`
`Individual dierences in brain dynamics: important implications
`for the calculation of event-related band power
`
`M. Doppelmayr, W. Klimesch, T. Pachinger, B. Ripper
`
`Department of Physiological Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstr. 34, A-5020 Salzburg, Austria
`
`Received: 22 July 1997 / Accepted in revised form: 22 April 1998
`
`Abstract. Measures of event-related band power such as
`event-related desynchronization (ERD) are convention-
`ally analyzed within ®xed frequency bands, although it is
`known that EEG frequency varies as a function of a
`variety of factors. The question of how to determine
`these frequency bands for ERD analyses is discussed and
`a new method is proposed. The rationale of this new
`method is to adjust the frequency bands to the individ-
`ual alpha frequency (IAF) for each subject and to
`determine the bandwidth for the alpha and theta bands
`as a percentage of IAF. As an example, if IAF equals
`12 Hz, the widths of the alpha and theta bands are larger
`as compared to a subject with an IAF of, e.g., only 8 Hz.
`The results of an oddball paradigm show that the
`proposed method is superior to methods that are based
`on ®xed frequencies and ®xed bandwidths.
`
`1 Introduction
`
`Although it is well known that alpha (the dominant
`EEG frequency) varies as a function of age, EEG
`frequencies are conventionally subdivided into ®xed
`frequency bands such as theta (4±8 Hz), alpha (8±
`13 Hz), beta (14±30 Hz), and gamma (30±70 Hz). From
`childhood to puberty the mean EEG frequency increases
`with age (Epstein 1980), but then decreases for the
`remaining life span. After puberty the alpha frequency
`starts to decline with increasing age. As an example,
`KoÈ pruner et al. (1984) have found a linear relationship
`within the age range of adult subjects. They have shown
`that a young adult of, for example, 20 years has an
`expected alpha frequency of about 11 Hz, whereas a 70-
`year old subject shows a drop of 2.65 Hz down to a
`frequency of 8.24 Hz. In addition, data from our
`laboratory have indicated that even subjects of the same
`age show a considerable variability in alpha frequency,
`
`Correspondence to: M. Doppelmayr
`(e-mail: michael.doppelmayr@sbg.ac.at,
`Tel. +43-662-80445136 or -5100, Fax: +43-662-80445126)
`
`with a mean standard deviation of about 1 Hz (e.g., the
`reviews in Klimesch 1996, 1997). This means that even
`for age-matched subjects, an interindividual dierence of
`about 2 Hz is quite a common case. Klimesch et al.
`(1990, 1993) have found evidence that these interindi-
`vidual dierences in alpha frequency are largely due to
`interindividual dierences in memory performance.
`Because of this large interindividual variability in al-
`pha frequency, signi®cant portions of alpha power will
`fall outside a ®xed frequency window when event-related
`desynchronization (ERD), a method originally proposed
`by Pfurtscheller and Aranibar (1977), or other types of
`band power measures are calculated. As an example, let
`us consider a subject with a low alpha frequency and let
`us assume that the lower alpha band falls below the
`frequency window of the ®xed band, which then covers
`only the upper alpha and some portions of the lower
`beta bands. In this case, event-related changes in the
`lower alpha band cannot be detected and changes in the
`upper alpha band will be misinterpreted if a ®xed band is
`used. This example demonstrates that frequency bands
`should be adjusted individually for each subject. One
`method that was applied in earlier studies (Klimesch
`et al. 1996a, 1996b, 1997a) in our laboratory was to use
`the individual alpha frequency (IAF) as an anchor point
`for distinguishing a lower from an upper alpha band.
`Although this method proved superior to the use of ®xed
`frequency bands (Schimke et al. 1990), the question still
`is whether the bandwidth may be considered a constant
`value that does not vary as a function of IAF. Before we
`consider this question in more detail, we want to show
`that besides the variation in IAF, the strikingly dierent
`reactions of the lower and upper alpha bands and the
`transition from alpha desynchronization to theta syn-
`chronization provide additional arguments for an indi-
`vidual adjustment of frequency bands.
`The results from principal component analyses have
`repeatedly shown that power values in the alpha band
`load on two dierent and orthogonal components, with
`highest loadings in the lower and upper alpha bands. As
`an example, Mecklinger et al. (1992) found two or-
`thogonal components, one with highest loadings be-
`tween 7 and 11 Hz and a second with highest loadings
`
`Wave Neuroscience EX2003
`PeakLogic v. Wave Neuroscience
`IPR2022-01550
`
`
`
`50
`
`between 10 and 13 Hz. These data indicate that power
`values of the lower and upper alpha bands vary largely
`independently of each other. Experiments from our
`laboratory have shown that lower alpha ERD varies as a
`function of attentional demands whereas upper alpha
`ERD is associated with (semantic) memory demands
`(Klimesch 1996, 1997). Thus, the use of individually
`adjusted frequency bands is essential to prevent task-
`related band power changes in the lower and upper al-
`pha bands overlapping and cancelling each other.
`Another important ®nding that questions the use of
`®xed frequency bands is based on the fact that, with
`increasing task demands, alpha desynchronizes whereas
`theta synchronizes (see the reviews in Schacter 1977 and
`Klimesch 1996). These opposite changes in theta and
`alpha band power can be observed if a reference condi-
`tion in which subjects are in a state of alert wakefulness
`is compared with a test condition in which the subjects
`perform some type of task. In a recent study (Klimesch
`et al. 1996a) we were able to demonstrate that the
`transition between alpha desynchronization and theta
`synchronization occurs within a narrow frequency range
`that varies as a function of IAF. This frequency, where
`theta synchronization gives way to alpha desynchroni-
`zation, is called the transition frequency (TF). Conse-
`quently, if rather broad frequency bands are used which
`are not adjusted to IAF, these eects of an event-related
`increase (synchronization) and decrease (desynchroni-
`zation) in band power will tend to cancel each other.
`It is the purpose of the present paper to show the
`usefulness of a new method for determining frequency
`bands as well as the bandwidth individually for each
`subject. It is proposed to use IAF as cut-o frequency
`between the lower and upper alpha bands and to use a
`percentage of IAF to determine the width of frequency
`bands. When attempting to determine the bandwidth
`individually, the crucial question is: What criterion
`should be used? We proceed from the idea that TF
`marks the cut-o frequency between the theta and lower
`alpha bands. In previous research we have found that
`TF lies at about 4 Hz below IAF and is signi®cantly
`correlated with IAF (cf. Klimesch et al. 1996a). It should
`be noted, however, that TF shows a variation of about
`0.5 Hz between dierent studies. In tasks where theta
`synchronization dominates, TF lies somewhat higher (at
`about 3.5 Hz below IAF) as compared to tasks where
`alpha desynchronization dominates. In the latter case,
`TF may be on average as low as about 4.5 Hz below
`IAF. Because a mean bandwidth of 4 Hz proved useful
`in previous studies, we suggest to uses steps of 20% IAF
`for an individual determination of the bandwidth. As an
`example, a subject with IAF 10 Hz would be ex-
`pected to show an individual bandwidth of 2 ´ 2 Hz for
`the lower alpha band. We have found (e.g., Klimesch
`et al. 1996a) that the width of the lower alpha band is
`about twice as wide as that of the upper band. Thus, for
`the analysis of ERD, we distinguish between three steps
`of 20% IAF, two steps below and 1 step above IAF. The
`two frequency bands below IAF are termed lower-1 and
`lower-2-alpha, respectively. Because we have found that
`the TF between alpha de- and theta synchronization lies
`
`at about 4 Hz below the IAF of about 10 Hz, and be-
`cause TF and IAF are signi®cantly correlated (cf.
`Klimesch et al. 1996a), we suggest to use steps of 20%
`IAF to cover the range of individual theta and alpha
`bands. Using individually determined frequency bands
`and bandwidths avoids a confusion between event-relat-
`ed changes in the lower and upper alpha band and pre-
`vents the desynchronizing and synchronizing responses
`of the alpha and theta bands cancelling each other.
`In order to show that the proposed method, which
`uses individually de®ned bands and widths (IBIW), is
`indeed superior to a method using ®xed bands and ®xed
`widths (FBFW) and also superior to a method using
`individually adjusted bands but ®xed widths (IBFW),
`control computations must be performed. For that
`purpose we use data from an oddball paradigm (in
`which subjects have to respond to rare target and ignore
`frequent nontarget stimuli) and distinguish two groups
`of subjects, one with high alpha frequency and another
`with low alpha frequency. Then, by applying the dif-
`ferent methods for de®ning frequency bands and band-
`widths, ERD is calculated for both groups of subjects.
`One obvious criterion for considering the proposed
`method superior to FBFW and IBFW is that the two
`groups of subjects with low and high alpha frequency do
`not show signi®cant dierences when frequency bands
`are de®ned according to IBIW. Another criterion is based
`on physiological considerations and refers to that fre-
`quency band which is given by the dierence between IAF
`and TF. Because TF de®nes the transition from the alpha
`to the theta band and because IAF de®nes the transition
`from the lower to the upper alpha band, the frequency
`window given by IAF ) TF equals the bandwidth of the
`lower alpha band. Thus, according to IBIW, the width of
`the lower alpha band IAF ) TF will always be equal to a
`certain percentage of IAF. To illustrate this idea, the
`three dierent types of relationships between IAF and
`TF (corresponding to FBFW, IBFW, and IBIW) are
`plotted in Fig. 1. The dashed horizontal line character-
`izes FBFW and shows that TF remains constant
`regardless of variations in IAF. The dotted line exem-
`pli®es IBFW and illustrates a case where TF always is a
`constant value below IAF (re¯ecting a ®xed bandwidth).
`The bold line illustrates IBIW and shows that ± as
`compared to IBFW ± TF is smaller for large values of
`IAF but larger for small values of IAF. Thus, the crite-
`rion for accepting IBIW as the adequate way to deter-
`mine frequency bands is that the experimentally obtained
`relationship between IAF and TF ®ts the regression line
`predicted for IBIW. If IBIW is superior to FBFW and
`IBFW, and if IAF ) TF is equal to the bandwidth of the
`lower alpha band and proportional to IAF, then a sig-
`ni®cant correlation between IAF ) TF versus IAF
`(including also a signi®cant correlation between TF and
`IAF) will be expected. It should be noted that for both
`methods,
`IBIW and IBFW, a positive correlation
`between TF and IAF is expected. However, for IBFW
`this correlation is solely due to the variations in IAF.
`Thus, because the bandwidth IAF ) TF is a strictly
`constant value for IBFW, only for IBIW is a signi®cant
`correlation between IAF ) TF and IAF predicted.
`
`
`
`51
`
`Fig. 1. Hypothetical regression lines between the individual alpha frequency (IAF) and the transition frequency (TF) of theta synchronization
`and alpha desynchronization for three dierent methods of frequency band de®nitions. While FBFW uses interindividually ®xed frequency bands
`with a bandwidth of 2 Hz for each frequency, for IBFW and IBIW the bands are adjusted to IAF. Whereas IBFW uses a ®xed width of 2 Hz,
`IBIW uses a percentage of 20% of the IAF as the width for the dierent frequency bands. The hypothetical line for FBFW is a horizontal line
`with a equal to mean TF (5.9 Hz in this study). For IBFW the mean dierence between IAF and TF (i.e., 4.6 Hz) was subtracted from each value
`of IAF. The regression line for IBIW was computed by subtracting the mean dierence (IAF ) TF) as a percentage from IAF (i.e., 44%)
`
`In order to avoid confusion it is important to note
`that in Fig. 1 the actual mean values for TF and
`IAF)TF which were found in the present study were
`used for plotting the regression lines. For FBFW the
`regression line lies at 5.9 Hz, which is the actual mean
`value of TF. The regression line for IBFW is based on
`the actual mean value of IAF)TF, which is 4.6 Hz or
`44% and which is used for the regression line for IBIW.
`This was done to allow for a better comparison between
`the actual and predicted data.
`The reason that all of the ERD analyses reported
`below are based on a bandwidth of 4 Hz (or 40%) for
`IAF)TF instead of the actual 4.6 Hz (or 44% respec-
`tively) is that TF varies between studies, depending on
`the speci®c task, and that a value of 4 Hz has proved
`useful in previous research. Thus, proceeding with a
`bandwidth of 4 Hz for IAF)TF allows for a better and
`easier comparison with results from previous studies as
`well as with data from other laboratories.
`
`2 Method
`
`2.1 Subjects
`
`EEG data were recorded from 19 right-handed subjects
`(6 male and 13 female students) who participated
`voluntarily in the experiment. The mean age was 21.6
`years (with a range of 20±31 years). Handedness was
`determined by asking subjects about the hand they use
`in ten dierent tasks such as handwriting, throwing a
`ball, etc. A subject was considered right-handed if he/she
`indicated to use the right hand for all of these dierent
`tasks and if none of the parents was left-handed.
`
`2.2 Stimuli and procedure
`
`A visual oddball paradigm was used in which targets
`and nontargets were presented randomly but with the
`restriction that no more than three targets or nontargets
`may occur in a row. A total of 200 stimuli were
`presented. Targets consisted of a row of ®ve X's
`(XXXXX) and were presented at a probability of 0.30.
`Nontargets consisted of ®ve O's (OOOOO) and were
`presented at a probability of 0.70. Subjects were
`instructed to press a response key with the right hand
`if a target occurs and to silently count the targets but to
`ignore nontargets. At
`the end of
`the experimental
`session, subjects had to report how many targets
`they had counted. The percentage of correct responses
`(pressing the response key) to targets was 99.2%.
`Targets and nontargets appeared for 1 second at the
`center of a computer monitor (see Fig. 2). They were
`0.7 cm in height and 3 cm in length. Subjects were
`placed at a distance of about 90 cm from the monitor in
`a comfortable armchair.
`
`Fig. 2. A single trial comprises a total of 7 s and is composed of a
`reference interval (250±1250 ms), the presentation of a warning signal
`(1875±2125 ms), and an imperative stimulus (3000±4000 ms). The
`intervals t1 and t2 (500 ms duration each) mark the time intervals for
`which ERD was calculated
`
`
`
`52
`
`A brief acoustic warning signal (3000 Hz, lasting for
`250 ms) appeared 1125 ms before a stimulus was pre-
`sented. The length of a single trial was 7 s and the length
`of the whole session was 23 min and 20 s for each sub-
`ject. In an attempt to avoid artifacts, subjects were asked
`to maintain ®xation by looking at the middle of the
`screen and to prevent eyeblinking as soon as the warning
`signal appeared.
`
`2.3 Recording and analysis of EEG data
`
`EEG activity was recorded with a set of 12 silver
`electrodes (F3, F4, C3, Cz, C4, T3, T4, P3, Pz, P4, O1
`and O2) according to the International Electrode (10±
`20) Placement system. For simplicity, only the data for
`Pz were analyzed. Furthermore, the electrooculogram
`(EOG) was recorded from two pairs of leads in order to
`record horizontal and vertical eye movements. All
`electrodes were attached with a Nihon Khoden glue
`paste to the scalp.
`Data were recorded monopolarly against a common
`reference placed on the nose. In order to eliminate the
`eects of the nose reference as well as other types of
`artifacts, the EEG recordings were corrected by sub-
`tracting the arithmetically averaged ear
`recordings
`[(A1 + A2)/2] from all of the monopolar recordings.
`EEG signals were ampli®ed by a 32-channel biosignal
`ampli®er system (frequency response: 0.16 to 30 Hz),
`subjected to an anti-aliasing ®lterbank (cut-o frequen-
`cy: 30 Hz, 110 dB/octave) and were then converted to a
`digital format via a 32-channel A/D converter. Sampling
`rate was 128 Hz. During data acquisition, EEG signals
`were displayed online on a monitor and stored on disk
`for later analyses.
`By visual inspection, all of the epochs were checked
`individually for artifacts (e.g., eye blinks, muscle arti-
`facts). After rejecting artifacts, an average of 35 out of
`the 60 attended stimuli remained for further analyses;
`only targets were analyzed.
`
`2.4 Calculation of ERD
`
`ERD represents the percentage of a change in band
`power during a test interval with respect to a reference
`within a de®ned frequency band. When calculating ERD,
`in a ®rst step the EEG data for each epoch and each
`channel are digitally band-pass ®ltered (in speci®c bands
`and with either ®xed or varied bandwidths as described
`below in Sect. 2.5), squared (in order to obtain simple
`power estimates) and averaged separately for each
`experimental condition and for each subject. Based on
`these data, the percentage of event-related changes in
`band power are calculated in using the ERD procedure
`proposed by Pfurtscheller and Aranibar (1977), who
`have coined the term event-related desynchronization or
`ERD. Thus, ERD is de®ned as the percentage of decrease
`or increase in band power during a speci®c interval as
`compared to a reference interval: ERD% {[(band
`power, reference interval) ) (band power, test interval)]/
`
`(band power, reference interval)} ´ 100. For a more
`detailed description see, for example, Pfurtscheller and
`Klimesch (1992). It is important to note that positive
`values indicate a power suppression, while negative ERD
`values (also termed ERS for event-related synchroniza-
`tion, cf. Pfurtscheller 1992; Pfurtscheller et al. 1996)
`re¯ect an increase in power.
`In the present study, an interval of 2750 to 1750 ms
`before the onset of the target was used as reference. Test
`intervals are the time periods of 500 ms preceding and
`following onset of the imperative stimulus. It is a well-
`established ®nding that during a variety of dierent
`tasks changes in band power can be observed with res-
`pect to a `baseline' which usually is an interval preceding
`the onset of a task or stimulus by a few seconds (e.g., the
`reviews in Pfurtscheller 1992; Pfurtscheller and Klimesch
`1992; Klimesch 1996).
`
`2.5 The determination of frequency bands
`
`Three dierent methods for determining the frequency
`windows were applied and according to this method,
`frequency windows for each of the delta, theta, lower-1-
`alpha, lower-2-alpha and upper-alpha bands were spec-
`i®ed. Table 1 shows the mean frequency windows
`averaged over the entire sample of 19 subjects as well
`as the values for the subject with the highest and the
`lowest IAF.
`
`2.5.1 Determination of FBFW
`
`The frequency windows had a standard bandwidth of
`2 Hz and were the same for all of the subjects. The delta
`band ranged from 2 to 4 Hz, the theta band from 4 to
`6 Hz, the lower-1-alpha from 6 to 8 Hz, the lower-2-
`alpha from 8 to 10 Hz, and the upper-alpha band from
`10 to 12 Hz.
`
`2.5.2 Determination of IBFW
`
`For each subject the peak frequency of the dominant
`EEG frequency in the alpha band for all recording sites
`was used as an anchor point. This mean IAF was
`calculated over the entire epoch of 7 s. Again, ®ve
`dierent frequency bands with a bandwidth of 2 Hz
`each were de®ned by using IAF as the individual anchor
`point: (IAF-8) to (IAF-6), (IAF-6) to (IAF-4), (IAF-4)
`to (IAF-2), (IAF-2) to IAF, and IAF to (IAF+2),
`termed delta, theta, lower-1-alpha, lower-2-alpha, and
`upper-alpha, respectively. Averaged over the entire
`sample of subjects, IAF was 10.6 Hz (see Table 1).
`
`2.5.3 Determination of IBIW of EEG frequencies
`
`As for IBFW, IAF was used as the cut-o frequency
`between the lower and upper alpha bands. However, the
`bandwidth was calculated as a percentage of the IAF:
`
`
`
`53
`
`Table 1 Frequency bands for three dierent methods of de®nition. The respective frequency values and the transition frequencies (TFs) are
`presented for the subject with the highest and the lowest individual alpha frequency (IAF ) as well as for the mean of the whole 19 subject
`sample
`
`IAF
`
`Delta
`
`Theta
`
`Lower-1-alpha
`
`Lower-2-alpha Upper-alpha
`
`TF
`
`From To
`
`From To
`
`From To
`
`From To
`
`From To
`
`FBFW lowest IAF
`highest IAF
`IAF of 19 subs.
`
`IBFW lowest IAF
`highest IAF
`IAF of 19 subs.
`
`IBIW lowest IAF
`highest IAF
`IAF of 19 subs.
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`2.0
`2.0
`2.00
`
`1.0
`4.5
`2.57
`
`1.8
`2.5
`2.11
`
`4.0
`4.0
`4.00
`
`3.0
`6.5
`4.57
`
`3.6
`5.0
`4.23
`
`4.0
`4.0
`4.00
`
`3.0
`6.5
`4.57
`
`3.6
`5.0
`4.23
`
`6.0
`6.0
`6.00
`
`5.0
`8.5
`6.57
`
`5.4
`7.5
`6.34
`
`6.0
`6.0
`6.00
`
`5.0
`8.5
`6.57
`
`5.4
`7.5
`6.34
`
`8.0
`8.0
`8.00
`
`7.0
`10.5
`8.57
`
`7.2
`10.0
`8.46
`
`8.0
`8.0
`8.00
`
`7.0
`10.5
`8.57
`
`7.2
`10.0
`8.46
`
`10.0
`10.0
`10.00
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`10.0
`10.0
`10.00
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`9.0
`12.5
`10.57
`
`12.0
`12.0
`12.00
`
`11.0
`14.5
`12.57
`
`10.8
`15.0
`12.68
`
`4.3
`6.5
`5.93
`
`4.3
`6.5
`5.93
`
`4.3
`6.5
`5.93
`
`(IAF ´ 0.2) to (IAF ´ 0.4), (IAF ´ 0.4) to (IAF ´ 0.6),
`(IAF ´ 0.6) to (IAF ´ 0.8), (IAF ´ 0.8) to IAF, and
`IAF to (IAF ´ 1.2).
`
`2.6 Determination of TF between synchronization
`and desynchronization
`
`As suggested by Klimesch et al. (1996a), each individual
`TF was determined by comparing the power spectra for
`the reference and test intervals. First, power spectra for
`the reference and test intervals were calculated for each
`subject and averaged over all trials. Then, the frequency
`of the transition region between the theta and alpha
`bands was determined within a ®xed frequency window
`of 4±10 Hz. This was done by determining that frequen-
`cy for each subject where the lines of the two power
`spectra intersected. This point was used as the indicator
`for the TF between theta and alpha. The idea behind this
`simple procedure is that during the test interval and in
`comparison the reference interval alpha power decreases
`whereas the theta power increases. Table 1 indicates the
`mean TF as well as the TF values for the subject with the
`highest and the lowest IAF.
`
`2.7 Statistical analyses
`
`First, a correlation was calculated between IAF and TF;
`it should be noted that a positive coecient simply
`would show that TF increases with increasing IAF.
`Thus, in a second step, the dierence between TF and
`IAF (representing the bandwidth of the two lower alpha
`bands) was correlated with the IAF.
`Three dierent predictions can be distinguished:
`
`(1) If TF is a constant value, TF and IAF would be not
`signi®cantly correlated; in this case, FBFW can be
`assumed.
`(2) If TF is signi®cantly correlated with IAF (cf. Fig. 1),
`IBFW or IBIW can be assumed.
`(3) In order to distinguish between the latter two cases
`the bandwidth IAF)TF is correlated with IAF. Only
`if this correlation is positive can IBIW be assumed.
`
`In order to compare the results of the three dierent
`methods for determining frequency bands, subjects with
`a high versus low IAF were distinguished. Group IAF)
`comprises 10 subjects (6 female, 4 male) falling below the
`median; group IAF+, on the other hand, included 9
`subjects (7 female, 2 male) with an IAF above the
`median. For each of the ®ve frequency bands (delta,
`theta, lower-1-alpha, lower-2-alpha and upper-alpha) a
`three-factorial ANOVA with ScheeÂ
`tests on the 5%
`level for pairwise comparisons of means was performed.
`The factors and their levels are Method (FBFW, IBFW,
`IBIW), Time (t1, t2;
`i.e., two 500 ms intervals, one
`preceding, the other following the onset of a target) as a
`repeated measure, and Group (IAF+, IAF)) as a
`between-subjects factor. The levels for factor TIME are
`the two 500 ms epochs preceding and following the
`onset of the imperative stimulus. These intervals were
`selected because t1 (prestimulus) represents a time
`interval with high attention so that a strong desynchro-
`nization in the lower-1-alpha and the lower-2-alpha can
`be expected. On the other hand,
`t2 (poststimulus)
`represents an interval with a simple cognitive demand
`which is characterized by a synchronization in the theta
`and delta band (Klimesch et al. 1997b). Because these
`ANOVAs were performed to test the hypothesis whether
`subjects with high and low IAF respond in a similar
`way, the interaction Group ´ Method is of particular
`interest.
`
`3 Results
`
`3.1 Correlations
`
`The correlation between IAF and TF shows a signi®-
`cant positive coecient (r 0.467, P < 0.05),
`indi-
`cating that a high IAF occurs together with a high TF.
`On the basis of
`this correlation, FBFW can be
`excluded. The highly signi®cant and positive correla-
`tion (r 0.580, P < 0.01) between IAF and the
`bandwidth (IAF)TF) rules out IBFW and supports
`IBIW. The respective regression lines are depicted in
`Fig. 3a and b.
`
`
`
`54
`
`Fig. 3. a Regression lines and the respective
`correlations between TF and IAF and b
`between the dierence (IAF ) TF) and IAF a
`shows the signi®cant positive correlation be-
`tween TF and IAF. Most importantly, the
`highest correlation was obtained between the
`dierence (IAF ) TF) and IAF as b reveals.
`This supports the hypothesis that the frequency
`bandwidths also vary as a function of IAF, as
`explained in the Introduction
`
`3.2 ANOVAs
`
`For the delta band, only factor Time reached signi®-
`cance at the 1% level (F 60.08, P < 0.01), showing a
`strong synchronization during t2.
`For the theta band, signi®cant eects were found for
`Method (F 13.86, P < 0.01), Time (F 18.11,
`P < 0.01), Group ´ Method (F 16.62, P < 0.01),
`and Group ´ Method ´ Time (F 7.45, P < 0.01).
`With respect to factor Method, the respective means
`indicate that synchronization is strongest for FBFW and
`weakest for IBFW. The means for factor Time reveal
`that, in contrast to t1, a pronounced synchronization
`was found during t2. For the means of interaction
`Group ´ Method, Schee tests for pairwise comparisons
`yield a critical dierence of 10.65 ERD%. Inspection of
`Fig. 4a shows that signi®cant dierences at the 5% level
`between IAF+ and IAF) (marked with asterisks) were
`found for FBFW during t2 and for IBFW during t1 and
`t2 but not for IBIW.
`The results for the lower-1-alpha bands show signi-
`®cant eects for Method (F 8.39, P < 0.01) and for
`Group ´ Method (F 8.44, P < 0.01). Here a mod-
`erate amount of desynchronization during t1 and t2
`was found. The ®ndings for factor Method reveal that
`the
`strongest desynchronization was obtained for
`
`IBFW and the smallest for FBFW. The Schee test for
`testing signi®cance of dierences on the 5% level be-
`tween the means of interaction Group ´ Method re-
`veals a critical value of 12.55 ERD%. As depicted in
`Fig. 4b,
`signi®cant dierences between IAF+ and
`IAF) can only be observed for FBFW during t2 and
`for IBFW during t1. Again no such eects can be ob-
`served for IBIW.
`In the lower-2-alpha band, signi®cant results were
`obtained for factor Time (F 15.09, P < 0.01) and the
`interaction Method ´ Time (F 6.43, P < 0.01). The
`respective means for TIME show a strong desynchro-
`nization that increases from t1 to t2. As Fig. 4c reveals,
`the signi®cant interaction is largely due to the higher
`level of ERD for group IAF) during t2 which was found
`for FBFW.
`In the upper-alpha band the only signi®cant eect
`was found for Time (F 16.73, P < 0.01), which
`shows an increase in desynchronization from t1 to t2.
`
`4 Discussion
`
`The results show that only for the proposed method
`(IBIW) were no signi®cant dierences obtained between
`the two groups of high and low alpha frequency (IAF+
`
`
`
`55
`
`Fig. 4. The means of interaction Group ´ Time ´ Method are shown for the theta, lower-1-alpha, and lower-2-alpha bands. Factor Group
`comprises IAF+ and IAF) (n for IAF+ 9, n for IAF) 10), factor Time includes the two time intervals t1 and t2 (see Fig. 2), and Method
`comprises IBIW, IBFW, and FBFW. Signi®cant dierences as determined by Schee tests on the 5% level between group IAF+ and IAF) are
`marked with asterisks. In the theta and lower-1-alpha bands, signi®cant dierences between IAF+ and IAF) were observed only for FBFW and
`IBFW. Because no signi®cant dierences were found for IBIW, the assumption is supported that IBIW is superior to FBFW and IBFW
`
`and IAF)). In sharp contrast to IBIW, large dierences
`between these two groups were particularly obtained
`for the conventional method that uses FBFW, as Fig. 4
`indicates. These ®ndings demonstrate that interindivid-
`ual dierences in alpha frequency and bandwidth can
`be adequately treated when applying the proposed
`method.
`The superiority of the proposed method can be do-
`cumented by referring to the following two examples.
`First,
`let us consider the ®nding that the traditional
`method, FBFW, shows no desynchronization for IAF+
`during t2 in the lower-1-alpha band (cf. Fig. 4b). In
`earlier experiments we have found consistently that,
`particularly in the lower alpha band, the presentation of
`a visual stimulus leads to a pronounced desynchroniza-
`tion with a very early onset that even precedes the pre-
`sentation of a stimulus and most
`likely re¯ects
`expectancy (e.g., Klimesch et al. 1992). This lack of
`desynchronization which was found for FBFW can
`easily be explained by considering the fact that group
`IAF+ comprises subjects with comparatively high alpha
`frequency. Thus, the ®xed lower-1-alpha band of group
`IAF+ already falls to a considerable extent below the
`lower boundary of the individual lower-1-alpha band.
`Consequently, either no or only a rudimentary lower
`alpha desynchronization can be detected. Another ex-
`ample well in line with this interpretation shows up in
`the theta band, where FBFW yields the largest amount
`of synchronization (cf. Fig. 4a). In a similar way as for
`the lower-1-alpha band, the ®xed theta band of group
`IAF+ already falls in part outside the theta frequency
`range if individual bands with ®xed widths are used.
`This ®nding is illustrated by Fig. 5, which shows that
`
`people with high IAF also have higher theta frequency
`and if ®xed bands are not adjusted individually, theta
`synchronization already is confused with the much
`larger delta synchronization.
`The transition frequency TF marks the upper fre-
`quency limit of the theta band. Thus, the positive cor-
`relation between IAF and TF clearly indicates that the
`theta band also varies interindividually and that about
`22% of the variance of theta frequency can be ex-
`plained by variations in alpha frequency. In addition,
`the correlation between IAF and the dierence between
`TF and IAF demonstrates a close covariation between
`the bandwidth (i.e., the dierence IAF)TF) and alpha
`frequency (IAF), which explains about 34% of the
`variance. Based on the results of the correlation anaysis
`and the predictions discussed in Sect. 2.7, IBIW (which
`is superior to FBFW and IBFW) can be accepted.
`Comparing the predicted relationship between IAF and
`TF with the actual data in Fig. 6 reveals that IBIW
`gives the best estimates. The regression line for the
`actual data is highly similar with the predicted regres-
`sion line for the proposed method but quite dierent
`from the predicted regression lines for FBFW and
`IBFW.
`Taken together, the reported results argue for the
`use of
`individually adjusted frequency bands when
`event-related band power measures of dierent types
`are used (Petsche and Rappelsberger 1992; Dujardin et
`al. 1995; Neubauer et al. 1995; Krause et al. 1996;
`Weiss and Rappelsberger 1996). It also appears likely
`that the individual determination of EEG frequencies
`may even prove useful for the analysis of ERPs (Basar
`et al. 1992; Basar-Eroglu et al. 1992; Basar and
`
`
`
`56
`
`Fig. 5. Hypothetical power spectra for a subject with a comparably high IAF of 12.5 Hz. The three dierent methods (FBFW, IBFW, and IBIW
`as discussed in the text) yield dierent frequency bands, as shown in the lower part of the ®gure. Note that only IBIW covers the entire spectrum.
`The bold line (representing the reference interval) and the dashed line [representing period t2 (see Fig. 2)] intersect at TF, which marks a transition
`between theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization
`
`SchuÈ rmann 1994). Basar and his research group have
`provided convincing evidence that event-related po-
`tentials (ERPs) under certain conditions are composed
`at least in part of phase-locked alpha activity (e.g.,
`Basar and SchuÈ rmann 1994; SchuÈ rmann and Basar
`1994). According to Basar (1972), phase-locked EEG
`activity as re¯ected by certain ERP components can be
`considered a resonance phenomenon that plays an
`important role in biological cybernetics. For future
`
`research in the increasingly important ®eld of alpha
`oscillations (Basar e

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.
After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.
Accept $ ChargeStill Working On It
This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.
Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.
A few More Minutes ... Still Working
It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.
Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.
We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.
You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.
Set your membership
status to view this document.
With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll
get a whole lot more, including:
- Up-to-date information for this case.
- Email alerts whenever there is an update.
- Full text search for other cases.
- Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

One Moment Please
The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.
Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!
If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document
We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.
If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.
Access Government Site