throbber
Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`0022-3565/02/3011-333–345$7.00
`THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS
`Copyright © 2002 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
`JPET 301:333–345, 2002
`
`Vol. 301, No. 1
`4642/974299
`Printed in U.S.A.
`
`4-(2-Chloro-4-methoxy-5-methylphenyl)-N-[(1S)-2-cyclopropyl-
`1-(3-fluoro-4-methylphenyl)ethyl]5-methyl-N-(2-propynyl)-1,
`3-thiazol-2-amine Hydrochloride (SSR125543A), a Potent
`and Selective Corticotrophin-Releasing Factor1 Receptor
`Antagonist. II. Characterization in Rodent Models of
`Stress-Related Disorders
`
`GUY GRIEBEL, JACQUES SIMIAND, R ´EGIS STEINBERG, MIREILLE JUNG, DANIELLE GULLY, PIERRE ROGER,
`MICHEL GESLIN, BERNARD SCATTON, JEAN-PIERRE MAFFRAND, and PHILIPPE SOUBRI ´E
`Central Nervous System Research Department, Sanofi-Synthelabo, Bagneux, France (G.G., B.S., P.R.); Exploratory Research Department,
`Sanofi-Synthelabo, Toulouse, France (D.G., M.G., J.S.); and Discovery Research Division, Sanofi-Synthelabo, Montpellier, France (R.S., M.J.,
`P.S., J.P.M.)
`Received October 11, 2001; accepted December 31, 2001
`
`This article is available online at http://jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
`ABSTRACT
`The present study investigated the effects of the novel corticotro-
`phin-releasing factor (CRF)1 receptor antagonist 4-(2-chloro-
`4-methoxy-5-methylphenyl)-N-[(1S)-2-cyclopropyl-1-(3-fluoro-4-
`methylphenyl)ethyl]5-methyl-N-(2-propynyl)-1,3-thiazol-2-amine
`hydrochloride (SSR125543A) in a variety of rodent models of
`anxiety, including conflict procedures (punished drinking and
`four-plate), exploration models (elevated plus-maze and light/
`dark), a fear/anxiety defense test battery, and several proce-
`dures based on stress-induced changes in physiological
`(isolation-induced hyperthermia and tail pinch-induced cortical
`norepinephrine release) or behavioral (social defeat-induced anx-
`iety, maternal separation-induced vocalization) parameters. More-
`over, the effects of SSR125543A were investigated in acute
`(forced swimming) and chronic (chronic mild stress; CMS) models
`of depression. SSR125543A and the CRF1 receptor antagonist
`antalarmin displayed limited efficacy in exploration-based anxiety
`models. In contrast, both compounds produced clear-cut anxio-
`
`lytic-like activity in models involving inescapable stress, including
`the conflict procedures, the social defeat-induced anxiety para-
`digm and the defense test battery (3–30 mg/kg i.p. or p.o.). These
`effects paralleled those of the anxiolytic diazepam. In addition,
`SSR125543A and antalarmin antagonized stress-induced hyper-
`thermia, distress vocalization, and cortical norepinephrine release.
`In the forced swimming test, 30 mg/kg p.o. SSR125543A and 3 to
`30 mg/kg p.o. antalarmin produced clear antidepressant-like
`effects. These latter results were strengthened by the findings
`from the CMS, which showed that repeated administration of
`10 mg/kg i.p. SSR125543A for 30 days improved the degrada-
`tion of the physical state, the reduction of body weight gain,
`and anxiety produced by stress. Together, these data indicate
`that SSR125543A shows good activity in acute and chronic
`tests of unavoidable stress exposure, suggesting that it may
`have a potential in the treatment of depression and some forms
`of anxiety disorders.
`
`Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) has been identified as
`a neuropeptide that plays a central role in the coordination of
`neuroendocrine, autonomic, and behavioral responses to
`stress (Vale et al., 1981). It is the main regulator of basal and
`stress-induced release of the adrenocorticotropic hormone
`(ACTH) (Stout et al., 1995). Two CRF receptor subtypes,
`CRF1 and CRF2, with distinct anatomical localization and
`pharmacology have been identified (Chalmers et al., 1996).
`CRF1 receptor expression is most abundant in neocortical,
`cerebellar, and limbic structures, whereas CRF2 receptor
`
`expression is generally prominent in subcortical structures.
`This anatomical information provided a basis for functional
`hypotheses related to CRF receptor subtypes and suggested
`that CRF may contribute significantly both to behavioral
`responses to stress and emotional behavior itself. This idea
`has been substantiated by numerous studies showing that
`i.c.v. application of CRF in rodents produces behavioral ef-
`fects similar to those observed when animals are exposed to
`stress (for review, see Griebel, 1999). Studies using CRF
`transgenic mouse lines overexpressing CRF or knockout mice
`
`ABBREVIATIONS: CRF, corticotropin-releasing factor; ACTH, adrenocorticotropic hormone; MDTB, mouse defense test battery; CMS, chronic
`mild stress; NE, norepinephrine ANOVA, analysis of variance; CP-154,526, butyl-[2,5-dimethyl-7-(2,4,6-trimethylphenyl)-7H-pyrrolo[2,3-d]pyrimi-
`din-4-yl]-ethylamine; R121919, 3-[6-(dimethylamino)-4-methyl-pyrid-3-yl]-2,5-dimethyl-N,N-dipropyl-pyrazolo[2,3-a]pyrimidin-7-amine.
`
`333
`
`1
`
`NEUROCRINE 1019
`
`

`

`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`334
`
`Griebel et al.
`
`lacking the CRF1 receptor further emphasized the involve-
`ment of CRF in stress-related behaviors, because the former
`exhibited a behavioral state resembling that produced by
`anxiety, whereas the latter showed reduced emotionality (for
`review, see Contarino et al., 1999). On a clinical level, a large
`body of evidence points to increased cerebrospinal fluid CRF
`concentrations in drug-free patients with major depression
`and post-traumatic stress disorder compared with healthy
`controls (for review, see Kasckow et al., 2001). In this context,
`it was hypothesized that CRF receptor antagonists may rep-
`resent novel agents for the treatment of stress-related disor-
`ders (Chalmers et al., 1996; Holsboer, 1999).
`Several classes of nonpeptide antagonists of CRF1 recep-
`tors have been identified (for review, see Gutman et al.,
`2000). A few of them have been studied extensively in exper-
`imental models of stress. For example, peripheral adminis-
`tration of the pyrrolopyrimidine derivatives antalarmin, CP-
`154,526, and R121919 was reported to reduce the effects of
`acute and repeated stressors on behavior in rodents, and
`some activity was found in classical models used to screen
`anxiolytics and antidepressants (for reviews, see Griebel,
`1999; Gutman et al., 2000). However, negative results were
`also reported with some of these procedures. It was suggested
`that CRF1 antagonists may produce positive effects only
`when the endogenous tone of CRF is high, thereby pointing to
`a crucial importance of baseline levels of stress when inves-
`tigating the behavioral actions of these compounds. Recently,
`the results of the first open-label study examining the effects
`of R121919 in 20 patients with major depression was pub-
`lished (Zobel et al., 2000). The compound was well tolerated
`by the patients and did not significantly affect ACTH or
`cortisol levels at baseline or after a CRF challenge. More
`importantly, significant reductions in depression and anxiety
`scores were observed after 30-day treatment with the com-
`pound. Although this small open-label study does not provide
`unequivocal proof, it brings further evidence that a selective
`CRF1 receptor antagonist may represent a promising alter-
`native to agents currently used for the treatment of anxiety
`and depressive disorders.
`In the present article, we report on the psychopharmacolog-
`ical profile of 4-(2-chloro-4-methoxy-5-methylphenyl)-N-[(1S)-2-
`cyclopropyl-1-(3-fluoro-4-methylphenyl)ethyl]5-methyl-N-(2-
`propynyl)-1,3-thiazol-2-amine hydrochloride (SSR125543A), a
`2-aminothiazole derivative, which displays high affinities for
`both native and recombinant human CRF1 receptors (Ki ⫽ 1
`and 2 nM, respectively), and 1000-fold selectivity for CRF1
`compared with CRF2 receptor (Gully et al., 2002). SSR125543A
`is a potent antagonist at the CRF1 receptor as shown by its
`ability to inhibit CRF-induced cyclic AMP synthesis in human
`retinoblastoma Y 79 cells (pA2 ⫽ 8.92) and ACTH secretion in
`mouse pituitary tumor AtT-20 cells (pA2 ⫽ 9.63). After in vivo
`experiments in rats, the compound was found to inhibit CRF-
`and restraint-stress induced ACTH secretion when given orally.
`In the present series of experiments, the behavioral effects of
`SSR125543A were examined using a variety of rodent models of
`anxiety, including conflict procedures (punished drinking and
`four-plate), exploration models (elevated plus-maze and light/
`dark), a fear/anxiety defense test battery, and several test pro-
`cedures based on stress-induced changes in physiological (iso-
`lation-induced hyperthermia and tail pinch-induced cortical
`norepinephrine release) or behavioral (social defeat-induced
`anxiety, maternal separation-induced distress vocalizations)
`
`parameters. Moreover, the effects of SSR125543A were inves-
`tigated in acute (forced swimming) and chronic (chronic mild
`stress; CMS) models used for the characterization of antide-
`pressants. In this latter experiment, the compound was given
`repeatedly for 30 days. Comparative data for the CRF1 receptor
`antagonist antalarmin and the anxiolytic and antidepressant
`diazepam and fluoxetine, respectively, obtained under the same
`experimental conditions, are also provided. Finally, possible
`unwanted effects of increasing doses of SSR125543A were ex-
`amined using motor activity and memory tests.
`
`Materials and Methods
`
`Ethics
`All experimental procedures described herein were approved by
`the Animal Care and Use Committee of Sanofi-Synthelabo Recher-
`che and fully comply with French legislation on research involving
`animal subjects.
`
`Animals
`Male Sprague-Dawley and Wistar rats (Iffa Credo, L’Arbresle, and
`Charles River, Saint-Aubin-le`s-Elbeuf, France), weighing 180 to
`330 g at the time of testing, were used in the punished drinking,
`elevated plus-maze, stress-induced hyperthermia, and forced swim-
`ming tests, and in the microdialysis experiment. They were housed
`in groups of three to eight. Different strains of rats were used to
`optimize conditions. For example, preliminary data from our labora-
`tory have shown that, unlike Sprague-Dawley rats, Wistar rats from
`our laboratory animal supplier are poor responders in the elevated
`plus-maze (i.e., they display weak avoidance responses of the open
`aversive arms), making it difficult to use them for the screening of
`anxiolytics. Male CD1 (social defeat stress-induced anxiety, horizon-
`tal wire test, rotarod, passive avoidance test, and actimeter), OF1
`(mouse defense test battery; MDTB), NMRI (four-plate test), and
`BALB/c (light/dark test, chronic mild stress procedure) mice weigh-
`ing 17 to 32 g were supplied by Charles River, Iffa Credo, or Janvier
`(Le Genest, France). CD1, OF1, and NMRI mice were housed in
`groups of 20, those used in the MDTB and in the chronic mild stress
`procedure were housed singly, and BALB/c mice used in the light/
`dark test were housed in groups of six. Female guinea pigs (Dunkin-
`Hartley) with four 5-day-old pups were obtained from Harlan (Horst,
`The Netherlands). Each mother was housed individually with her
`litter provided with sawdust. Moreover, male Long Evans rats (400 –
`500 g) (Iffa-Credo) were used as threat stimulus in the MDTB. All
`animals were maintained under standard laboratory conditions (21–
`23°C; 40 – 60%relative humidity) and kept on a 12-h light/dark cycle
`with light onset at 6:00 AM.
`
`Compounds
`Compounds were prepared as solutions or suspensions in physio-
`logical saline or distilled water containing 0.1% Tween 80 (anta-
`larmin, diazepam, and fluoxetine) or 5% dimethyl sulfoxide and 5%
`Cremophor EL (SSR125543A). The
`compounds used were
`SSR125543A, antalarmin, diazepam, and fluoxetine (synthesized by
`Sanofi-Synthelabo, Bagneux and Toulouse, France). Compounds ad-
`ministered i.p. and p.o. were given in a constant volume of 5 ml/kg
`(rats), 2 ml/kg (guinea pig pups), or 20 (mice) ml/kg. The i.p. route
`was used in some experiments because an oral administration would
`have increased further the level of stress [i.e., distress vocalizations
`in guinea pig pups, stress-induced norepinephrine (NE) release in
`rats, and chronic mild stress in mice] or because it would have
`interfered with the procedure (i.e., water deprivation in the punished
`drinking test in rats).
`
`2
`
`

`

`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`Psychopharmacological Profile of SSR125543A
`
`335
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`(30h)
`dampsawdust
`Housinginmild
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`Accesstoanempty
`
`bottle(2h)
`
`(30h)
`dampsawdust
`Housinginmild
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`Waterdeprivation
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`(30min)
`waterat32°C
`Forcedwashin
`
`(16h)
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`Pairedhousing
`
`(10min)
`restriction
`withfood
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`(10h)
`dampsawdust
`Pairedhousingin
`
`Waterandfood
`
`(16h)
`deprivation
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`deprivation(16h)
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`Waterandfood
`
`Foodrestriction
`
`(3h)
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`(10h)
`dampsawdust
`Pairedhousingin
`
`(30min)
`waterat32°C
`Forcedwashin
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`smalltube(1h)
`
`Confinementin
`
`light/darkcycle
`
`Inversionofthe
`
`Accesstoanempty
`
`(30h)
`dampsawdust
`Housinginmild
`
`bottle(2h)
`
`Waterdeprivation
`
`(16h)
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`Dark(2h)
`
`(10h)
`dampsawdust
`Pairedhousingin
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`Foodrestriction
`
`(3h)
`
`Waterandfood
`
`(16h)
`deprivation
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`smalltube(1h)
`Confinementina
`
`(30min)
`waterat32°C
`Forcedwashin
`
`6:00PM
`
`2:00PM
`
`9:00AM
`
`Week3
`
`6:00PM
`
`2:00PM
`
`9:00AM
`
`Week2
`
`6:00PM
`
`2:00PM
`
`9:00AM
`
`Week1
`
`Day7
`
`Day6
`
`Day5
`
`Day4
`
`Day3
`
`Day2
`
`Day1
`
`StressExposure
`
`Startof
`
`Chronicmildstressschedule
`TABLE1
`
`3
`
`

`

`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`and two enclosed arms (50 ⫻ 10 ⫻ 50 cm), arranged so that the arms
`of the same type were opposite each other, connected by an open
`central area (10 ⫻ 10 cm). To prevent rats falling off, a rim of
`Plexiglas (0.5 cm in height) surrounded the perimeter of the open
`arms. The illumination in the experimental room consisted of one red
`neon tube fixed on the ceiling, so that experiments were performed
`under dim light conditions. The light intensity on the central plat-
`form was 10 lux. At the beginning of the experiment, rats were
`placed in the center of the maze, facing one of the enclosed arms, and
`observed for 4 min. The apparatus was equipped with infrared
`beams and sensors capable of measuring time spent in open arms,
`number of open-arm entries, and number of closed-arm entries (de-
`fined as entry of all four limbs into an arm of the maze). In addition,
`rats were observed via video-link by an observer located in an adja-
`cent room. This allowed the recording of a more ethologically orien-
`tated measure: 1) attempt: attempt at entry into open arms followed
`by avoidance responses. This includes stretched attend posture (the
`rat stretches forward and retracts to original position); and 2) head-
`dipping: protruding the head over the ledge of an open arm and down
`toward the floor (this response can occur while the animal’s body is
`in the closed arms, central square, or on open arms). The results
`were expressed as mean ratio of time spent in open arms to total time
`spent in both open and closed arms, mean ratio of entries into open
`arms to total entries into both open and closed arms, mean total
`number of both closed and open arm entries, mean total number of
`attempts, and mean total number of head-dips. Data were analyzed
`with one-way ANOVA. Subsequent comparisons between treatment
`groups and control were carried out using Dunnett’s t test. Experi-
`ments were performed 60 min after p.o. administration of
`SSR125543A, antalarmin, or diazepam.
`Stress-Induced Hyperthermia in Rats. The basal rectal tem-
`perature of grouped rats (5–7/cage) was measured with a telether-
`mometer (DM 852; ELLAB Instruments, Copenhagen, Denmark).
`Animals were then removed from their home cage, and placed indi-
`vidually in a small transparent plastic cage (25 ⫻ 15 ⫻ 27 cm).
`Temperature was measured again three times at 15-min intervals.
`Isolation yielded an enhanced body temperature that putatively
`reflects a stress-induced anxiogenic response. SSR125543A, diaze-
`pam, or antalarmin were administered p.o. or i.p. 60-min before
`basal temperature measurement. Data were analyzed by two-way
`ANOVA (time ⫻ treatment) with repeated measures, followed by
`Dunnett’s t test.
`Stress-Induced Cortical Norepinephrine Release in Rats.
`Cortical extracellular NE levels were increased after a 15-min tail
`pinch in awake rats (Funk and Stewart, 1996). They were measured
`in 30-␮l dialysate samples by a high-performance liquid chromatog-
`raphy system with coulometric detection as previously described
`(Marco et al., 1998), except for the mobile phase, which contained 7%
`
`336
`
`Griebel et al.
`
`Fig. 1. Effects of diazepam (f), antalarmin (_), and SSR125543A (^) in
`the punished drinking conflict test in rats. Data represent mean ⫾ S.E.M.
`多, P ⬍ 0.05 (Dunnett’s t test). n ⫽ 14 to 20.
`
`Anxiolytic-Like Activity of SSR125543A
`Punished Drinking Test in Rats. The procedure was a modifi-
`cation of the technique described by Vogel et al. (1971). At the
`beginning of the experiment, rats, deprived of water but not of food
`for 48 h before testing, were placed in cages (32 ⫻ 25 ⫻ 30 cm) with
`a stainless steel grid floor. Each cage was placed in sound-attenuated
`boxes that were well ventilated and contained a drinking tube con-
`nected to an external 50-ml burette filled with tap water. Trials were
`started only after the animal’s tongue entered in contact with the
`drinking tube for the first time. An electric shock (0.6 mA/500 ms)
`was delivered to the tongue after every 20 licks. The number of
`shocks was recorded automatically during a 5-min period. Data were
`analyzed with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Subsequent
`comparisons between treatment groups and control were carried out
`using Dunnett’s t test. Experiments were performed 30 min (anta-
`larmin and diazepam) or 60 min (SSR125543A) after i.p. injection of
`the compounds. A dose of 2.5 mg/kg i.p. morphine was tested as
`negative control because this test involves the application of painful
`stimuli.
`Elevated Plus-Maze Test in Rats. The test apparatus is based
`on that described by Pellow et al. (1985). All parts of the apparatus
`were made of dark polyvinylplastic with a black rubber floor. The
`maze was elevated to a height of 50 cm with two open (50 ⫻ 10 cm)
`
`TABLE 2
`Effects of SSR125543A, antalarmin, and diazepam in the elevated plus-maze test in rats
`Data represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. n ⫽ 7 to 14.
`
`Compound
`
`Dose
`
`Time Open Arms
`
`Entries Open Arms
`
`Head-Dips
`
`Attempts
`
`Total Arm Entries
`
`SSR125543A
`
`Antalarmin
`
`Diazepam
`
`mg/kg p.o.
`0
`3
`10
`30
`0
`3
`10
`30
`0
`1
`3
`10
`
`%
`10.5 ⫾ 2.5
`13.3 ⫾ 3.5
`25.3 ⫾ 4.5
`17.2 ⫾ 3.7
`13.4 ⫾ 1.6
`14.6 ⫾ 5.6
`24.1 ⫾ 3.9
`30.9 ⫾ 5.2*
`12.4 ⫾ 2.0
`16.5 ⫾ 4.8
`32.9 ⫾ 6.3*
`49.1 ⫾ 7.2*
`
`* P ⬍ 0.05 (Dunnett’s t test).
`
`%
`22.1 ⫾ 4.3
`23.2 ⫾ 5.3
`44.7 ⫾ 6.0
`33.7 ⫾ 6.2
`32.7 ⫾ 2.9
`27.3 ⫾ 8.1
`43.3 ⫾ 5.9
`47.3 ⫾ 3.1
`30.1 ⫾ 3.1
`30.8 ⫾ 7.5
`45.4 ⫾ 7.5
`63.0 ⫾ 6.0*
`
`14.9 ⫾ 2.1
`19.9 ⫾ 3.2
`26.1 ⫾ 2.7
`19.2 ⫾ 2.6
`12.1 ⫾ 2.2
`12.9 ⫾ 2.9
`29.4 ⫾ 4.3*
`26.3 ⫾ 3.1*
`17.9 ⫾ 3.0
`16.6 ⫾ 3.5
`26.4 ⫾ 3.9
`43.4 ⫾ 5.5*
`
`7.4 ⫾ 0.6
`6.2 ⫾ 0.5*
`4.9 ⫾ 0.4*
`5.4 ⫾ 0.4*
`8.0 ⫾ 0.5
`7.9 ⫾ 0.6
`6.3 ⫾ 1.1
`5.9 ⫾ 1.0
`7.5 ⫾ 0.6
`7.5 ⫾ 0.8
`4.1 ⫾ 0.6*
`1.4 ⫾ 0.3*
`
`10.7 ⫾ 1.0
`11.1 ⫾ 0.9
`12.1 ⫾ 0.6
`12.4 ⫾ 1.0
`11.7 ⫾ 1.4
`11.9 ⫾ 1.0
`14.3 ⫾ 0.6
`12.3 ⫾ 1.1
`11.4 ⫾ 0.8
`13.3 ⫾ 1.5
`14.1 ⫾ 1.1
`12.6 ⫾ 1.6
`
`4
`
`

`

`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`Psychopharmacological Profile of SSR125543A
`
`337
`
`a neon tube fixed on the ceiling provided the room illumination so
`that the light intensity in the center of the illuminated box was 1000
`lux. An opaque plastic tunnel (5 ⫻ 7 ⫻ 10 cm) separated the dark box
`from the illuminated one. At the beginning of the experiment, a
`mouse was placed in the illuminated box, facing the tunnel. Record-
`ing started when the animal entered the tunnel for the first time.
`The apparatus was equipped with infrared beams and sensors capa-
`ble of recording the following parameters during a 4-min period: 1)
`time spent by mice in the lit box; 2) attempt at entry into the lit box
`followed by avoidance responses (this includes stretched attend pos-
`ture; the mouse stretches forward and retracts to original position);
`3) total number of tunnel crossings; and 4) activity in the lit box.
`Data were analyzed with one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s t
`test analysis. Experiments were performed 30 min (antalarmin and
`diazepam) or 60 (SSR125543A and diazepam) min after p.o. admin-
`istration of the compounds.
`Four-Plate Test in Mice. The test apparatus is based on that
`described by Boissier et al. (1968). The apparatus consisted of a cage
`with a floor composed of four rectangular metal plates connected to
`a device that can generate electric shocks (1 mA, 0.2 s). After a 15-s
`latency period, the animal is subjected to an electric shock every time
`it went from one plate to another. The number of punished crossings
`is recorded during a 1-min test period. Experiments were carried out
`60 min after p.o. administration of SSR125543A, antalarmin, or
`diazepam. In a second experiment, the duration of the anxiolytic-like
`action of 3 mg/kg p.o. SSR125543A was investigated. Mice were
`administered with the compound and placed in the apparatus 1, 2, 4,
`or 6 h later. Each animal was tested once. In a third experiment,
`possible development of tolerance to the anxiolytic-like activity was
`investigated after repeated administration of SSR12543. The com-
`pound was given orally at doses of 3 or 10 mg/kg, once daily for eight
`consecutive days. Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA followed
`by Dunnett’s t test.
`Mouse Defense Test Battery. The test was conducted in an oval
`runway as described previously (Griebel et al., 1997). Procedure: 1)
`Pretest: 3-min familiarization period. Sixty minutes after p.o. admin-
`istration of SSR125543A, antalarmin, or diazepam, subjects were
`placed into the runway for a 3-min familiarization period, in which
`line crossings were recorded. 2) The rat avoidance test. Immediately
`after the 3-min familiarization period, the experimenter introduced a
`hand-held dead rat (killed by CO2 inhalation just before the begin-
`ning of the experiment) five times at one end of the runway and
`brought up to the subject at a speed of approximately 0.5 m/s.
`Approach was terminated when contact with the subject was made
`or the subject ran away from the approaching rat. If the subject fled,
`avoidance distance (the distance from the rat to the subject at the
`point of flight) was recorded. 3) Chase/flight test. The rat was then
`brought up to the subject at a speed of approximately 2 m/s. A
`constant distance of 2 m separated the rat and the subject when the
`former was introduced in the runway. The following parameters
`were recorded: number of stops (pause in movement), orientations
`(subject stops then orients the head toward the rat) and chase speed
`(measured when the subject is running straight). The rat was re-
`moved after the chase was completed. 4) Straight alley. By the
`closing of two doors (60 cm distant from each other), the runway was
`then converted to a straight alley in which the subject was con-
`strained. The rat was introduced in one end of the straight alley.
`During 30 s, the number of approaches/withdrawals (subject must
`move more than 20 cm forward from the closed door then return to it)
`and immobility time were recorded. After this session, it was re-
`moved from the straight alley area. 5) Forced contact. Finally, the
`experimenter brought the rat up to contact the subject in the straight
`alley. Approaches were directed quickly (within 1 s) to the subject’s
`head. For each such contact, upright postures and bites by the
`subjects were noted. 6) Post-test: Contextual defense. Immediately
`after the forced contact test, the rat was removed and the doors were
`opened. Escape attempts were recorded during a 3-min session. Data
`were analyzed by one-way ANOVA, followed by Dunnett’s t test.
`
`Fig. 2. Effects of SSR125543A, antalarmin, and diazepam on isolation
`stress-induced hyperthermia in rats. Data represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. 多,
`P ⬍ 0.05 (Dunnett’s t test). n ⫽ 7 to 17.
`
`acetonitryl as organic agent. The analytical system consisted of an
`electrochemical detector ESA Coulochem II equipped with a model
`5014 analytical cell (ESA, Chelmsford, MA). The NE levels in frac-
`tional samples were converted to a percentage of the mean value of
`the 45-min baseline measurements before treatment. Time course
`effects of tail pinch on NE levels were analyzed by ANOVA with
`repeated measures,
`followed by Dunnett’s
`test analysis.
`t
`SSR125543A and antalarmin were administered i.p. 30 min before
`tail pinch. Compound antagonism was evaluated during the tail
`pinch sampling collection. Statistical analysis was carried out using
`ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s t test.
`Light/Dark Test in Mice. The test apparatus is based on that
`described by Misslin et al. (1989). It consisted of two polyvinylchlo-
`ride boxes (20 ⫻ 20 ⫻ 14 cm) covered with Plexiglas. One of these
`boxes was darkened. A desk lamp placed 20 cm above the lit box and
`
`5
`
`

`

`338
`
`Griebel et al.
`
`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`Fig. 3. A, effect of 15 min tail pinch on prefrontal cortex NE release. The changes in NE levels are expressed as a percentage of the mean value of the
`three basal samples collected at ⫺60, ⫺45, and ⫺30 min before the tail pinch. Each data point represents mean ⫾ S.E.M of five to six animals, P ⬍
`0.01 compared with control group by ANOVA with repeated measures followed by Dunnett’s t test. B, reversal of tail pinch-induced NE release by
`
`SSR125543A and antalarmin given i.p. 180 min ((cid:0)) and 30 min (^) before pinch, respectively. 䡺, vehicle. Each data point represents mean ⫾ S.E.M
`
`of NE release during tail pinch from five to seven animals. 多, P ⬍ 0.05, 多多 P ⬍ 0.01 compared with respective control group by ANOVA followed by
`Dunnett’s t test.
`
`Social Defeat Stress-Induced Anxiety in Elevated Plus-
`Maze in Mice. Social defeat was used as stressor before exposure to
`the elevated plus-maze. This stressor has been shown to produce
`significant anxiogenic-like activity without any physical signs of
`distress. The procedure was a modification of the technique de-
`scribed by Miczek (1979). A naı¨ve mouse was placed in the cage of a
`resident male aggressor, which was selected for high levels of ag-
`gression (Simiand et al., 1993). Social agonistic offensive and defen-
`sive behaviors were interrupted by the experimenter and the in-
`truder removed from the area when it displayed a submissive
`posture after being attacked. Thereafter, the intruder was returned
`to the resident cage for 60 min and placed in a cylindric wire mesh
`enclosure to avoid physical contact or injury. At the end of the
`interaction period, the intruder mouse was placed onto the central
`platform of the elevated plus-maze during a 5-min period (Lister,
`1987). To increase slightly open arm exploration, a rim (1 cm in
`height) surrounded the perimeter of these arms. Time spent in open
`arms was recorded. The results were expressed as mean ratio of time
`
`spent in open arms to total time spent in both open and closed arms.
`The compounds were administered i.p. or p.o. 15 min (antalarmin),
`30 min (diazepam), or 60 min (SSR125543A) before social defeat.
`Data were analyzed by a single factor ANOVA or with the nonpara-
`metric Kruskal-Wallis test. Subsequent comparisons between treat-
`ment groups and control were carried out using Dunnett’s t test
`procedure or the nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test with ␣adjust-
`ment of Holm, respectively.
`Maternal Separation-Induced Distress Vocalizations in
`Guinea Pig Pups. The procedure was adapted from that described
`by Molewijk et al. (1996). Briefly, from day 9 of age, pups entered at
`the most, three pretest sessions (with 2-day intervals) consisting of 5
`min of isolation in a sound-attenuated cage equipped with white
`noise and white illumination, and the duration of their vocalizations
`were recorded by the experimenter. Immediately after the 5-min
`isolation, the subjects were returned to their mothers and litter-
`mates. Pups emitting vocalization during at least 120 s entered
`subsequent compound experimentation. Each pup was tested with
`
`TABLE 3
`Effects of SSR125543A, antalarmin, and diazepam in the light/dark test in mice
`Data represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. n ⫽ 12 to 14.
`
`Compound
`
`SSR125543A
`
`Diazepam
`Antalarmin
`
`Diazepam
`
`Dose
`
`mg/kg i.p.
`0
`1
`3
`10
`30
`2.5
`0
`1
`3
`10
`30
`3
`
`* P ⬍ 0.05 (Dunnett’s t test).
`a Number of beams crossed in the lit box.
`
`Time in
`Lit Box
`
`s
`20.1 ⫾ 9.2
`2.6 ⫾ 1.9
`4.0 ⫾ 2.7
`3.4 ⫾ 3.2
`14.9 ⫾ 6.7
`83.6 ⫾ 14.7*
`16.6 ⫾ 10.0
`10.7 ⫾ 6.8
`10.3 ⫾ 7.8
`14.3 ⫾ 6.8
`25.5 ⫾ 10.8
`125.8 ⫾ 22.9*
`
`Activity in
`Lit Boxa
`
`4.6 ⫾ 2.3
`0.1 ⫾ 0.1
`0.9 ⫾ 0.7
`0.3 ⫾ 0.3
`3.5 ⫾ 1.9
`27.4 ⫾ 5.4*
`6.6 ⫾ 4.0
`2.2 ⫾ 2.2
`3.0 ⫾ 2.0
`2.8 ⫾ 1.8
`5.5 ⫾ 2.2
`27.8 ⫾ 7.5*
`
`Tunnel
`Crossings
`
`5.1 ⫾ 1.7
`1.6 ⫾ 0.3
`2.4 ⫾ 0.9
`1.6 ⫾ 0.4
`5.1 ⫾ 1.4
`15.4 ⫾ 2.1*
`3.4 ⫾ 1.2
`2.3 ⫾ 0.7
`3.4 ⫾ 1.5
`4.4 ⫾ 1.8
`4.9 ⫾ 1.6
`6.8 ⫾ 1.8
`
`Attempts
`
`20.2 ⫾ 2.6
`20.4 ⫾ 2.9
`20.3 ⫾ 1.7
`22.8 ⫾ 2.4
`20.7 ⫾ 1.8
`11.8 ⫾ 2.2*
`19.5 ⫾ 1.9
`20.2 ⫾ 1.4
`23.2 ⫾ 3.9
`21.9 ⫾ 2.4
`18.0 ⫾ 2.5
`5.8 ⫾ 1.3*
`
`6
`
`

`

`Downloaded from
`
`jpet.aspetjournals.org
`
` at ASPET Journals on January 30, 2020
`
`Psychopharmacological Profile of SSR125543A
`
`339
`
`were analyzed by a two-way ANOVA (treatment ⫻ week) with re-
`peated measures followed by Newman-Keuls post hoc test. At the
`end of the 10-week CMS period, mice were tested in the elevated
`plus-maze and the light/dark tests (for details, see above) to assess
`the impact of CMS on anxiety levels. These behaviors were evaluated
`because individuals with a major depressive episode frequently
`present with symptoms of anxiety (DSM-IV, 1994). In the elevated
`plus-maze test, anxiety was evaluated by measuring the number of
`mice that entered the open arms, whereas in the light/dark test, it
`was assessed by measuring activity in the illuminated box. In the
`former, results were expressed as total number of mice that entered
`the open arms and analyzed by a chi square independence (␹2) test,
`whereas in the latter, data were analyzed by a Student’s t test. The
`administration of 10 mg/kg SSR125543A started 4 weeks after the
`beginning of the CMS. Animals were treated i.p. once a day until all
`experiments were completed (30 days).
`
`Evaluation of Potential Side Effects of SSR125543A
`Effects on Spontaneous Locomotor Activity in Mice: Ac-
`timeter. Testing was conducted in square, clear Plexiglas boxes
`(22 ⫻ 27 ⫻ 10 cm) equipped with infrared beams and sensors and
`placed in sound-attenuated cupboards. Horizontal locomotor activity
`was quantified as total number of beams crossed during a 20-min
`period. Sixty minutes after p.o. administration of vehicle or various
`doses of SSR125543A, subjects were placed individually in the center
`of the apparatus. ED50 values were calculated by probit analysis.
`Effects on Muscle Tone in Mice: Horizontal Wire Test. It
`consisted of individually taking mice by the tail and allowing them to
`grasp a horizontally strung wire (20 cm above the bench level, 2 mm
`in diameter, 15 cm in length) with their forepaws. Inability to grasp
`the wire with the forepaws or inability to actively grasp the wire
`within 5 s with at least one hindpaw is measured. ED50 values were
`calculated by probit analysis. Experiments were performed 60 or 120
`min after oral administration of SSR125543A.
`Effects on Motor Coordination in Mice: Rotarod Test. The
`apparatus consisted of a plastic cylinder (4 cm in diameter) turning
`at 4 turns/min. Sixty or 120 min after oral administration of
`SSR125543A, mice were placed on the turning rotarod. The occur-
`rence of fall from the rotarod was noted during the 2-min period that
`followed. ED50 values were calculated by probit analysis.
`Effects on Learning and Memory in Mice: Passive Avoid-
`ance Test. The apparatus consisted of a black and white two-
`compartment box separated by a “guillotine” door. The white com-
`partment was small (10 ⫻ 10 ⫻ 12 cm), lit by a 100-W bulb and with
`plastic floor. The black compartment was large (22.5 ⫻ 16 ⫻ 12 cm)
`with a stainless grid floor connected to a constant current shock
`generator. Sixty minutes after oral treatment with SSR125543A,
`mice were placed in the small light compartment. Entrance into the
`dark box usually occurred within 30 s and was punished by an
`electri

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket