throbber
Gu, 2001;48:125-13 1
`
`125
`
`Occasional viewpoint
`
`Visceral hypersensitivity: facts., speculations., and challenges
`
`Visceral hypersensitivity is currently the holy grail ... .it is
`widely regarded as the reason for the development of func(cid:173)
`tional gastrointestinal diseases, including functional dys(cid:173)
`pepsia and irritable bowel syndrome.' Although the field
`has advanced considerably in the past decade, it is
`necessary to take stock and realistically appraise our
`current understanding. Identifying issues of controversy
`will help address the directions or priorities to advance this
`field in the future. Thus our objectives are to review the
`current understanding of the neuroanatomy and physiol(cid:173)
`ogy of gut sensation, briefly review examples of the
`relationship between symptoms and sensorimotor dysfunc(cid:173)
`tion, and discuss controversies and speculations requiring
`more thorough study. In pursuing this discussion, we have
`drawn on experience and data from several regions of the
`gut as work in a single region does not provide the compre(cid:173)
`hensive or broad perspectives necessary.
`
`Neuroanatomy and physiology of sensation
`Enteroendocrine cells in the lining of the gut serve as
`chemical and mechanical transducers for local reflexes (for
`example, peristalsis) or initiation of afferent projections to
`the central nervous system.' 2 As with somatic sensation,
`gut afferent signals reach conscious perception through a
`three neurone chain.2 The first order neurone, whose cell
`body is in the dorsal root ganglion, terminates in the dorsal
`column laminae of the spinal cord (fig 1). Enpassantfibres
`project to noradrenergic neurones in prevertebral ganglia,
`and this reflex centre results in modulation of viscus func(cid:173)
`tions, including motility. Somatic and visceral afferents
`converge on dorsal horn neurones and result in viscero(cid:173)
`somatic projection or referred pain. Descending modula(cid:173)
`tory fibres (serotonergic, adrenergic, and possibly others)
`from brain stem centres such as the periaqueductal grey
`alter the sensitivity of the dorsal horn neurones and thus
`serve to centrally control the intensity of perception during
`visceral stimulation (fig I).'
`The second order neurone projects from the dorsal horn
`of the spinal cord to the thalamus and reticular formation in
`the brain stem (fig 1). The ascending pathways are located
`in the spinoreticular and spinothalamic tracts. Recently, a
`nociceptive spinal pathway was identified in the dorsal col(cid:173)
`umn in primates; these project nociception from viscera
`such as the colorectum, pancreas, and duodenum.' •
`These second neurones synapse with autonomic and
`satiety centres and with the third order neurone that leads
`to emotional responses (limbic system) and conscious per(cid:173)
`ception (sensory cortex). These projections lead to changes
`in pulse rate, blood pressure, appetite, and emotions in
`response to visceral pain. The loci of projection in the sen(cid:173)
`sory cortex are still not fully understood; there is evidence
`that the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, and cerebellum
`are activated during distensions of the oesophagus,
`stomach, and rectum.
`Currently, we have a limited understanding of the cerebral
`processing of visceral stimuli, the pathways and mediators of
`visceral afferents, the role of end organ modulation of sensa(cid:173)
`tion, and association of symptoms with some sensorimotor
`dysfunctions. These will be reviewed below but it is essential
`to acknowledge the many gaps in our understanding. These
`gaps are particularly evident in comparison with the
`
`Brain
`
`Sensation
`
`Ascending
`spinothalamic
`and spinoreticular
`fibres
`
`. .
`
`GI tract
`
`Desending adrenergic and
`serotonergic fibres
`Figure 1 Three order ,ieurone chain resulting in visceml perception; ,zore
`the ,lesce,u/i,zg path~s converging on the dorsal ham ,ieurone modulating
`the proj,cr:io,,., from thts relay station to the surfaa atul to the bra111.
`
`thorough characterisation of transmitters/mediators'... in(cid:173)
`volved in the reflex responses at the prevertebral ganglia. The
`candidate transmitters in visceral perception include sero(cid:173)
`tonin (5-HT), calcitonin gene related peptide, substance P,
`norepinephrine, and opiates (peripherally at kappa recep(cid:173)
`tors, centrally at mu receptors).'• '0 There are receptor sub(cid:173)
`types for all of these transmitters and advances in therapy
`will require full characterisation of the roles of transmitters
`and receptors along the sensory neural axis.
`
`Definitions of visceral sensation parameters
`Prior to discussing the pros and cons of measurements and
`the relationship between physiological parameters and
`symptoms, a few definitions are essential.
`(1) Accommodation is relaxation of the stomach in the early
`postprandial period. Using the barostat, wall tone has
`been assessed by the relative change (that is, not an
`absolute measurement) of stomach volume under
`constant pressure. However, the volume of accommoda(cid:173)
`tion can also be measured with novel imaging methods
`(for example, magnetic resonance imaging or single
`photon emission computed tomography, discussed
`below).
`
`Abbreviations used in this paper : ms, irritable bowel syndrome.
`
`www.gt11j11l.co111
`
`Bausch Health Ireland Exhibit 2053, Page 1 of 7
`Mylan v. Bausch Health Ireland - IPR2022-00722
`
`

`

`126
`
`Camilleri, Coulie, Tack
`
`(2) Compliance is the volume response (y axis) to an
`imposed pressure (x axis) and it has a sigmoid
`relationship with initial reflex relaxation (change in
`wall tension without volume change), followed by a
`linear section that reflects partly the elasticity of the
`viscus wall, and a final plateau phase (fig 2).
`Compliance is inversely related to elastance. Wall
`compliance is measured by means of isobaric stepwise
`(ramp) distension (for example, with a barostat). The
`nature of the balloon (latex v polyethylene) and the
`method of inflation influence the results.11–13
`(3) Wall
`tension has been calculated using the law of
`Laplace (T»P.r), derived with a slight di Verence in the
`formula for the actual shape (for example, cylinder,
`sphere) of the balloon in the viscus segment tested. This
`measurement assumes
`several
`factors which are
`unknown in most human experiments (discussed
`below under “Stretch versus tension modulation of
`aVerent discharges”). It was claimed that measure-
`ment of tension would be more relevant to understand
`sensory changes14 but correlation of sensation in
`response to tension based stimuli may not be
`significantly better than with pressure based stimuli.
`(4) Hypersensitivity, hyperalgesia, allodynia. Hypersensitivity
`refers to increased sensation of stimuli. In practice, this
`is appraised by measurement of threshold volumes or
`pressures for first sensation or pain. Alternatively, it
`refers to the increased scores of symptoms (including
`pain) in response to standard stimuli. Hyperalgesia
`refers to increased pain sensation in response to a cer-
`tain stimulus. Allodynia refers to the appreciation that
`a stimulus, which was previously not perceived as
`being painful, becomes painful. These terms, which
`were originally used in reference to somatic sensation,
`have been adapted to studies of visceral sensation.
`
`Stretch versus tension modulation of aVerent
`discharges
`Although the reasons for hypersensitivity to distension are
`unclear, mechanoreceptors must be activated to initiate,
`convey, or perceive the distending stimulus. It has been
`proposed that mechanoreceptors are either in series or in
`parallel with muscle fibres.
`In animals,
`in parallel
`mechanoreceptors respond to stimuli that elongate the
`stomach wall;
`in series mechanoreceptors respond to
`stimuli
`that
`increase the tension within the stomach
`wall.15 16 Figure 3 illustrates the responses of tension mech-
`anoreceptors (in series) and elongation mechanoreceptors
`(in parallel) to diVerent stimuli (distension, relaxation, and
`contraction of smooth muscle). In series receptors are acti-
`vated during distension and contraction against a resist-
`ance; conversely, in series receptors are inactivated during
`
`250
`
`200
`
`150
`
`100
`
`50
`
`Volume (ml)
`
`0
`
`0
`
`10
`Pressure (mm Hg)
`Figure 2 Example of a compliance curve (volume response to imposed
`pressure) in a segment of human colon. The curve is obtained by stepwise
`increments of intraballoon pressure and simultaneous measurement of
`intraballoon pressure. Note the initial “cushion” in which an increase in
`pressure does not result in any change in volume. The second portion of the
`compliance curve is more linear and partly reflects the elasticity of the viscus.
`
`20
`
`In series
`
`In parallel
`
`Activated
`
`Activated
`
`Inactivated
`
`Activated
`
`Distension
`
`Relaxation
`
`Contraction
`
`Activated
`Inactivated
`Figure 3 Schematic illustration of responses of tension mechanoreceptors
`(in series) and elongation mechanoreceptors (in parallel) to distension,
`relaxation, and contraction of gastric smooth muscle. Mechanoreceptors are
`modelled as coils and positioned in parallel (right) or in series (left) to the
`muscle. In series, receptors are activated during distension and contraction
`against a resistance; they become inactivated during relaxation. In parallel,
`receptors are activated during distension and relaxation, and become
`inactivated during contraction.
`
`receptors are activated during
`relaxation. In parallel
`distension and relaxation and are inactivated during
`contraction.
`In human studies, gastric distension results in elongation
`as well as increased wall tension of the stomach; the stom-
`ach also reflexly responds to the stimulus applied by
`contracting. Hence it is unclear which of these two types of
`mechanoreceptors mediates sensitivity to distension of the
`proximal stomach in vivo. Observations made in recent
`studies provide arguments for involvement of both types of
`receptors. For example, during postprandial gastric
`accommodation, an increase in proximal gastric volume
`(and hence elongation) occurs but this does not cause
`food
`enhanced perception17 18 until a large volume of
`stimulates a sense of satiety and fullness. The latter is
`probably mediated by a change in wall tension from the
`pressures imposed by food and reflex contractions. In
`humans, one way to assess the roles of volume versus ten-
`sion in gastric sensation is to evaluate the eVects of high
`volume clamping by fixed volume inflation of a balloon
`while pressure is allowed to vary. This allows measurement
`of muscle tension while changes in muscle length are
`“neutralised” by the high volume clamp. In this experimen-
`tal setting, phasic contractions which are associated with
`phasic increases in wall tension cause enhanced percep-
`tion, probably through in series receptors which are under
`increased “load” with viscus contraction.19
`Thumshirn and colleagues20 demonstrated that wall ten-
`sion, estimated by Laplace’s law, was significantly corre-
`lated with sensation scores (r=0.4, p<0.05) during a phar-
`macological study of gastric sensitivity, suggesting that
`>80% of the sensation variance (1−r2) is attributable to
`other factors, such as visceral aVerent functions or central
`control (spinal or supratentorial). To assess the relative
`contribution of both receptor types in mediating sensation
`in vivo, novel approaches and more sensitive methodology
`are needed. Distrutti and colleagues14 suggested that a
`“tensostat” may facilitate this task but the relationships (r
`values) between symptoms and tension or pressure in their
`work diVered minimally. An alternative method, which has
`been used to measure rectal wall tension, is impedance
`planimetry; increased wall tension was associated with
`greater sensation of the need to defecate. However, similar
`
`www.gutjnl.com
`
`Bausch Health Ireland Exhibit 2053, Page 2 of 7
`Mylan v. Bausch Health Ireland - IPR2022-00722
`
`

`

`Visceral hypersensitivity
`
`127
`
`increases in sensation were observed with increases in dis-
`tension pressure.21
`It has been suggested that the simplified law of Laplace
`can be used to estimate wall tension during barostat stud-
`ies of hollow organs, and that this level of wall tension
`determines the level of perception during distension stud-
`ies.14 However, this formula makes a number of assump-
`tions that are not necessarily fulfilled by the extant experi-
`mental conditions. Those assumptions include: the wall of
`the viscus is infinitely thin; the intraluminal balloon and
`viscus have a perfectly defined (for example, spherical)
`shape that can be defined mathematically; the pressure
`external to the viscus is known and is evenly distributed.
`Most importantly, the Laplace formulae do not take into
`account the modulatory eVects of changes in the contrac-
`tile state of the viscus which may occur reflexly or in
`response to neurohumoral or pharmacological modulation
`and which are superimposed on the compliance of the hol-
`low organ. The limitations in current assessments of
`tension with tensostat or barostat were recently discussed
`by Gregersen22 who emphasised the error in the assump-
`tion that tension is equal throughout the proximal stomach
`(that is, isotropic), particularly in view of the complex and
`variable geometry of the stomach within and between indi-
`viduals. These diVerences in geometry render invalid a
`theoretical attribution of shape (for example, spherical v
`ellipsoid v more complex).
`A strict application of Laplace’s law to isobaric measure-
`ments also appears to lead to conclusions that conflict with
`daily experiences. For example, relaxation (or elongation of
`the circumference) of the proximal stomach under isobaric
`conditions would predict increased wall tension as T~r
`when P is constant. Thus if tension was the determining
`factor in sensation, Laplace’s law would predict increased
`sensation at times when the viscus is relaxed, for example
`during the 3–5-fold increase in gastric volume that occurs
`within five minutes of starting a meal. Conversely, under
`conditions of adequate accommodation and of pharmaco-
`logical relaxation, the experimental evidence suggests that
`symptoms elicited by mechanical stimuli are usually
`reduced in healthy subjects,17 20 unless massive relaxation is
`induced.
`
`Measurement of visceral sensations
`In general, the literature in humans shows two types of vis-
`ceral sensation measurements in vivo (fig 4). The first
`addresses the sensation of mechanical, electrical, or other
`stimuli applied within the gut, and uses standardised
`symptom based questionnaires (visual analogue scale or
`adjectival scale) to determine thresholds or severity of
`symptoms induced. The second method measures changes
`in cerebral blood flow using positron emission tomography,
`
`Visceral sensation: methods for measurement
`
`Function
`
`Method
`
`Sensation of mechanical stimuli
`
`Threshold
`
`AML, tracking,
`random staircase
`
`VAS
`
`Phasic distensions
`
`Cerebral blood flow
`
`PET, fMRI, SPECT
`
`Figure 4 Schema demonstrating distension paradigms used in visceral
`sensation studies. PET, positron emission tomography; fMRI, functional
`magnetic resonance imaging; SPECT, single photon emission computed
`tomography; VAS, visual analogue scale; AML, ascending method of
`limits.
`
`functional magnetic resonance imaging, or single photon
`emission computed tomography during visceral stimula-
`tion.
`
`METHODS OF MEASURING SENSORY THRESHOLDS
`Methods of measuring the threshold for initial perception
`or discomfort/pain have generally used ascending method
`of limits, tracking, or a random staircase design.12–14 In
`these methods, sequentially increased pressure or volume
`distensions are delivered until the subject perceives either
`first sensation or the symptom of discomfort/pain. After the
`threshold is identified, a computer method randomly
`delivers a pressure or volume stimulus which is either
`above or below the previously identified threshold. This
`serves to fine tune the level of the threshold for either vol-
`ume or pressure distensions. In the random staircase
`method,
`the stimulus paradigm does not necessarily
`increase continually as in ascending method of limits but
`randomly applies the stimulus either of greater or lower
`intensity to try to avoid response bias.23 While these meth-
`ods have received a great deal of application in the
`literature, their sensitivity and potential for response bias
`have not been adequately assessed. For example, when
`ascending method of limits and tracking are used to assess
`first perception and pain, it is possible that the subject will
`be interrogated 40 or 50 times while assessing the thresh-
`olds. This clearly could introduce an element of response
`bias which is probably worse with increasing number of
`distensions.
`
`DISTENSIONS USING PRESSURE BASED MECHANICAL STIMULI
`To avoid the potential inaccuracies introduced by response
`bias, we have used a restricted number (three to five) of
`distensions using pressure based mechanical stimuli which
`are applied in a randomised order.20 24–26 During disten-
`sions, the individual is asked to complete a visual analogue
`scale pertaining to the symptoms that are of interest (for
`example, pain and gas in the colon; pain and urgency in the
`rectum; or bloating, nausea, and pain in the stomach).
`
`CEREBRAL BLOOD FLOW MEASUREMENTS
`Cerebral blood flow measurements are intended to identify
`the projections in the brain of visceral stimuli applied in the
`gut. These will be discussed in greater depth below. How-
`ever, it is important to realise that changes in cerebral
`blood flow detected by these methods range from 2% to
`5%. Sensitivity to detect increases in cerebral blood flow
`over background activity, with variations unrelated to the
`specific stimulus, is somewhat vulnerable because of the
`relatively low absolute changes in blood flow that can be
`expected.27–29 Thus there is a low signal to noise ratio which
`renders interpretation diYcult.
`
`Symptoms and disturbed sensorimotor functions:
`any role as a biological marker?
`Irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) is associated with rectosig-
`moid hypersensitivity30 31 which has been postulated to
`represent a biological marker for IBS.32 Several lines of evi-
`dence lead us to question this suggestion. Firstly, across
`studies, the prevalence of rectal hypersensitivity in IBS is
`20–80% and may only be demonstrable in response to
`repetitive stimuli rather than single stimuli. Secondly, there
`was only a weak correlation between rectal sensory thresh-
`olds and current pain, and no significant correlation with
`pain severity in the prior two weeks.33 Thirdly, changes in
`rectal sensory thresholds did not predict response to
`therapy in one study reported to date.34 Fourthly, demon-
`stration of rectal hypersensitivity has not yet contributed to
`the diagnosis or alteration of pharmacotherapy in IBS.
`Fifthly,
`the increased sensation has been shown to
`
`www.gutjnl.com
`
`Bausch Health Ireland Exhibit 2053, Page 3 of 7
`Mylan v. Bausch Health Ireland - IPR2022-00722
`
`

`

`128
`
`Camilleri, Coulie, Tack
`
`coincide, in some studies, with the timing of reflex rectal
`contractions, suggesting that there is also a motor compo-
`nent to the augmented sensitivity.31 Hence it should to be
`emphasised that application of rectal sensation tests in the
`diagnosis or management of IBS is premature. The meth-
`odology for sensory testing is not yet standardised (for
`example, ascending method of limits, tracking, random
`staircase) or completely validated (for example, coeYcient
`of variation, test-retest reliability, sensitivity, specificity).
`Several studies have confirmed that, as a group, patients
`with functional dyspepsia are hypersensitive to isobaric and
`isovolumetric balloon distension of the proximal stom-
`ach,17 35 that is, the thresholds for first perception and dis-
`comfort are lower
`in dyspeptics
`than in controls.
`Hypersensitivity to gastric distension is a feature of
`functional and not organic dyspepsia.36 It is not known if
`gastric sensory thresholds are correlated with current or
`recent symptom severity or if hypersensitivity is associated
`with specific symptoms in functional dyspepsia patients. A
`preliminary study reported that almost half of functional
`dyspepsia patients have hypersensitivity to gastric disten-
`sion, and that postprandial pain is significantly more
`prevalent in these patients.37 However, one can question
`the relevance of fasting perception thresholds as a biologi-
`cal marker for functional dyspepsia, which is by definition
`a symptom complex that occurs in the postprandial period.
`Patients who report pain during fasting gastric distensions
`are potentially more prone to report postprandial pain;
`pressure induced increased sensation during the postpran-
`dial period may be a better marker of
`functional
`dyspepsia.38 39
`Several authors have shown that functional or non-ulcer
`dyspepsia patients have reduced postcibal gastric accom-
`modation compared with controls.18 38 Among 40 consecu-
`tive non-ulcer dyspeptics who underwent measurement of
`gastric accommodation, Tack and colleagues18 demon-
`strated that early satiety and weight loss were significantly
`more frequent in patients with impaired versus normal
`accommodation. As fasting gastric compliance is normal,
`these data suggest heightened sensitivity of the sensory
`apparatus from gut to cortex, with presumed modulation
`of hypothalamic or other satiety centres.
`
`Controversial interpretation of visceral sensation
`data
`In the next section we will address a number of controver-
`sial issues pertaining to visceral sensation. Such controver-
`sial statements and interpretations include the following:
`(1) If a drug alters perception without a change in compli-
`ance, the drug must aVect visceral aVerent function.
`(2) Wall tension determines the level of sensation in a vis-
`cus.
`(3) Relaxation results in reduced perception.
`
`COMPLIANCE AND PERCEPTION
`The interpretation of perception data in response to phar-
`macological perturbations has incorporated measurements
`of organ compliance. Thus when compliance is unchanged
`but sensation is, it has been implied that aVerent nerve
`function has been altered. This is illustrated by the exam-
`ple of octreotide, which alters sensation without changing
`compliance,40–43 suggesting that its eVect must be on aVer-
`ent nerve function. However, octreotide also reduces post-
`prandial colonic tone, suggesting that it may blunt reflex
`contraction to physiological
`stimuli44; another
`study
`suggested octreotide did not alter either rectal sensation or
`compliance.45
`Compliance data are far more complex than would be
`suggested from the linear interpretation of data that have
`typically been applied to the compliance curves. Thus in
`
`the gut, compliance curves have an initial “cushion” in
`which an increase in pressure does not result in any change
`in volume (see fig 2). This accommodation to a pressure
`stimulus can be altered by a pharmacological perturbation,
`such as with an ♡
`2 adrenergic agonist. This portion of the
`compliance curve can be assessed mathematically by
`estimating the ♢ component on a power exponential analy-
`sis of the compliance curve26 or the inflection point in the
`curve.32 The physiological contribution of tone and tension
`to this part of the compliance curve has not been fully
`evaluated.
`The second portion of the compliance curve is more lin-
`ear and partly reflects the elasticity of the viscus. This may
`explain why compliance is rarely altered by diseases, except
`those associated with replacement fibrosis (scleroderma,
`radiation) or by drugs. Clonidine significantly alters the
`linear portion of the colonic compliance curve,26 suggesting
`that neuromuscular function may partly alter this aspect of
`compliance.
`
`TENSION
`The relationship between tension in the wall and
`sensation14 is also more complex and, as indicated in the
`previous discussion, requires more thorough study. When a
`compliance curve is shifted to the left or right with no
`change in volume but a change in pressure, a simple appli-
`cation of Laplace’s law would suggest that there has been a
`change in wall tension. Such a change may not be identified
`if only the slope of a linear model is applied to the compli-
`ance curve.
`
`ISOVOLUMETRIC STIMULATION: RELAXATION VERSUS
`ANTINOCICEPTION
`Another pitfall in the interpretation of data in the literature
`pertains to observations with isovolumetric stimuli. These
`volume based mechanical stimuli are vulnerable to misin-
`terpretation; thus changes in wall tension or relaxation will
`result in greater volumes required to achieve the threshold
`of sensation. At first glance, this would suggest a true sen-
`sory eVect of the perturbation or drug. However, as is
`shown from a recent example in the literature,46 volume
`based mechanical stimuli are vulnerable because changes
`in compliance or relaxation are associated with greater vol-
`umes to achieve threshold, but no such change in the pres-
`sure thresholds. In a study of IBS patients, Delvaux and
`colleagues46 demonstrated that alosetron 0.25 mg twice
`daily or 4 mg twice daily was associated with increases in
`the perception threshold and pain threshold to volume dis-
`tensions. However, there was no significant diVerence in
`pressure thresholds, suggesting absence of an antinocicep-
`tive action of alosetron. Studies of colonic compliance per-
`formed in the same evaluation provide an explanation for
`the observed diVerences in volume thresholds as alosetron
`significantly altered the compliance of the colon. Other
`data suggest that relaxation alone is unlikely to result in
`reduced sensation. For example, in a study by Tack and
`colleagues47 performed in 18 healthy volunteers who were
`pretreated for five days with cisapride 10 mg four times
`daily or placebo, the accommodation response of the
`stomach was significantly greater with cisapride. However,
`cisapride did not alter the pressure or volume of first per-
`ception and it actually decreased pain thresholds to both
`volume and pressure stimuli relative to placebo in the fast-
`ing state.
`Surveying the literature, there are several other examples
`(table 1)
`in which drugs have been demonstrated
`unequivocally to reduce sensation to volume distension but
`eVects on pressure induced discomfort have either not
`been evaluated or have not been as clearly demonstrated as
`eVects on volume distension. Examples of altered volume
`
`www.gutjnl.com
`
`Bausch Health Ireland Exhibit 2053, Page 4 of 7
`Mylan v. Bausch Health Ireland - IPR2022-00722
`
`

`

`Visceral hypersensitivity
`
`129
`
`Table 1 Pharmacological modulation of visceral tone: documented and
`potential sensory eVects
`
`Agent
`
`Action
`
`Reduced sensation or
`increased pain threshold to
`
`Volume
`distension
`
`Pressure
`distension
`
`Atropine51 52
`CCK51
`5-HT3 antagonists
`OND53 54
`GRA48
`ALO46
`5-HT1 agonists49 55
`
`Octreotide40 45
`
`♡2 Agonist20 26
`Fedotozine50
`Cisapride47
`
`fl Colonic tone
`No eVect on tone
`
`?
`?
`
`+
`+
`+
`+
`
`fl Colonic tone pm
`No change in compliance
`fl Colonic tone,› compliance
`fl Colonic and gastric fasting,
`and pm tone
`fl Colonic pm tone,› phasic
`contractions
`fl Colonic tone,› compliance
`?
`+
`No change in compliance
`fl Gastric tone pm. ?flcompliance −
`
`+
`
`?
`?
`
`?
`?
`?
`?
`
`?/+
`
`+
`+
`−
`
`pm, post meal; CCK, cholecystokinin; OND, ondansetron; GRA, granisetron;
`ALO, alosetron.
`?, unknown; +, reduced sensation; −, increased sensation
`
`thresholds include eVects of ondansetron and granisetron,
`5-HT3 antagonists,48 alosetron (as described above46)
`octreotide,43 sumatriptan,49 and cisapride.47 In contrast,
`there is good evidence that the ♡
`2 adrenergic agonist cloni-
`dine increases compliance of the colon, reduces colonic
`tone, and also markedly reduces pain sensation during
`mechanical distension of the descending colon.26 Similarly,
`the kappa opioid agonist fedotozine50 reduces sensation to
`both volume and pressure stimuli, suggesting it has antino-
`ciceptive activity. The role of cytokines, purines, neuro-
`kinins, and other mediators on sensation and compliance
`requires further study in animals and humans.10
`DiVerences between relaxant and antinociceptive ac-
`tions were demonstrated convincingly in comparisons
`between the eVects of nitroglycerin and clonidine in gastric
`physiology in humans.20 Both these classes of drugs result
`in relaxation of the stomach during fasting and postprandi-
`ally. However, whereas clonidine reduced the sensation of
`pain in a dose dependent manner no such eVect of
`nitroglycerin was observed. In fact, healthy volunteers
`exposed to nitroglyercin tended to have a greater sensation
`of nausea. The selective antinociceptive activities of cloni-
`dine were also demonstrated in experiments performed in
`the human descending colon. Thus whereas
`saline
`(placebo) was associated with a stepwise, intensity related
`perception of gas and pain, clonidine markedly diminished
`the median sensation score for pain, and the ♡
`2 antagonist
`yohimbine increased pain sensation.26 Intriguingly, neither
`clonidine nor yohimbine significantly altered the sensation
`of gas relative to saline placebo. These findings suggest that
`clonidine has a selective eVect on nociceptive pathways26
`rather than on all aVerents.
`
`CEREBRAL BLOOD FLOW MEASUREMENTS
`Changes in cerebral blood flow during viscus stimulation
`require further study. Positron emission tomography and
`functional magnetic resonance imaging are the most widely
`used techniques. After intravenous injection of a radio-
`active compound, positron emission tomography assesses
`blood flow or regional cerebral metabolism in brain areas.
`Functional magnetic resonance imaging detects increases
`in oxygen concentration in areas of heightened neuronal
`activity without administration of radioactive compounds.
`For a more extensive review of these techniques, their
`advantages and limitations, the reader is referred to Aziz
`and Thompson.56 It has been demonstrated that changes in
`cerebral blood flow or evoked potentials are associated with
`viscus sensations or contractions.57 58It is also assumed that
`changes in cerebral blood flow reflect a change in cerebral
`
`control of sensory function23 but little is known of the
`transmitters involved or the sensitivity of changes in blood
`flow relative to perception. One of the most intriguing
`observations has been activation of a focus in the left dor-
`solateral prefrontal cortex in patients with IBS. Thus in a
`landmark study by Silverman et al in 1996, six patients with
`IBS were found to have activation of the left dorsal lateral
`prefrontral cortex in anticipation of a painful rectal stimu-
`lus.27 Cerebral blood flow in these patients was diVerent
`from that of healthy controls in two respects. Firstly, there
`was no activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and sec-
`ondly, there was no focal change in blood flow during the
`actual painful rectal stimulus in patients with IBS. Selective
`activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in
`anticipation of the painful stimulus suggests a change in
`attention or vigilance as this part of the brain is involved in
`focus, attention, anticipation, vigilance, and memory
`recall.27
`The initial observations have also been confirmed in
`another study59 that demonstrated activation of the same
`focus of the frontal cortex in patients with IBS or fibro-
`myalgia. Interestingly, the rectal distension stimulated this
`region in IBS patients but not in healthy controls or in
`those with fibromyalgia. In contrast, the left dorsolateral
`prefrontal cortex was activated in fibromyalgia patients fol-
`lowing somatic stimulation, but not following rectal stimu-
`lation. Taken together, these two studies suggest that
`attention and vigilance are activated in anticipation and in
`response to specific stimuli that may be related to patient
`recall as the location of the stimulus is relevant to the
`patient’s symptoms.
`While these changes in cerebral blood flow are extremely
`interesting, it is important to note that several factors may
`influence such changes. Indeed, during somatic painful
`stimuli,
`it has been demonstrated that attention or
`vigilance,60 the unpleasantness61 experience of the stimulus,
`gender,62 and even facial expression63 may influence the
`intensity of changes of cerebral blood flow, as well as the
`locus of that change. For example, Bushnell and col-
`leagues60 have shown that, during imposed attention by
`enhancing a somatic stimulus with a thermal task, there
`was significant activation of the somatosensory cortex. In
`contrast, during application of the painful somatic stimulus
`on its own or during an auditory task, there was no such
`somatosensory cortex activation. This partly explains the
`previous literature in which human brain imaging studies
`did not consistently reveal pain related activation of the
`somatosensory cortex. It also suggests that somatosensory
`cortex activation is highly modulated by cognitive factors
`that alter pain perception, including attention and previous
`experience.
`The perceived u

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket