throbber
72
`
`IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON MAN-MACHINE SYSTEMS, VOL. MMS-11, No. 1, MARCH 1970
`
`An Electrotactile Display
`
`ROBERT M. STRONG, memBER, IEEE, AND DONALD E. TROXEL, MEMBER,
`
`IEEE
`
`Abstract—An explorable electrotactile display has been con-
`structed and tested. A thus far neglected sensation was identified
`and has been shown to be more useful than the more common
`electrotactile sensations. Exploration of the surface of
`the elec-
`trotactile display elicits a sensation describable as texture. Ex-
`periments have indicated that the intensity of this texture sensation
`is due primarily to the peak applied voltage rather than to current
`density as is the case for the classical electrotactile sensation. For
`subjects employing the texture sensation, experimental results
`are given for approximate thresholds and for the effect of electrode
`area on these thresholds. A boundary-localization measurement
`is offered as a measure of the usefulness of the display for textured-
`area presentation, and form-separation measurements are given
`as a measure of usefulness for line-drawing presentations. A pro-
`posed model for the mechanism producing the texture sensation
`is offered as a guide for future experimentation and display-engi-
`neering development.
`
`INTRODUCTION
`
`E HAVE BEENinvestigating electrotactile ma-
`
`W trix displays for use as an information input to
`
`the blind. Previous work concerned with electro-
`tactile displays has, for the most part, used rather large
`electrodes firmly attached to various parts of the body
`such as the chest, back, and arms. Ourinvestigations have
`been concerned primarily with an array of fairly small
`electrodes, which the subjects have been able to actively
`search with their fingers much as they might search an
`array of mechanical tactile stimulators.
`
`STIMULATOR CHARACTERISTICS
`
`The array consisted of small electrodes 70 mils in di-
`ameter that were spaced on 100-mil centers (Fig. 1). The
`maximum extent of the array was 1.0 inch wide by
`1.8 inches long. The heel of the subject’s hand rested on
`the single-return electrode. The pulses applied to the
`subject via the matrix of electrodes were of the bipolar
`rectangular type [as later shown in Figure 8(a)| from a
`source whose output impedance was 200 000 ohms. In all
`of the experiments reported here, unless otherwise noted,
`the pulse was symmetrical with a halfwidth of 0.60 ms,
`and a repetition rate of 200 pps.
`In most of the experiments the subjects had control of
`the stimulus amplitude, andin all cases they could easily
`interrupt the stimuli by removing their fingers from the
`display.
`
`revised October 2, 1969.
`Manuscript received July 21, 1969;
`This work was supported principally by the National Institutes
`of Health under Grants 5P01 GM15006-03 and 5P01 GM14940-03,
`and in part by the Joint Services Electronics Program under Con-
`tract DA28-043-AMC-02536[E].
`The authors are with the Department of Electrical Engineer-
`ing and Research Laboratory of Electronics, Massachusetts In-
`stitute of Technology, Cambridge, Mass. 02139.
`
`
`
`Pigek: Stimulator array.
`
`SENSATIONS ELIcrIrEeD
`types of sensa-
`The subjects reported two distinct
`tions. The first of these is similar to the sensation that
`Gibson [1] has reported when he used electrodes on the
`fingers; that is, that the sensations appear to be deep
`within the finger, indeed often concentrated in the joints,
`and that the sensation seems to progress up the finger,
`involving more of the finger as the stimulus amplitude
`is increased. Subjects reported that this type of sensation
`wasrelatively unpleasant and that it did not subjectively
`offer much information about the presentation. This sen-
`sation apparently has a mechanism moredirectly related
`to the peak stimulus current than to the voltage applied
`between the electrodes. A completely different sensation
`was experienced by most subjects when their fingers were
`dry and had,
`therefore, a high skin resistance. If the
`subject brushed his finger lightly over the surface, the
`surface appeared to acquire a texture, which could be
`varied by varying the stimulus parameters.’
`This sensation bears many of the properties of an
`ordinary texture sensation,
`the most
`important being
`that it is a relatively small amplitude effect, and that it
`disappears in the absence of finger motion. The sensation
`does not seem to be at all unpleasant, and its qualities
`
`1 The possibility that such a sensation existed, and its prob-
`able mechanism were suggested by Malinckrodt et al. [9].
`Valve Exhibit 1037
`Valve Exhibit 1037
`Valve v. Immersion
`Valve v. Immersion
`
`

`

`STRONG AND TROXEL: ELECTROTACTILE DISPLAY
`
`73
`
`TABLE I
`THRESHOLD VARIATIONS
`
`Threshold Value (volts peak)
`
`
`JD LBDate RK
`
`
`
`can be varied quite a bit by changing either the pulse-
`repetition rate or the peak voltage. The mechanism for
`this “texture” sensation seems to be directly related to
`the peak stimulus voltage, not to the stimulus current.
`The typical peak stimulus current at
`the sensation
`threshold for the texture sensation was of the order of
`50 vA, and a wide variation was noted. The peak voltages
`at threshold varied considerably with such circumstances
`as electrode size (see Experiment 2) but were generally
`of the same magnitude as those needed to elicit the
`sensations of Gibson [2] and were often lower.
`The texture sensation exhibits two peculiarities, both
`apparently due to skin-resistance effects. The first is the
`need to “warm up” the finger. Subjects would typically
`spend 3 to 5 minutes making scanning motions over the
`display surface when attempting to first acquire the sen-
`sation at the beginning of a session. It is believed that
`this serves the dual purpose of drying the skin surface
`and permitting appropriate adjustment of
`the finger
`pressure. The second phenomenon was a tendency for
`the texture’ sensation to fail or become sporadic. This
`appears to occur on days when the subject is, for un-
`known reasons, unable to increase his skin resistance
`sufficiently.
`
`EXPERIMENTS
`
`Several brief experiments were performed on three sub-
`jects in order to determine in a rough manner the char-
`acteristics of such a display. First, the characteristics of
`the sensation itself were examined, and results are pre-
`sented for the threshold of texture sensation, and the
`variation of that
`threshold with electrode area. The
`second group of experimentsis related to a display using
`“textured areas” as the presentation element. This group
`includes measurements of the just-noticeable differences
`for amplitude and pulse-repetition rate and the localiz-
`ability of a boundary. Finally, the applicability of the
`display to the presentation of point and line figures was
`investigated by measuring the minimum spacing needed
`to permit the user to determine that two such figures are
`distinct and not part of a larger figure.
`The first two experiments presented involve threshold
`measurements. These thresholds were measured by de-
`tecting both the ascending and descending limits in
`alternate sequences until 15 pairs of values had been
`accumulated for each measurement. The measurements
`were repeated on successive sessions over a two-week
`period. Since the number of samples is small, these re-
`sults, like those of the experiments that appear later in
`the paper, should be taken as indications of the range in
`which the actual values of the parameters measured can
`be expected to fall, not as accurate measurements of the
`parameters themselves.
`
`Experiment 1—Texture Threshold
`
`Table I shows the variation of the threshold of sensa-
`tion over the three subjects and the range into which the
`thresholds fall for the texture sensation on a single
`
`25 /24
`
`49
`
`31
`
`20/36
`
`47/40
`
`November 23
`November 24
`November 25
`December 1
`December 2
`December 3
`December4
`December 5
`December 6
`26
`December 7
`21
`December 14
`37
`December 16
`
`
`29
`37
`
`26
`
`43/36
`
`pattern presentation. The pattern used is shown in Fig.
`2(a), and the surrounding electrodes were grounded. All
`values reported are measurements in volts of peak pulse
`amplitude.
`A remarkably wide variation exists between subjects.
`It should be noted also, however, that a large variation
`occurs for a single subject from one session to the next.
`Experiment 2—Effect of Electrode Area on Threshold
`A number of threshold measurements were made in
`order to determine the effect of the electrode area on the
`threshold of touch. Over a period of two weeks and at
`least four sessions for each subject, measurements were
`made of the thresholds for five different patterns of ex-
`cited electrodes. These patterns are shownin Fig. 2(b)-(f).
`Measurements of each threshold were made on at least
`two different days and the results averaged to arrive at
`the result presented. The results are presented as a plot
`of threshold versus an area parameter, measured in units
`of electrode area. In Fig. 3 are shown the averageresults
`for three subjects using the texture sensation.
`These results show a reduction in threshold with an
`increase in electrode area. This result appears to be con-
`trary to the findings of Gibson et al. [2] for the previously
`reported electrotactile sensations. Included on the graph
`is a curve of the form
`
`J
`T=V/A;
`
`the shape predicted in part by a model for the produc-
`tion of the texture sensation, which appears at the end
`of this paper.
`In the development of the textured area form of the
`display, the primary considerations have been the ability
`of the subject to distinguish between textures and to
`locate the boundary between two areas of different tex-
`tures in a situation similar to that expected in the use of
`an explorable display. The presentations for the experi-
`ments described below are,
`therefore, all based on the
`simultaneous presentation of two different textures on
`two disjoint but immediately adjacent areas of the dis-
`play array. In all cases the size of an area was maintained
`larger than the finger pad so that the subject could feel
`
`

`

`74
`
`IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON MAN-MACHINE SYSTEMS, MARCH 1970
`
`200

`O0OO000000
`900


`0060

`OOQOO00CO
`00
`REGION ||OQOQOQQOOOOO nes
`(a)
`(d)
`
`08000
`90000
`60000
`000
`
`(b)
`QQ
`0060
`0000
`oo
`
`oo
`°°
`
`(e)
`

`
`REGION 2
`
`OOOOOO0OO
`
`© SouRcE |
`© SOURCE 2
`
`Electrode patterns for threshold measurements.
`
`Fig. 4. Presentations for JND experiments.
`
`TABLE II
`AMPLITUDE JusT NoTICEABLE DIFFERENCES
`
`
`
` Level JD LB RK
`
`
`
`
`
`Near Threshold
`JND (volts)
`Center (volts)
`Percent
`Midrange
`JND (volts)
`Center (volts)
`Percent
`Near Maximum
`6.04
`5.85
`3.25
`IND (volts)
`77.2
`64.1
`54.1
`Center (volts)
`
`
`
`6.0 10.8Percent 7.8
`
`1.32
`23.2
`5.7
`
`3.17
`43.4
`7.3
`
`1.77
`30.9
`5.7
`
`2.35
`38.6
`6.1
`
`1,20
`23.2
`5.2
`
`2.24
`39.6
`5.8
`
`
`
`e AVERAGE THRESHOLD
`(THREE SUBJECTS)
`

`
`
`
`\Tee
`2/a
`
`Lt
`l
`L Lo L
`IL
`L
`i
`L
`L
`i
`4
`8
`12
`16
`20
`24
`28
`AREA (ELECTRODE AREA UNITS)
`
`r 40
`o>
`uo)
`¥ 30
`
`a 5o
`
`O 20
`xWw)ta
`== 10
`
`0
`
`0
`
`Fig. 3. Effect of area on texture threshold.
`
`the textures independently if he wished. The boundary
`between the two areas was always a straight line, and
`its location was always specified along an axis perpendic-
`ularto it (Fig. 4).
`
`Experiment 3—Just Noticeable Difference for Amplitude
`This measurement was made by fixing the amplitude
`of one area and varying the amplitude of the other. For
`each measurement 100 presentations were made. In each
`presentation the lower area was fixed at the center am-
`plitude and the upper area was fixed at an amplitude
`selected by a uniform random choice from a predetermined
`range. The stimuli thus differed only in amplitude and
`location. The experiment was performed three times, once
`each at 10 percent above the ascending threshold, at the
`“most comfortable” level selected by the subject, and at
`approximately 10 percent below the maximum level the
`subject would accept for long periods. The test was of the
`two-alternative-forced-choice type,
`the choices being
`“bottom is stronger” and ‘‘top is stronger.”
`The resulting data were treated by determining the
`percentage of the time the response “bottom” was given
`for a particular stimulus pair, and graphing the results.
`The reported just noticeable difference (JND) is then one
`half the interquartile range on that graph, or where per-
`centages are given, the percentage that this figure repre-
`sents of
`the center value of
`the same measurement
`(Table ID.
`
`Experiment 4—Just Noticeable Difference for Frequency
`In order to determine approximately the JND for
`frequency,
`the subjects were presented with the same
`situation described in the previous experiment, but with
`stimuli that differed only in pulse-repetition rate. In all,
`75 presentations were made for each measurement. In
`each presentation the lower area was fixed in frequency
`at 200 pps. Only one determination was made, and the
`range from which the samples were selected for the upper
`area was the entire useful frequency range of 100 to
`1000 pps.
`The same spatial presentation was used as in Experi-
`ment 3, and again the subject was permitted as much
`time as he wanted to make a decision. Decision times
`averaged about 10 secondsforall subjects. The presenta-
`tion amplitude was set at the subject’s most comfortable
`level, and the amplitudes of the two areas were identical.
`No attempt was made, however, to correct for the effect
`of pulse-repetition rate on the subjective strength, and so
`the stimuli were subjectively the same strength only
`when the frequencies were identical.
`The subjects were asked to designate which of the two
`areas was “coarser,” a term that had come into common
`use to designate frequency-based texture differences. The
`resulting data were treated as in Experiment 3 (Table IID).
`The results are consistent with their reports in other
`situations,
`though the JND appears to be somewhat
`larger than might have been hopedfor.
`
`

`

`STRONG AND TROXEL: ELECTROTACTILE DISPLAY
`
`75
`
`TABLE III
`TABLE IV
`ArEa-BounpARY LOCALIZATION
`Frequency Just NoriceaBLe DIFFERENCES
`
`
`Subject
`IND
`Percentage
`JD
`LB
`RK
`
`
`Near threshold
`38.0
`76 pps
`JD
`—1.86
`0.74
`0.16
`mean*
`LB
`83 pps
`41.5
`
`
`
`
`RK Error<variancet77 pps 38.5 0.14 1.09 2.02
`
`
`
`amplitudet
`22.9
`24.3
`15.4
`:
`Twice threshold
`~1.58
`0.21
`0.19
`Center frequency is 200 pps.
`mean
`
`Error4variance 0.38 0.25 2.48
`
`
`amplitude
`45.7
`48.5
`30.8
`Preferred amplitude
`The remaining three experiments in the textured-area
`0.20
`0.04
`1.94
`
`
`group were performed with the same stimulus arrange- Error¢variance 0.16 0.47 1.96
`
`
`amplitude
`27.0
`26.7
`23.1
`ment. In each case, two areas of the same form used in
`
`the JND experiments were used, but with the location of
`the transition from one stimulus region to the other
`variable. In each case,
`the stimuli remained constant
`throughout
`the experiment. Each measurement repre-
`sents 50 presentations. Each presentation had a number
`N of rows of pins in the upper-electrode group connected
`to one source, and the remainderof the pins in the array
`connected to a second source.
`The subject was permitted to explore the display for
`an unlimited period of time but was requested to respond
`quickly. He was asked to specify the number of rows in
`the upper area. The resulting data were, in each case,
`analyzed to derive the mean and variance of the localiza-
`tion error in one dimension.
`
`* Meansare in tenths of inches.
`} Variances are in hundredths of square inches.
`¢ Amplitudes are in volts.
`
`distinet boundary, the boundary in this case was more
`like an indistinct region than like a sharp transition. This
`remained true even for very large amplitude differences,
`only disappearing when the border was emphasized by
`grounding a row of pins. No experiment was performed
`to test this technique.
`The results appear in Table V. The amplitude values
`used are recorded as well as the mean and variance of the
`errors. As might be expected, these results show a some-
`what larger variance than the previous experiment, but
`it is not excessively large.
`
`Experiment 7—Frequency Boundary
`The previous boundary-localization experiment was
`also carried out with a 2-JND difference in frequency.
`The procedures were the same as was the method of data
`analysis. In this case, the frequencies and amplitude were
`taken from the results of the frequency JND experiment.
`The results appear as Table VI. With the notable ex-
`ception of the results of JD, which show small variance,
`these results are similar to those of the amplitude-bound-
`ary experiment. The subjects again reported that the
`boundary was not a distinct thing, but rather an indistinct
`region between twoareas of distinctly different texture.
`
`Experiment 8—Form-Separation Measurements
`The last group of measurements to be presented are
`related to point-and-line presentations. In each of the
`measurements given below the subject was presented
`with two figures whose separation was variable. The
`figures were all simple points and lines. The unexcited
`areas of the display were connected to ground. The sub-
`jects were premitted to select the amplitude that they
`felt most comfortable with, much as they would in using
`a point-and-line display.
`The subject was informed of the shape of the pattern,
`and was allowed to adjust the amplitude to a comfortable
`level. He was then instructed to report either that he
`could feel two distinct forms or that they were merged.
`It was impressed on the subject that a report of “separate”
`
`Experiment 5—Area-Boundary Localizations
`
`The boundary between an “on” and an “off” or
`grounded area was explored at three amplitudes, thresh-
`old plus 10 percent, twice that value, and at the level
`preferred by the subject as “best.” In each case, the
`upper portion of the array was excited with a standard
`pulse and the lower region of the array was grounded.
`The results are presented in Table IV. Decision times
`were on the order of 30 seconds.
`Subject RK reported having difficulty maintaining the
`sensation on the day these data were taken, and his skin
`resistance was abnormally low. It is presumed that this
`is the reason for the large variance that his results exhibit.
`
`Experiment 6—Amplitude Boundary
`
`In order to obtain an initial measurement of the ability
`of a subject to locate the boundary between two areas
`excited by signals that were identical except for differing
`amplitudes, an experiment was performed using the
`results of Experiment 3. Using the same stimuli as in
`the “best amplitude” case of that experiment, with the
`amplitudes differing by 2 times the measured JND, the
`procedure of Experiment 5 was repeated.
`At this level of difference all of the subjects felt that
`the difference in amplitudes was easily detectable. Any
`smaller difference, however, would elicit complaints that
`the difference was not always clear. Those subjects
`utilizing the texture sensation reported that unlike the
`situation of the previous experiment that provided a
`
`

`

`76
`
`IREE TRANSACTIONS ON MAN-MACHINE SYSTEMS, MARCH 1970
`
`TABLE V
`Two AREA-TRANSITION LocALIZATION—AMPLITUDE DIFFERENCES*
`
`Subject
`Center Amplitude (volts)
`Error
`Meant
`Variancet
`
`
`1.47
`—0.166
`39.6
`JD
`1.56
`—0.024
`43.4
`LB
`1.88
`0.106
`38.6
`RK
`
`
`* Localization of the boundary between two areas whoseexcitation
`differs by two JND’s in amplitude.
`+ Mean error is measured in tenths of inches.
`t Error variance is measured in hundredths of square inches,
`
`TABLE VI
`Two Arga-TRaAnsitTion LocaLizaTION—FREQUENCY DIFFERENCES*
`
`Error
`Subject
`Amplitude (volts)
`Meant
`Variance}
`
`
`0.078
`0.71
`39.6
`JD
`2.70
`0.311
`43.4
`LB
`3.67
`0.74
`38.6
`RK
`
`
`* Localization of the boundary between two areas whose excitation
`differs by two JND’sin frequency.
`+ Meanerror is measured in tenths of an inch.
`t Error variance is measured in hundredths of a square inch.
`
`was to be given only if they were felt to be separate, not
`if they “might”’ be separate.
`The data collected have been organized to show the
`percentage of responses “‘separate” that were elicited for
`each numberof grounded pins separating the twofigures,
`and an average over the subject set is also given. Since
`the measurement
`is relatively coarse, due to the con-
`straint of pin size in the available electrode array, the
`results are given in tabular form. Each set of results
`represents 75 presentations to each subject with separa-
`tion distances ranging from 0 to 5 pins. The presentations
`were made in a random order, with no attempt to con-
`strain the response time. Response times averaged about
`10 seconds.
`The patterns are shownin Fig. 5, and the corresponding
`results as Tables VII-IX. Note that the measurement
`given X is the number of unexcited electrodes between
`figures. Therefore, the minimum distance between elec-
`trode segments that are excited is 0.LX ++ 0.03 inch.
`
`PRoposED MECHANISM AND MopEL FoR TEXTURE
`SENSATION
`
`The mechanism that we propose is that an electrically
`induced variation in the vertical force between the sub-
`ject’s skin surface and the display electrode is converted
`by the friction mechanism into a variation in the lateral
`force (tangential to the skin surface) as the subject passes
`his finger across the electrode. This variation in lateral
`force then causes motions of the portions of skin in con-
`tact with the surface over the electrode to vary about the
`relatively constant motion of the whole finger. The re-
`
`L x2
`C00®00@000
`—{
`--O.l in
`
`(a)
`908000800
`
`o0ecoceDo
`x3
`(b)
`oooogosCo
`
`BQO00COBDO
`AP
`fae
`
`(c)
`
`Fig. 5. Presentations for form-separation test.
`
`TABLE VII
`SEPARATION OF Two Points
`
`
`Separation X
`(in point
`spacings) RK
`LB
`JD
`Average
`
`
`Subject Responses (percent)
`
`2.7
`8.3
`0
`0
`0
`5.5
`0
`16.6
`0
`i
`34.6
`58
`83.3
`12.5
`2
`93.1
`94
`88.2
`97
`3
`4
`100
`100
`100
`100
`5
`100
`100
`100
`100
`
`
`-
`
`Necessary separation is 3 points.
`
`TABLE VIII
`SEPARATION OF Two LINES
`
`
`Separation X
`(in point
`spacings) RK
`LB
`JD
`Avetage
`
`
`Subject Responses (percent)
`
`0
`0
`0
`0
`0
`0
`0
`0
`0
`1
`42.4
`42
`68.6
`16.6
`2
`95
`94
`100
`91
`3
`100
`100
`100
`100
`4
`
`
`
`
`100 100 1005 100
`
`Necessary separation is 3 points.
`
`TABLE IX
`SEPARATION OF A Porn’ FROM A LINE
`
`Separation X
`(in point
`spacings)
`RK
`LB
`JD
`Average
`
`
`Subject Responses (percent)
`
`5.5
`0
`0
`17
`0
`39
`0
`33
`83
`1
`96.5
`100
`98
`91
`2
`100
`100
`100
`100
`3
`100
`100
`100
`100
`4
`
`
`
`
`100 100 1005 100
`
`Necessary separation is 2 points.
`
`

`

`STRONG AND TROXEL: ELECTROTACTILE DISPLAY
`
`q7
`
`quired variation in vertical force is assumed to be gener-
`ated by electric-charge accumulations in the subject’s
`finger and on theelectrode surface.
`The following symbols will be used in the discussion.
`
`FINGER MOTION
`——_____
` SKIN
`fy
`SURFACE
`
`TABLE
`SURFACE
`
`ELECTRODES
`INSULATING
`PLASTIC
`
`Fig. 6. Applicable forces.
`
`values measured for mechanical thresholds in the tangen-
`tial direction are similar to those for vertical motion,
`and about 10 microns (about 4 X 10~* in) peak-to-peak.
`displacement [3], [5], [6].
`The value of mechanical impedance in the tangential
`direction has been measured as about 0.48 Ib/in [3], [4].?
`It is not practical to directly measure the coefficient of
`friction, as it depends on a great many factors including
`applied finger pressure, atmospheric humidity, and indi-
`vidual skin factors, especially perspiration. However,
`subjects indicate a coefficient 4, for the finger in contact
`with the dry electrode, of the order of 1, with a maximum
`variation in that judgment of less than a factor of 3 in
`either direction. This value was the result of simply
`asking the subjects to estimate the ratio of horizontal to
`vertical force in their finger motions. We would, there-
`fore, expect to require a vertical-force variation on the
`order of 2 X 107‘ Ib, peak to peak.
`The electrical model used, in the form of a pair of
`closely spaced parallel plates of the sameslice, is shown
`in Fig. 7. The electrically induced force between the two
`“plates” and the capacitance of the system are given by
`
`f.(t)
`
`AG
`
`F,
`
`Be
`
`f. = wf.
`Zn
`
`a(t)
`
`A
`
`C
`
`The vertical componentof force (pounds)
`between the skin and theelectrode.
`The variation with time of any function
`G(é) about its average value (in the units
`of GO).
`The force (pounds) applied by the sub-
`ject in the vertical direction. (It is as-
`sumed to be essentially constant).
`The coefficient of friction for the skin-
`display surface pair.
`in
`The total tangential force (pounds)
`steady-state finger motion.
`The mechanical stiffness, in pounds per
`inch, of the skin in the direction tangen-
`tial to the skin surface.”
`The position (inches) of the local skin
`surface with respect
`to a coordinate
`framefixed to the finger.
`The thickness (inches) of insulator layer ¢.
`T;
`The dielectric constant of insulator layer7.
`€;
`T; = T;/s; The effective thickness (inches) of ma-
`terial 7.
`The area of skin contact with the elec-
`trode.
`The capacitance (farads) of the skin sur-
`face in contact with the electrode.
`The instantaneous voltage applied to
`vo)
`__&Afv()]?_
`the subject.
`Ah = 34 fy)
`The peak voltage applied in the jth ex-
`V;
`perimental case.
`s (subscript) applies to the subject’s skin.
`p (subscript) applies to the plastic insulator.
`= 8.854 X 10°7'*f/m is the permittivity of free space.
`
`GA
`CRG:
`
`The pertinent forces are shown in Fig. 6. Mathemati-
`cally, this system can be described as follows:
`
`f. = F, + Af,
`
`fh =uf,
`
`Af, = u Af,
`
`Aa(t) = Af.()/Zn-
`
`Wewish to show that Az(é) is of the same magnitude
`as the motion required for mechanical stimulation of the
`skin in a tangential direction, at frequencies near the
`pulse frequency used to elicit. the texture sensation. The
`
`that the mechanical impedance
`[4]
`2Tt has been shown [3],
`of the skin, for small
`relative displacements,
`is essentially a
`spring in nature, with a constant stiffness in the range of dis-
`placements encountered in these experiments.
`
`In order to extract the parameters of this model, in
`particular the skin thickness, an experiment was per-
`formed. It consisted of measuring the threshold of sensa-
`tion for each of three conditions.
`The active electrode consisted of the entire right half
`of the display array, much larger than the subject’s finger-
`tip. This was done three times in scrambled order, and
`the results averaged to yield the values given in Table X.
`Note that only one subject was used to derive these values.
`These values were measured on only one subject, and
`only one day. While variations in the specific voltage.
`values do occur,
`the same calculated values were ob-
`
`3 This value was not measured on the finger pads. It was meas-
`ured for small excursions (less than 6 mils) and we can expect
`that the skin of the finger pad is not significantly stiffer over:
`small excursions than the skin of the upper arm where the meas-
`urement was made. Franke’s measurements unfortunately do not
`apply directly because of the large areas of contact and large
`excursions involved,
`though they indirectly support
`the figure-
`given [4].
`
`

`

`78
`
`LOW
`Teterion eM)
`
`weEsSII ee
`
`INSULATING PLASTIC
`PLASTIC '
`ELECTRODE
`
`(a)
`
`PLATE #
`(VERY LARGE AREA)
`Ts
`T
`
`és
`€p
`
`tELECTRODE
`
`(b)
`
`SKIN SURFACE LAYER
`INTERPOSED
`INSULATOR
`
`Fig. 7. Electrical model.
`
`TABLE X
`
`
`Condition
`Average Peak Voltage at Threshold (volts)
`
`
`17
`No insulator
`73
`0.5-mil insulator
`110
`1.0-mil insulator
`
`
`tained with another subject.* It should be noted that
`these figures also reflect a large difference in the coeffi-
`cient of friction between the insulated and uninsulated
`cases, so that the insulated and uninsulated cases cannot
`be directly compared.
`The insulator used was polyvinylidene chloride (Dow
`Saran); its pertinent properties are shown in Table XI.
`In the experimental situation there is a return electrode
`under the palm of the same hand in addition to the con-
`tact at the fingertip. The results of other experiments,
`not reported here, indicate that this electrode is usually
`quite wet from perspiration, and can thus be neglected
`when compared to the dry fingertip contact. Skin re-
`sistances measured in this experimental situation on the
`order of 2 X 10° ohms with no interposed insulator and
`dry finger electrodes, and 5000-10 000 ohms with both
`electrodes wet. No measurable current flows with the
`insulator between the finger and the electrode.
`It has been assumed that the tangential-force varia-
`tion on the skin is the sameat all three threshold meas-
`urements shown in Table X. The voltage pulse train
`applied to the user’s finger is shown in Fig. 8(a). The as-
`sumption is made that the time constant (RC product)
`of the skin is much smaller than the voltage pulsewidth,
`in order to guarantee the rectangular shape of the force
`pulse.
`The variation in tangential force is thus just the peak
`value of the electrically produced force, or
`
`yA Wmax)
`“he = 30 Ef) 2 May
`
`4Skin thicknesses computed using the model change consid-
`erably, but calculated forces do not.
`
`IERE TRANSACTIONS ON MAN-MACHINE SYSTEMS, MARCH 1970
`
`TABLE XI
`
`PROPERTIES OF THE INSULATOR
`
`
`Dielectric constant
`
`3.5-5.5
`(approximately 4.5 at 200 Hz)
`350 V/mil
`Dielectric strength
`> 10% O-em
`Resistivity
`
`'
`I
`~—5 millisec —4
`
`
`0.6
`
`millisec
`
`0.6
`
`
`millisec
`
`“Vmax
`
`
`
`max
`
`f
`
`(a)
`1
`aye
`!
`kx 5 millisec
`1.2
`millisec
`
`(b)
`
`Fig. 8. Voltage and pulse waveforms.
`
`giving
`
`6AM, max) =
`EA (v, max) -=
`6Alu; max) _
`“OP +h.) “2h+t,)
`“2h. +f.)
`
`Manipulation of the force functions for the two insu-
`lated cases, noting that 7,, = 27, and uw. = ps, can
`be made to give
`
`the
`thickness,
`For each 0.50 X 107° inch of physical
`insulating material used has an effective thickness of
`Tf, = 0.111 X 10% in = 7,,.
`
`This gives an effective thickness of the dry surface skin
`layer to be
`
`T, = 0.108 x 107°
`
`The variation in vertical force generated by the model,
`using this value of skin thickness and the threshold volt-
`age measured for the no-insulator case of the experi-
`mentis
`
`Af, = 93.3 X 10°° lb perelectrode point.
`
`Since the voltages needed to elicit sensation become
`extremely high for single-electrode presentations, the ex-
`periment used here was performed using an array of active
`electrodes much larger than the finger pad. This fact
`makes it difficult, however, to determine the actual con-
`tact area being used by the subject. The result is, there-
`fore, reported in terms of the force per electrode area,
`which is 3.846 X 10°° in. It is estimated that the maxi-
`mum possible contact area is about 5 points, and the
`minimum about 1 point.
`
`

`

`IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON MAN-MACHINE SYSTEMS, VOL. MMS-11, No. 1, MaRcH 1970
`
`79
`
`This value of force does not, of course, take into account
`the surface coefficient of friction. However, unless the
`subjects have consistently and repeatedly overestimated
`the coefficient of friction, the resultant tangential force
`would be greater than the expected force needed to cause
`sensation by the proposed mechanism.
`Earlier it was assumed that the RC time constant for
`the skin was small compared to the pulsewidth. To show
`that this is true, we calculate the value of this surface
`capacitor as
`
`C=
`
`
`eA
`T
`
`C = 8.56 pF per point electrode.
`
`the maximum value of capacitance is about
`Thus,
`45 pF, and the corresponding RC product, using the skin
`resistance for the case of two wet electrodes, becomes
`45 X 107° second. This value is clearly much smaller
`than the pulsewidth of 10°° second. Thus, we can ex-
`pect the full effect of the rectangular force pulse.
`
`CONCLUSION
`
`Theexistence of a texture effect produced by an elec-
`trical stimulator has been demonstrated. We have pre-
`sented a justification for a model of this effect in which a
`physical motion of the skin is caused by the potential
`difference between the clectrode and the interior side of
`the skin. The texture effect has been clearly distinguished
`from the usual type of electrical stimulation by noting a
`
`direct dependence of the perceived stimulus intensity and
`the applied voltage rather than the usual result of the
`stimulus intensity being a function of the applied current.
`Indeed, the use of an insulator between the electrode and
`skin produces no apparent change in the perceptual
`qualities of the texture effect, while the resulting current
`is several orders of magnitude lower than that normally
`required to elicit electrotactile sensations. An explorable
`tactile display has been built that utilizes this effect, and
`the results of preliminary experiments indicate that a
`person can resolve details presented by this electro-
`tactile display.
`
`REFERENCES
`{1] R.H. Gibson, private communication, April 1967.
`[2] —~, “Electrical stimulation of
`the skin senses,” Progress
`Rept. 4, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh,
`Pittsburgh, Pa., February 1, 1967.
`[3] D. S. Alles, “Kinesthetic feedback system for amputees via
`the tactile sense,” Sc.D. dissertation. Department of Mechan-
`ical Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cam-
`bridge, 1968.
`[4] E. K. Franke, “Mechanical impedance of the surface of the
`human body,” J. Appl. Phystol., vol. 3, pp. 582-590, 1950-1951.
`[5] R. T. Verrillo, “Investigation of some parameters of
`the
`cutaneous threshold of vibration,” J. Acous. Soc. Am., vol.
`34,

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket