or used them before, you can see
`that the numberof possible actions is limited. The holes are clearly
`there to put somethinginto, and the only logical things that will fit are
`fingers. The holes are affordances: they allow the the fingers to be
`inserted. The sizes of the holes provides@a5H#i#B to limit the possible
`fingers: the big hole suggests several fingers, the small hole only one.
`The mapping between holes and fingers—the set of possible opera-
`tions—is suggested and constrained by the holes. Moreover, the opera-
`tion is not sensitive to finger placement: if you use the wrongfingers,
`
`Without a good model weoperate by rote, blindly; we do operations
`as we weretold to do them; wecan’t fully appreciate why, whateffects
`to expect, or what to doif things go wrong. As long as things work
`properly, we can manage. When things go wrong, however, or when
`
`PROVIDE A GOOD CONCEPTUAL MODEL
`
`12
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`5
`
`eo?
`r ae¥
`U9
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`13
`
`10
`
`10
`
`

`

`we come upona novelsituation, then we need a deeper understanding,
`a good model.
`For everyday things, conceptual models need not be very complex.
`After all, scissors, pens, and light switches are psetty simple devices.
`There is no need to understand the underlying physics or chemistry of
`each device we own,simply the relationship between the controls and
`the outcomes. When the model presented to us is inadequate or wrong
`(or, worse, nonexistent), we can havedifficulties. Let me tell you about
`myrefrigerator.
`
`My househas an ordinary, two-compartmentrefrigerator—nothing
`very fancy aboutit. The problem is that I can’t set the temperature
`properly. Thereare only two things to do: adjust the temperature ofthe
`freezer compartment and adjust the temperature of the fresh food
`compartment. And there are two controls, one labeled “freezer,” the
`other “fresh food.” What's the problem?
`You try it. Figure 1.8 shows the instruction plate from inside the
`refrigerator. Now, supposethe freezeris too cold, the fresh food section
`just right. You wantto makethe freezer warmer, keeping the fresh food
`constant. Go on, read the instructions, figure them out.
`
`1.8 MyRefrigerator. Two compartments—fresh food and freezer—and two con-
`trols (in the fresh food unit). Theillustration showsthe controls and instructions.
`Your task: Suppose the freezer is too cold, the fresh food section just right. How
`would youadjustthe controls so as to make the freezer warmer and keep the fresh
`food the same? (From Norman, 1986.)
`
`TO STABILIZE
`
`NORMAL SETTINGS
`COLDER FRESH FOOD
`COLDEST FRESH FOOD
`COLDER FREEZER
`WARMERFRESH FOOD
`OFF (FRESH FD & FRZ)
`
`1 SET BOTH CONTROLS
`2 ALLOW 24 HOURS
`
`
`
`
`COOLING UNIT
`
`1.9 Two Conceptual Models for My Refrigerator. The model A (above) is
`provided by the system image oftherefrigerator as gleaned from the controls and
`instructions; B (below)is the correct conceptual model. The problem is thatit is
`impossibleto tell in which compartmentthe thermostatis located and whether the
`
`two controls are in the freezer and fresh food compartment, or vice versa.
`
`
`
`14
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`15
`
`11
`
`11
`
`

`

`Oh, perhaps I’d better warn you. The two controls are not indepen-
`dent. The freezer control affects the fresh food temperature, and the
`fresh food controlaffects the freezer. And don’t forget to-wait twenty-
`four hours to check on whether you madetherightadjustment, ifyou
`can remember what you did.
`
`Control of the refrigerator is madedifficult because the manufac-
`turer provides a false conceptual model. There are two compartments
`and two controls. The setup clearly and unambiguously provides a
`simple model for the user: each controlis responsible for the tempera-
`ture of the compartmentthatcarries its name. Wrong. In fact, there is
`only one thermostat and only one cooling mechanism. Onecontrol
`adjusts the thermostatsetting, the other the relative proportion of cold
`air sent to each of the two compartmentsofthe refrigerator. This is
`whythe two controls interact. With the conceptual model provided by
`the manufacturer, adjusting the temperatures is almost impossible and
`alwaysfrustrating. Given the correct model, life would be much easier
`(figure 1.9).
`Why did the manufacturer present the wrong conceptual model?
`
`1.10 Conceptual Models. Thedesign modelis the designer’s conceptual model. The
`user's model is the mental model developed through interaction with the system. The
`system image results from the physical structure that has been built (including docu-
`mentation, instructions, and labels). The designer expects the user’s model to be
`identical to the design model. But the designer doesn’t talk directly with the
`user—all communication takes place through the system image. If the system
`image does not make the design model clear and consistent, then theuser will end
`up with the wrong mental model. (From Norman, 1986.)
`
`DESIGN
`
`Perhapsthe designers thought the correct model was too complex, that
`the model they were giving was easier to understand. But with the
`wrong conceptual model,it is impossible to set the controls. And even
`though I am convinced I now know the correct model, I still cannot
`accurately adjust the temperatures because the refrigerator design
`makesit impossible for me to discover which control is for the thermo-
`stat, which control is for the relative proportion of cold air, and in
`which compartment the thermostat is located. The lack of immediate
`feedback for the actions does not help: with a delay of twenty-four
`hours, who can remember what wastried?
`Thetopic of conceptual models will reappear in the book. They are
`part of an important conceptin design:gmemtabi@im@éle; the models people
`have of themselves, others, the environment, and the things with
`which they interact. People form mental models through experience,
`training, and instruction. The mental model of a device is formed
`largely by interpreting its perceived actions andits visible structure. I
`call the visible part of the device the system image (figure 1.10). When
`the system imageis incoherent or inappropriate, as in the case of the
`refrigerator, then the user cannot easily use the device.If it is sen
`plete or contradictory, there will be trouble.
`\e Lawidt
`‘
`on
`(law iS ~T FO -\r
`cot
`ar ws
`\
`\et
`-
`4
`MAKE THINGSVISIBLE >“
`(
`: A bite
`'
`ad
`Theproblems caused by inadequateattention tovisibility are all neatly
`demonstrated with one simple appliance: the modern telephone.
`
`I stand at the blackboard in my office, talking with a student, when
`my telephonerings. Once, twiceit rings. Ipause, trying to complete my
`sentence before answering. The ringing stops. “I’m sorry,” says the
`student. “Not your fault,” I say. “But it’s no problem, the call now
`transfers to my secretary’s phone. She‘ll answerit.” As welisten we r\
`hear her phonestart to ring. Once,
`twice. I look at my watch. Six
`o‘clock: it’s late, the office staff has left for the day. I rush out ofmy See
`office to my secretary’s phone, butas Iget there, it stops ringing. “Ah,”
`I think, “it’s being transferred to another phone.” Sure enough, the
`Phonein the adjacentoffice nowstarts ringing. Irush to that office, but
`it is locked. Back to myoffice to get the key, out to the locked door,
`fumble with thelock, into the office, and to the now quietphone. Ihear
`a telephone downthe hall start to ring. Could thatstill be my call,
`
`a
`
`16
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`17
`
`12
`
`12
`
`

`

`The modern telephone did not happen bya

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket