throbber
Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 157–161
`
`j j j
`Why use placebos in clinical trials? A narrative review of the
`methodological literature
`Andrew J. Vickersa,*, and Anton J. M. de Craenb
`aIntegrative Medicine Service and Biostatistics Service, Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center, 1275 York Avenue, New York, NY 11215, USA
`bDepartment of Clinical Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, Netherlands
`Received 20 May 1998; revised 16 June 1999; accepted 23 June 1999
`
`Abstract
`
`Researchers have a number of different options for their choice of control or comparison intervention in randomized trials. We sur-
`veyed the methodological literature looking for reasons why a researcher might choose to administer a placebo (i.e., a mimic interven-
`tion) to control subjects. Two implicit assumptions were that the issue of placebo is pertinent only to drug trials and that the nonplacebo
`effect of a treatment is the “real” or “true” effect. Explicit reasons given in the literature for the use of placebos were facilitating blinding
`and controlling for the placebo effect. The importance of the latter was often inadequately argued. Reasons to avoid placebos in con-
`trolled trials, other than ethics and feasibility, are that placebos do not inform real decisions, and may interfere with accurate estimation
`of effect size and with nonspecific aspects of treatment. Placebo-controlled trials have high internal validity but may be difficult to apply
`to clinical practice; the situation is reversed for trials without placebo control. © 2000 Elsevier Sciences Inc. All rights reserved.
`
`Keywords: Placebos; Research design; Double blind method; Clinical trials; Randomised controlled trials
`
`1. Introduction
`
`Randomized clinical trials involve a comparison between
`at least two interventions. One can usually be termed the
`“experimental” intervention and is the treatment that the re-
`searchers are setting out to test. The other is the “control”
`intervention and is what the experimental intervention is
`tested against.
`Researchers have a number of different options for their
`choice of control intervention, including placebo, usual
`care, or some form of active intervention. More than one
`control intervention may be used in a trial or even in a sin-
`gle arm of a trial. The important distinction, and the focus of
`this article, is between placebo-controlled trials and those
`without placebo control. We define placebo as an interven-
`tion used in a clinical trial that is administered with the in-
`tention of mimicking some other intervention so that a com-
`parison can be made. Though the most common placebo is
`the “sugar pill” in drug trials, placebos can and have been
`used for physical interventions such as ultrasound [1], spi-
`nal manipulation [2, 3], and even surgery [4].
`The question we ask concerns the reasons why a re-
`
`searcher should choose to use a placebo—rather than usual
`care or active treatment—as the control (i.e., comparison)
`intervention in a clinical trial. Though this question has
`wide implications for clinical trial protocols, implications
`ranging as far as good clinical practice and informed con-
`sent procedures, much of the discussion on this issue has
`centered around ethics and feasibility, rather than methodol-
`ogy. For example, Campbell [5] discusses whether it is fea-
`sible to design a placebo that mimics acupuncture treatment
`appropriately. Similarly, Rothman and Michels [6] give nu-
`merous cases of placebo-controlled trials, such as those on
`antidepressants, which they consider unethical because con-
`trol patients were denied treatment of proven value.
`We are concerned with the situation in which it is both
`ethical and feasible to use a placebo in a clinical trial. We will
`argue that there are methodological reasons to use or avoid
`placebos in clinical trials and that these have not always been
`discussed appropriately in the literature on clinical trial meth-
`odology. Furthermore, we will argue that the choice between
`placebo or other control group depends on the research ques-
`tion being asked and may be thought of in terms of the dis-
`tinction between internal and external validity.
`
`2. Reasons to use placebos in clinical trials
`
`* Corresponding author. Tel.: 1-212-639-8629; fax: 1-212-794-5851.
`E-mail address: vickersa@mskcc.org
`
`We examined a number of journal articles [6–16] and
`textbooks [17–32] looking for reasons to use placebo con-
`
`0895-4356/00/$ – see front matter © 2000 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights reserved.
`PII: S0895-4356(99)00139-0
`
`SPRUCE - EXHIBIT 2006
`Neurocrine Biosciences, Inc. v. Spruce Biosciences, Inc.
`PGR2022-00025
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`158
`
`A.J. Vickers, A.J.M. de Craen / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 157–161
`
`trols in clinical trials. We first report on the implicit as-
`sumptions that appear to be made when discussing placebo
`controls in clinical trials. We then discuss the explicit argu-
`ments for and against using placebos in clinical trials.
`
`3. Implicit assumptions about placebo
`
`One of the most immediately apparent features of the lit-
`erature is the prevalence of two implicit assumptions. First,
`it is generally assumed in the literature on the methodology
`of clinical trials that the issue of placebo is pertinent only to
`drug trials. An example would be a paper entitled “Scien-
`tific and ethical issues in the use of placebo controls in clin-
`ical trials” [7]. Though placebo is initially defined in gen-
`eral terms as “any therapeutic procedure . . . without
`specific activity” and the experimental group is at first de-
`scribed as “test therapy” or “intervention,” the substantive
`content of the paper is entirely concerned with drug trials:
`within a page, the “effect of a therapeutic intervention” be-
`comes the “pharmacologic effect.”
`The second implicit assumption is that the “true” or
`“real” effect of a treatment and its specific (i.e., nonplacebo)
`effect are the same thing. For example, Ernst and Resch [8]
`define a “true treatment effect” as the “perceived treatment
`effect minus . . . non-specific effects.” Similarly, Hennekens
`and Buring [23] contrast “psychological effects” of a drug
`with “true physiologic benefits” and Gordis [21] states that
`“the placebo plays a major role in identifying . . . the real
`benefits of an agent.”
`We disagree with both of these assumptions. First, the is-
`sue of placebo is clearly not confined to drug trials. Sec-
`ondly, the total effect of a treatment, as experienced by the
`patient, includes both a specific and a nonspecific compo-
`nent. There is no reason to define one part of the total treat-
`ment effect as the “real” or “true” effect.
`
`4. Explicit arguments for the use of placebos
`in clinical trials
`
`A number of explicit arguments for the use of placebos
`in clinical trials can also be found in the literature.
`
`1.
`
`2.
`
`The use of a placebo group controls for the placebo
`
`effect. Rickels [15], for example, argues that placebo
`groups control for suggestibility, “milieu or psycho-
`therapy”; Altman [17] points out that “the act of tak-
`ing some treatment may itself have some benefit to
`the patient.”
`According to
`Placebos facilitate outcome blinding.
`Hennekens and Buring [23], for example, the use of a
`placebo will minimize bias in the “ascertainment of
`both subjective disease outcomes and side-effects.”
`Active controls can also enable blinding, particularly
`in drug trials. So this is primarily an argument about
`the advantages of placebo control over both no treat-
`
`3.
`
`4.
`
`5.
`
`ment control and active treatment control when this is
`unlike the test intervention (e.g., physiotherapy con-
`trol in a chiropractic trial).
`Placebos promote similarity of clinician and patient
`Feinstein [19]
`compliance in each comparison group.
`argues that patients may withdraw from a trial, or be
`less compliant with follow-up procedures, if they are
`aware that they are in the control group. Similarly, cli-
`nicians may be tempted to give those patients not re-
`ceiving the experimental intervention some form of
`compensatory care.
`Placebos control for natural remission and nontrial
`Pledger
`
`et al. [14] claim that placebo
`interventions.
`controls are used “to control for spontaneous symp-
`tom change or remission.”
`Placebo controls have advantages over active con-
`Where an intervention is compared with a stan-
`trols.
`dard therapy, it can be difficult to know whether it has
`activity above that of placebo. If results in both
`groups are equivalent, a claim of greater than placebo
`activity for the test treatment involves an implicit,
`nonrandomized comparison with the placebo arm of
`an earlier study of the control treatment [12,13,16].
`As such, placebos are often demanded by regulatory
`authorities to provide a standard comparison.
`
`Of these five points, the first two clearly hold: placebos
`control for the placebo effect and, by enabling blinding,
`control bias when assessing outcome. Point 3 is really an
`extension of point 2: in this case, blinding allows control of
`performance bias. Point 4 cannot be used to decide whether
`placebos should be used: placebos control for natural dis-
`ease variation but then so do other types of control. Point 5
`is an extension of the first point: active control trials are
`seen as inferior to placebo-controlled trials because they fail
`to exclude the possibility that a test treatment has no greater
`than placebo effects.
`The two arguments for the use of placebos in clinical tri-
`als are therefore that it facilitates blinding and that it con-
`trols for the placebo effect. There is at least some empirical
`evidence that blinding affects the results of randomized tri-
`als [33,34]. However, what are the arguments for control-
`ling for the placebo effect?
`
`5. Why control for the placebo effect in clinical trials?
`
`For the purposes of this article, we will define placebo
`effects as those which are attributable to factors other than
`the characteristic content of the intervention under trial [35].
`Such factors have been categorized by Feinstein [19] into
`three components: passive, active, and psychic. The passive
`component refers to the natural course of disease. The ac-
`tive component refers to co-interventions accompanying the
`main intervention: an example would be such as dietary
`therapy provided alongside a hypoglycemic drug in diabe-
`tes. The psychic (or psychological) component refers to the
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`A.J. Vickers, A.J.M. de Craen / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 157–161
`
`159
`
`patient’s anticipation of benefit. The main argument in the
`literature about placebo effects concerns this third, psycho-
`logical, component of the placebo effect.
`Just as with the use of words like “true” and “real” to de-
`scribe specific effects, methodologists seem to take it as ob-
`vious that the psychological component of the placebo ef-
`fect should be eliminated in clinical trials. Klerman [11], for
`example, describes how placebos control for “attention,”
`“the patient’s expectations and hope for improvement,” and
`even “the cognitive reframing of the meaning given by the
`patient to his or her illness.” These are said to “obscure and
`confound the demonstration of drug efficacy.” However, it
`is not stated why we should be interested in “efficacy”
`rather than in the total effects of treatment including the
`psychological component of the placebo effect.
`Fletcher
`
`et al. [20] are rare in giving an explicit reason
`why controlling the placebo effect is important: “it is also
`useful to know what part of the total effect is specific and
`what is nonspecific so as to avoid dangerous, uncomfortable
`or costly interventions when relatively little of their effect
`can be attributed to their specific actions.” This argument is,
`however, dubious. Even if an intervention is “dangerous,
`uncomfortable and costly,” patients and clinicians will want
`to know first, the overall benefits relative to costs and risks,
`and second, how this compares to other possible interven-
`tions. They should choose the intervention with the largest
`benefit to cost and risk ratio, irrespective of the proportion
`of benefit that is attributable to the psychological compo-
`nent of placebo.
`The underlying assumption of Fletcher
`et al.’s argument
`
`is that if an intervention is a placebo, it can be replaced by
`some other placebo intervention to the same effect. This in
`turn involves assuming that there is a single, relatively con-
`stant “placebo effect” that can be elicited readily and the
`size of which is constant across different interventions. The
`evidence on the placebo effect suggests, conversely, that it
`varies enormously and that it is modified by numerous pa-
`tient and clinician variables, including practitioner expecta-
`tion [36], patient expectation [37], the color of medication
`[38], and previous exposure to treatment [39]. The size of
`an intervention’s nonspecific component is particular to that
`intervention. As such, two interventions may have different
`effects on patient outcome even though both were equiva-
`lent to placebo in clinical trials.
`We believe that there are good reasons to control for the
`placebo effect in clinical trials, even though these have
`never been made explicit in the literature. First, it is worth
`asking whether certain therapies have more than placebo ac-
`tion in order to plan research strategically. Research com-
`paring different dosages and formulations of a drug is not
`appropriate until it has been shown, in a placebo-controlled
`trial, that dosage and formulation had anything to do with
`the drug’s therapeutic effect: for example, trials comparing
`high- and low-dose nicotine gum took place only after the
`superior effectiveness of nicotine gum over placebo had
`been demonstrated [40].
`
`A second reason for controlling the placebo effect in
`clinical trials is that the size of the specific effect of an inter-
`vention arguably is more stable than that of its placebo com-
`ponent. The psychological effects of a treatment are likely
`to vary considerably depending on the outlook of the indi-
`vidual patient and clinician, current therapeutic fashions,
`media hype, and so on. New interventions announced in a
`blaze of excitement and publicity (be they laetrile or
`Prozac) can result in large placebo effects that may dissipate
`over time.
`
`6. Reasons to avoid the use of placebos in clinical trials
`
`In our survey of the methodological literature, surpris-
`ingly few authors described reasons other than ethics or fea-
`sibility to avoid placebos in clinical trials.
`
`1.
`
`2.
`
`Placebo-controlled trials may interfere with accurate
`
`
`Fletcher et al. [20] argue that:
`estimation of effect size.
`“what is important to clinicians and their patients is
`the total effect of the intervention beyond what would
`have otherwise occurred in the course of disease with-
`out treatment.” Placebo-controlled trials measure only
`the specific effect of treatment. In the case of thera-
`pies for which the psychological component of pla-
`cebo is large, placebo-controlled trials may show only
`a small effect size even when the total effect, includ-
`ing placebo, is of significant clinical value. We have
`examined this issue at greater length elsewhere [41]
`as has Feinstein [19], who argues that the role of “ia-
`trotherapy” is often overlooked in the evaluation of
`therapeutic techniques. The importance of nonspecific
`effects may also be of particular importance in drug
`cessation trials. For example, in a recent study of an-
`kle edema that compared open withdrawal of diuretic
`drugs to continued treatment, the authors argued that:
`“for general practitioners it is more relevant to know
`the total effect of stopping drugs openly” than to “sep-
`arate the pharmacological effect and the placebo ef-
`fect” [42]. Furthermore, important outcomes can
`sometimes depend on knowledge of treatment assign-
` [43], for instance, randomized 716
`ment. Little
`et al.
`primary care patients with sore throats to a prescrip-
`tion for antibiotics, no prescription, or a prescription
`that could be collected if symptoms persisted for 3
`days. In addition to measuring duration of illness, the
`study looked at outcomes such as intention to consult
`a doctor for similar attacks in the future, satisfaction
`with care, and antibiotic use. The authors argued that
`these outcomes could best be assessed if a trial “mim-
`icked perceptions and behaviour of everyday prac-
`tice.”
`Placebo-controlled trials interfere with nonspecific
`Numerous aspects of the clini-
`aspects of treatment.
`cian–patient relationship have been identified as mod-
`ifying the placebo response. Giving a prescription
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`160
`
`A.J. Vickers, A.J.M. de Craen / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 157–161
`
`3.
`
`with confidence, for example, has been shown to im-
`prove outcome [44]. Increased treatment effects have
`also been linked to patient expectation of benefit
`[45,46]. However, it seems probable that a placebo-
`controlled trial will reduce both clinician confidence
`and patient expectation: it is difficult to give a double-
`blind prescription with enthusiasm.
`Placebo-controlled trials do not inform real deci-
`Clinical practice does not normally involve a
`sions.
`decision between giving a treatment or a placebo.
`Typically, the decision that needs to be made is which
`of several treatments should be given or whether any
`treatment is preferable to no intervention. It is this
`second set of choices that should therefore determine
`the control groups in clinical trials. For instance,
`knowing that antidepressant A is better than placebo
`and that antidepressant B is also better than placebo
`may be useful, but does not help a psychiatrist choose
`which drug to prescribe.
`
`7. Discussion
`
`Placebo-controlled trials facilitate blinding and control
`for the placebo effect. However, they do not give informa-
`tion, such as effect size or comparative effectiveness of real-
`world choices, which is useful for clinicians in the field.
`They therefore have high internal validity, or rigor, and low
`external validity, or generalizability. A trial without a pla-
`cebo control may provide information that is more immedi-
`ately applicable to clinical practice. However, except in cir-
`cumstances such as a comparison between two drugs,
`blinding will not be possible and the trial will be more prone
`to bias. Trials without placebo control therefore have higher
`external validity at the possible expense of internal validity.
`Like so many other aspects of research, the choice of
`control group will ultimately depend on the question being
`asked. If researchers are interested in whether an interven-
`tion has effects over and above those of placebo, a placebo-
`controlled trial is clearly indicated. However, researchers
`need to think carefully about why it is important to know
`the extent of an intervention’s nonplacebo effects. We have
`argued that the relevance of this question should not be as-
`sumed to be self-evident. If researchers are interested apply-
`ing their results to clinical practice, a usual care or active
`treatment control group may be more appropriate. In such a
`comparison, researchers will need to consider carefully how
`they will minimize bias. In some cases, the risks of bias will
`outweigh the disadvantages of placebo control, especially
`where the use of placebo is unlikely to interfere signifi-
`cantly with usual clinical care.
`In conclusion, whatever might be implicit in the method-
`ological literature, the choice of a placebo control group in a
`clinical trial is not simple or unproblematic. A placebo-con-
`trolled trial is useful in determining whether an intervention
`has a specific effect. However, trials without placebo con-
`trol may be required to provide information of clinical
`
`value, such as the effect size of an intervention relative to an
`alternative treatment strategy. Such trials provide, in the
`words of Knottnerus and Dinant [47], “medicine-based evi-
`dence,” the obvious prerequisite for evidence-based medi-
`cine. We suggest that a wider debate needs to take place on
`the use of placebo controls and, more generally, on the na-
`ture of questions to be asked in clinical trials.
`
`Acknowledgments
`
`The authors thank Asbjorn Hrobjartsson of the Nordic
`Cochrane Centre for his comments on a draft version of the
`manuscript.
`
`References
`
`[1] Ho KH, Hashish I, Salmon P, Freeman R, Harvey W. Reduction of
`post-operative swelling by a placebo effect. J Psychosom Res 1988;
`32:197–205.
`[2] Morgan JP, Dickey JL, Hunt HH, Hudgins PM. A controlled trial of
`spinal manipulation in the management of hypertension. J Am Osteo-
`path Assoc 1985;85:308–13.
`[3] Kokjohn K, Schmid DM, Triano JJ, Brennan PC. The effect of spinal
`manipulation on pain and prostaglandin levels in women with pri-
`mary dysmenorrhea. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1992;15:279–85.
`[4] Moseley JB, Wray NP, Kuykendall D, Willis K, Landon G. Arthro-
`scopic treatment of ostearthritis of the knee: a prospective, random-
`ized, placebo-controlled study. Am J Sports Med 1996;24:28–34.
`[5] Campbell A. Hunting the snark: the quest for the perfect acupuncture
`placebo. Acupunc Med 1991;9:83–4.
`[6] Rothman KJ, Michels KB. The continuing unethical use of placebo
`controls. N Engl J Med 1994;33:394–7.
`[7] Clark PI, Leaverton PE. Scientific and ethical issues in the use of pla-
`cebo controls in clinical trials. Annu Rev Public Health 1994;15:19–38.
`[8] Ernst E, Resch KL. Concept of true and perceived placebo effects.
`BMJ 1995;311:551–3.
`[9] Haegerstam G, Huitfeldt B, Nilsson BS, et al. Placebo in clinical drug
`
`
`trials—a multidisciplinary review. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol
`1982;4:261–78.
`[10] Keating JC. The placebo issue and clinical research. J Manipulative
`Physiol Ther 1987;10:329–32.
`[11] Klerman GL. Scientific and ethical considerations in the use of pla-
`cebo controls in clinical trials in psychopharmacology. Psychophar-
`maco Bull 1986;22:25–29.
`[12] Leber P. The placebo control in clinical trials (a view from the FDA).
`Psychopharmaco Bull 1986;22:30–2.
`[13] Makuch RW, Johnson MF. Dilemmas in the use of active control
`groups in clinical research IRB Rev Hum Subjects Res 1989;11:1–5.
`[14] Pledger GW, Hall D. Active control trials: Do they address the effi-
`cacy issue?. Proc Am Stat Soc 1986:1–10.
`[15] Rickels K. Use of placebo in clinical trials. Psychopharmacol Bull
`1986;22:19–24.
`[16] Temple R. Government viewpoint of clinical trials. Drug Inform J
`1982;16:10–7.
`[17] Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chap-
`man and Hall 1991.
`[18] Campbell MJ, Machin D. Medical Statistics: a Commonsense Ap-
`proach. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons, 1993.
`[19] Feinstein AR. Clinical Epidemiology. The Architecture of Clinical
`Research. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Company, 1985.
`[20] Fletcher RH, Fletcher SW, Wagner EH. Clinical Epidemiology. The
`Essentials (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins, 1996.
`[21] Gordis L. Epidemiology. Philadelphia: W.B.G. Saunders Company,
`1996.
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`
`
`A.J. Vickers, A.J.M. de Craen / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 53 (2000) 157–161
`
`161
`
`[22] Greenberg RS. Medical Epidemiology. Norwalk, CT: Appleton &
`Lange, 1993.
`[23] Hennekens CH, Buring JE. Epidemiology in Medicine. Boston, MA:
`Little, Brown and Company, 1987.
`[24] Hulley SB, Cummings SR. Designing Clinical Research. Baltimore,
`MD: Williams & Wilkins, 1988.
`[25] Knapp RG, Miller III MC. Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics.
`Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins, 1992.
`[26] Kramer MS. Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics. Berlin/Heidel-
`berg: Springer-Verlag, 1988.
`[27] Meinert CL. Monographs in Epidemiology and Biostatistics. Clinical
`Trials. Design, Conduct, and Analysis (vol. 11). New York: Oxford
`University Press, 1986.
`[28] Morton RF, Hebel JR, McCarter RJ. A Study Guide to Epidemiol-
`ogy and Biostatistics (3rd ed.). Gaithersberg, MD: Aspen Publish-
`ers, Inc., 1989.
`[29] Pocock SJ. Clinical trials: A Practical Approach. Chichester: John
`Wiley and Sons, 1983.
`[30] Sackett DL, Haynes RB, Guyatt GH, Tugwell P. Clinical Epidemiol-
`ogy. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company, 1991.
`[31] Spilker B. Guide to Clinical Studies and Developing Protocols. New
`York: Raven Press, 1984.
`[32] Weiss NS. Monographs in Epidemiology and Biostatistics. Clinical
`Epidemiology: The Study of the Outcome of Illness (vol. 11). New
`York: Oxford University Press, 1986.
`[33] Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes RJ, Altman DG. Empirical evidence of
`bias. JAMA 1995;273:408–412.
`[34] Colditz GA, Miller JB, Mosteller F. How study design affects out-
`comes in comparisons of therapy. I: Medical. Stat Med 1989;8(4):
`441–54.
`[35] Turner JA, Deyo RA, Loeser JD, Von Korff M, Fordyce WE. The im-
`portance of placebo effects in pain treatment and research. JAMA
`1994;271:1609–14.
`
`[36] Gracely RH, Dubner R, Deeter WR, Wolskee PJ. Clinicians’ expecta-
`tions influence placebo analgesia. Lancet 1985;i:43.
`[37] Pastorello EA, Codecasa LR, Gerosa A, Buonocore E, Sillano V, Za-
`nussi C. The role of suggestion in asthma. I. Effects of inactive solu-
`tion on bronchial reactivity under bronchoconstrictor or bronchodila-
`tor suggestion. Ann Allergy 1987;59:336–8.
`[38] de Craen AJM, Roos PJ, de Vries AL, Kleijnen J. Effects of colour of
`drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs and of their ef-
`fectiveness. BMJ 1996;313:1624–6.
`[39] Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and
`verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain 1990;43:121–8.
`[40] Silagy C, Mant D, Fowler G, Lancaster T. Nicotine replacement ther-
`apy for smoking cessation. In: Lancaster T, Silagy C, Fullerton D (ed-
`itors) Tobacco Addiction Model of The Cochrane Database of Sys-
`tematic Reviews. Available in The Cochrane Library [database on
`disk and CDROM] The Cochrane Collaboration; Issue 4. Oxford:
`Update Software, 1998.
`[41] de Craen AJM, Vickers AJ, Tijssen JGP, Kleijnen J. The number-
`needed-to-treat and placebo-controlled trials. Lancet 1998;351:310.
`[42] de Jonge JW, Knottnerus JA, van Zutphen WM, de Bruijne GA, Stru-
`ijker Boudier HAJ. Short term effect of withdrawal of diuretic drugs
`prescribed for ankle oedema. BMJ 1994;308:511–3.
`[43] Little P, Williamson I, Warner G, Gould C, Gantley M, Kinmonth
`AL. Open randomised trial of prescribing strategies in managing sore
`throat. BMJ 1997;314:722–7.
`[44] Thomas KB. General practice consultations: is there any point in be-
`ing positive? BMJ 1987;294:1200.
`[45] Gryll SL, Katahn M. Situational factors contributing to the placebo
`effect. Psychopharmacology 1978;47:253–61.
`[46] Skovlund E. Should we tell trial patients that they might receive pla-
`cebos? Lancet 1991;337:1041.
`[47] Knottnerus JA, Dinant GJ. Medicine based evidence, a prerequisite
`for evidence based medicine. BMJ 1997;315:309–10.
`
`

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket