throbber
Eye (2007) 21, 633-638
`© 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0950-222X/07 $30.00
`www. nature com/eye
`
`@
`
`Oa
`
`a wb
`
`a=Q
`
`i?)
`
`=r
`
`c
`?)
`b=
`
`How red is a white
`eye? Clinical grading
`of normal
`conjunctival
`hyperaemia
`
`Abstract
`
`Purpose To quantify the level of normal
`bulbar conjunctival hyperaemia using the
`Cornea and Contact Lens Research Unit
`
`(CCLRU) grading scale, and to investigate
`inter-observer agreement.
`Methods Bulbar conjunctival hyperaemia
`was assessed by two trained observers, using
`the CCLRU grading scale (zero to four units)
`interpolated into 0.1 increments, on the right
`eye of 121 healthy, non-contact lens-wearing
`subjects (male =—58, female = 63, median
`age = 28 years, range 16-77). The eye was
`observed using a slit-lamp bio-microscope
`(x10 magnification) under diffuse, white
`illumination. The subject’s position of gaze
`was directed to allow grading of four
`quadrants: superior, nasal, inferior, and
`temporal conjunctiva. Bulbar redness was
`defined as the average of those four grades of
`conjunctival hyperaemia. A further twenty
`subjects were recruited to assess inter-
`observer agreement (male =—8, female = 12,
`median age = 23 years}.
`Results The average bulbar redness was
`1.93 (+0.32 SD) units. The nasal (2.3+0.4)
`and temporal (2.1+0.4) quadrants were
`significantly redder than the superior
`(1.6+0.4) and inferior (1.7+ 0.4) quadrants
`(P< 0.0001). Males had redder eyes than
`females by 0.2 units. Inter-observer 95%
`limits of agreement for bulbar redness was
`0.38 units.
`
`Conclusions The average bulbar redness of
`1.9 units was higher than expected, reflecting
`the design of the grading scale. A bulbar
`redness of greater than 2.6 units may be
`considered abnormal, and a change in
`bulbar redness of >0.4 units may be
`significant.
`Eye (2007) 21, 633-638. doi:10.1038/sj.eye.6702295,;
`published online 3 March 2006
`
`PJ Murphy', JSC Lau', MML Sim' and RL Woods?
`
`Keywords: clinical grading; bulbar redness;
`conjunctival hyperaemia
`
`Introduction
`
`Conjunctival hyperaemia is caused by
`vasodilation of the conjunctival blood vessels
`against the white background of the sclera. The
`vasodilation produces the red appearance of the
`white of the eye, and so the condition is
`sometimes referred to as ‘red eye’, whereas an
`apparently healthy eye with no vasodilation is
`referred to as a ‘white eye’ (Figure 1).
`Increased conjunctival hyperaemia is a
`clinical sign for a wide range of ocular disease,
`inflammation, and irritation. Among the many
`conditions it has been recorded with are
`
`meibomian gland dysfunction and marginal
`blepharitis,’ conjunctivitis?“ contact lens
`wear,” cosmetics,” hypertension, diabetes,*
`acute angle-closure glaucoma, autoimmune
`disease, chemical injury,”° episcleritis,
`uveitis,’sickle cell disease,” and
`pharmaceutical drug use.’*
`Clinical grading scales that allow the
`assessinent of severity have been developed for
`many ocular conditions, including the anterior
`chamberangle,"* iris neovascularisation,’*
`retinal nerve fibre layer atrophy,”* focal
`narrowing ofretinal arterioles in glaucoma,”
`diabetic retinopathy," hypertensive
`arteriosclerosis,’’ tarsal abnormalities,*" and
`lens opacities.*! Similar scales have been
`developed to grade conjunctival hyperaemia.
`These bulbar redness scales have utilised verbal
`
`22-36
`
`descriptions, photographs, or paintings that
`illustrate an increasing level of conjunctival
`hyperaemia, and they have been particularly
`used in clinical studies of contact lens wear and
`dry eye???" The grading scale is typically
`divided into four or five grades. However, the
`
`Page 1 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`"Cardiff University, School of
`Optometry and Vision
`Sciences, Cardiff, UK
`
`*Schepens Eye Research
`Institute, Harvard Medical
`School, Boston, MA, USA
`
`
`
`Correspondence:
`PJ Murphy,
`School of Optometry and
`Vision Sciences,
`Cardiff University,
`King Edward VIl Avenue,
`Cardiff CF10 3NB, UK
`Tel: +44 2920 874703;
`Fax: + 44 2920 874859.
`E-mail: MurphyR@
`cfac.uk
`
`Received: 5 July 2005
`Accepted in revised form:
`15 January 2006
`Published online: 3 March
`2006
`
`This work was presented at
`fhe Association for Research
`in Vision and
`
`Ophthalmology Annual
`Meeting, Fort Lauderdale,
`Florida, 25th-29th April
`2004
`
`Page 1 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

`

`634
`
`How red is a white eye?
`PJ Murphy ef af
`
`
`
`Figure 1 Typical ‘normal’ white eye (courtesy of Dr Trefford L
`Simpson, Centre for Contact Lens Research, School of Optome-
`try, University of Waterloo, Canada).
`
`scales can be interpolated into decimal intervals to
`increase their sensitivity.”Papas“ showed that by
`decimalising the Cornea and Contact Lens Research Unit
`(CCLRU) grading scale for bulbar redness, the grading
`approximates an interval scale. The problem of an
`ordinal grading scale, producing unequal grading
`divisions, has also been considered by using digitised
`morphing of the grading scales, or by removing the
`subjective input of the observer through image analysis
`of the image.*”
`Although conjunctival hyperaemia is accepted as an
`important clinical sign of ocular disease or inflammation,
`and grading scales are frequently used to assess the
`severity or degree of change in bulbar redness, no
`previous studies have been presented that consider the
`normal, unstimulated level of conjunctival hyperaemia.
`An understanding of what can be considered normalis
`crucial when assessing any presenting conjunctival
`hyperaemia. In this paper, we report the prevalence of
`conjunctival hyperaemia in healthy, non-contact lens
`wearing eyes, in a cross-sectional study and inter-
`observer agreement of the CCLRU bulbar redness scale.
`
`Materials and methods
`
`Prevalence study
`
`A total of 121 healthy subjects (male = 58, female = 63,
`median age = 28 years, range = 16-77) participated. All
`subjects had no current or previous ocular disease or
`systemic disease, medication, or allergy knownto affect
`bulbar redness. Subjects with subclinical minor ocular
`conditions, such as marginal blepharitis, may have been
`included. As such, our sample represents a typical
`population that may be present in a clinic. Contact lens
`wearers were included, if the contact lenses had not been
`
`worn during the previous 2 weeks. A duration of 2 weeks
`has been considered as sufficient time for any contact
`lens-related conjunctival hyperaemia to have resolved.**
`Conjunctival hyperaemia was assessed by twotrained
`observers (JL, MS) using the CCLRU grading scale,
`interpolated to 0.1 unit increments. This photographic
`scale was developed by the CCLRUat the University of
`New South Wales, Australia and comprised four images
`that increase in severity of the condition, and are labelled
`as follows: 1, very slight; 2, slight; 3, moderate; 4, severe.
`Only the right eye of each subject was examined using a
`slit-lamp bio-microscope (x 10 magnification) under
`diffuse, white illumination. The subject’s position of gaze
`was directed to allow grading of four quadrants:
`superior, nasal, inferior, and temporal. The bulbar
`redness score was defined as the average of the scores of
`the four quadrants.
`
`Inter-observer study
`
`A further 20 subjects (male =8, female = 12, median
`age = 21 years, range = 19-28) were recruited to assess
`the inter-observer agreement between the two observers,
`at the completion of the prevalence study. The study
`procedure was repeated using the same selection criteria
`and grading procedures. The four quadrants of the right
`eye of each subject were independently graded by the
`two observers (JIL, MS) (ie masked from each other's
`observations), and the order of subject assessment by
`observer was randomised.
`
`Data analysis
`
`As the prevalence (Figure 2a—e) and the inter-observer
`difference (Figure 4) data were approximately normally
`distributed, and this grading scale approximates an
`interval scale,“ and Barbeito and Simpson” have argued
`that parametrical statistical tests can be applied to such
`data, we used parametric statistical tests. Inter-observer
`agreement was determined as the 95% limits of
`agreement,” which is 1.96 times the standard deviation
`of the inter-observer difference scores (ie grade from
`observer 1 minus grade from observer 2).
`
`Results
`
`Prevalence study
`
`As shownin Figure 2, a significant difference in redness
`was found between quadrants (repeated measures
`ANOVA,F360 = 281, P-<0.0001). Post hoc paired t-tests
`found significant differences in redness betweenall
`quadrants (f29>4.3, P<0.0001), with the nasal (2.3+0.4)
`(mean units+S5D) and temporal (2.1+0.4) quadrants
`
`
`
`Eye
`
`Page 2 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`Page 2 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

`

`How red is a white eye?
`&
`
`PJ Murphy et af
`635
`
`Superior
`
`b Inferior
`
`Temporal
`
`d Nasal
`
`a
`
`on
`
`0.56
`
`1.0
`
`1.6
`
`2.0
`
`2.5
`
`3.0
`
`0.5
`
`1.0
`
`1.6
`
`2.0
`
`25
`
`3.0
`
`numberofeyes a
`Bulbar redness 0.5
`
`grade (units
`
`f
`
`Temporal
`
`1.0
`
`15
`
`2.0
`
`2.5
`
`3.0
`
`
`
`
`Superior
`
`Inferior
`
`2.3+0.4 Nasal
`
`redness grade (units)
`
`Figure 2. Distribution of (a-d) the redness scores for each quadrant, (e) bulbar redness scores, and (f) the quadrant mean redness
`scores.
`
`redder than the superior (1.6+0.4) and inferior (1.7 +0.4)
`quadrants.
`The average bulbar redness was 1.93 (40.32) units
`(Figure 2e). Figure 3 showsthat bulbar redness appeared
`to increase slightly with age by about 0.05 units per
`decade (ry145 = 0.23, P=0.01); however, a multiple
`regression analysis (F113 =9.4, P =0.0002) found that
`most of the apparent effect of age was explained by
`males having redder eyes than females by 0.22 units (age:
`ts = 1.48, P=0.14; gender: fy, = 3.43, P = 0.0008), there
`being more older subjects who were male and more
`younger subjects who were female. One observer had a
`slight tendency to record higher redness scores, which
`wasnot accounted for by differences in subject ages or
`genders (difference in average bulbar redness 0.22 units,
`Fi 13 =4-1, P=0.045).
`
`3.0
`
`25
`
`2
`52.0,

`8
`o
`
`1.5 4
`
`Bulbar redness
`

`
`
`
`?
`
`25
`a
`
`‘5
`
`&
`
`t
`
`K
`
`’

`Hh,
`a
`
`1.0 +
`10
`
`20
`
`:
`30
`
`:
`1
`50
`40
`age (years)
`
`&
`
`+
`60
`
`Female
`© Male
`+
`70
`
`80
`
`redness appeared to increase with age
`Figure 3 Bulbar
`(n=121), but the effect was explained by the greater bulbar
`redness of males.
`
`Inter-observer study
`
`Nosignificant difference was found between the grading
`of the two observers, overall or for each quadrant
`
`(ti9<1.54, P>0.14), except for the temporal quadrant
`(ty = 2.54, P = 0.02). Figure 4 showsthe inter-observer
`comparison of redness scores for the average bulbar
`redness, and the tendency for one observerto give higher
`
`Page 3 of 6
`
`
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`Fye
`
`Page 3 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

`

`How red is a white eye?
`
`PJ Murphy et af
`636
`
`Bulbar redness
`
`(units)
`difference
`
`Inter-observer agreement in bulbar redness (1 = 20)
`Figure 4
`was good, with a coefficient of agreementof 0.4 units.
`
`microscope at x 10 magnification. Conjunctival
`hyperaemia is often observed unaided at a distance of
`about 1 m (eg facing a patient across a desk). Even at our
`low slit-lamp magnification, smaller blood vessels
`become evident and may influence the observer’s
`perception of the clinical grade. However, the CCLRU
`grading scale was designed with the expectation that the
`observer would useaslit lamp. Efron”® reported a similar
`0.5
`4
`1.5
`2
`2.5
`3
`higher than expected hyperaemia grading, indicating
`grade by observer 1 (units)
`that a grade of more than 2 (with the Efron Grading
`Scale) is abnormal. The Efron scale offers a similar
`grading range to the CCLRUscale, but is pictorial. It
`seemslikely, then, that the normal ‘white’ eye is redder in
`appearance than commonly determined through casual
`observation.
`
`scoresto redder eyes (r* = 0.38, P = 0.004). The 95% limits
`of agreement was 0.38 units for bulbar redness, and
`varied between quadrants with a maximum of 0.85
`(inferior) and minimum of 0.50 (nasal) units. Agreement
`may have been improved by controlling gaze eccentricity,
`which may be appropriate for a research study, but is not
`practical in clinical practice.
`
`Discussion
`
`The average bulbar redness of 121 people with healthy
`(white) eyes was 1.9 units. As the upper 95% confidence
`limit was 2.6 units, a CCLRU bulbar redness grade of
`more than 2.6 may be considered abnormal. This average
`grade and upper confidence limit was higher than our a
`priori expectations. In similar studies of healthy eyes, the
`median corneal staining grade was 0.1 units and the
`upper confidence limit was 0.5 units,““ whereas the
`average upper palpebral conjunctiva grade was1.2 units
`and the upper confidence limit was 2.0 units.** Although
`the typical conjunctival staining is consistent with the
`generalised verbal grading proposed by Woods™ and
`implied by the written descriptions associated with the
`CCLRUgrading scale and other grading scales (eg Efron,
`1997), typical palpebral conjunctival grades and bulbar
`redness grades appear higher. We consider two
`alternative explanations for bulbar redness: either the
`normal ‘white eye’ appearanceis redder than previously
`assumed, or the calibration of the grading scale is wrong.
`As McMonnies and Ho* and McMonniese¢ al*®
`described how conjunctival hyperaemia can vary with
`factors such aslack of sleep, eyestrain, wind, dust, smog,
`smoke, and alcohol, we screened our subjects for these
`factors. However, 11 potential subjects who did not meet
`our selection criteria, mainly because of the use of
`medications, showed no apparent difference in bulbar
`redness compared to the 121 healthy eyes. Figure 1
`demonstrates the effect of image magnification on the
`perception of conjunctival hyperaemia. In our study, the
`eye was observed underdiffuse light, using a slit-lamp
`
`Turning to the second hypothesis,if it is assumed that
`‘normal’ should be located around the lower grades on
`the scale (eg Woods, 1989) to provide room for
`progression of the condition; then the high average
`bulbar redness and range of 1.2-2.9 units among these
`121 healthy eyes suggest that the CCLRU scale may have
`an inadequate or misplaced dynamic range. A good
`grading scale mustbe bothsensitive to the severity of the
`condition and specific in determining what is normal.
`Althoughthe average bulbar redness grade for our
`subject population was higher than weexpected, the
`wide variance of the distribution (Figure 2e) and the
`relatively small inter-observer 95% limits of agreement
`indicate that the gradeis able to distinguish between
`degrees of conjunctival hyperaemia. Figure 3 showsthat
`no eye received a bulbar redness grade of 1 unit or less.
`The photographic image used as an example for a grade
`1 (very slight) is particularly white in appearance, and
`mayillustrate unusually low conjunctival hyperaemia.
`Although this grade may not be needed for grading the
`normal appearance, it ensures that the scale provides for
`the abnormal condition of a very white eye, such as that
`produced by anaemia. It also suggests that the CCLRU
`grading scale for conjunctival hyperaemia may need to
`be extended to values greater than its current maximum
`grade of 4 units.
`It is interesting to note the variation in redness across
`the four conjunctival quadrants, and the age- and
`gender-related differences in average bulbar redness. The
`nasal and temporal quadrants have the highest redness
`scores, possibly reflecting their exposure to
`environmental conditions. The same variation was noted
`by McMonnies et al?* and Papaset al.*' For the purpose of
`analysis, the four quadrants were averaged to produce
`our bulbar rednessscore. As this bulbar redness includes
`
`the results of the superior and inferior quadrants,if the
`observer had chosen to grade conjunctival hyperaemia
`from the exposed conjunctiva only, then the bulbar
`redness scores would have been higher. If bulbar redness
`
`
`
`Eye
`
`Page 4 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`Page 4 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

`

`How red is a white eye?
`PJ Murphy ez af
`
`ge
`637
`
`had been based on the temporal and nasal quadrants
`only, the average bulbar redness would have been
`2.214036 (range 1.2-3.1) units, and the upper 95%
`confidence limit 2.92 units. When a bulbar redness score
`
`is recorded, the quadrants viewed should be recorded
`and care must be taken when comparing bulbar redness
`scores. In our study, males tended to have redder eyes
`than females by about 0.2 units, and bulbar redness
`increased by 0.05 units per decade. These findings are
`similar to McMonnies and Ho,” who observed 470 non-
`contact lens wearers (227 males and 252 females). Using
`the McMonnies scale, with its six grades and no decimal
`interpolation, they found an average difference of 0.5
`units between genders, and a grading change of 0.16
`units per decade. The gradual increase in redness with
`age may be attributable to a reduction in arteriolar wall
`muscle tone, but there is no obvious explanation for the
`difference between genders.
`A difference in bulbar redness of 0.4 units or more
`
`between observers may be considered significant,
`because such a difference would be greater than the
`inter-observer 95% limits of agreement found for our two
`observers. No significant difference was found between
`the two observers, except for the temporal quadrant.
`There was a tendency for observer 1 to give lower scores
`than observer 2 to eyes that were more red (Figure 4). In
`our study, the two observers were trainee optometrists.
`Before the study commenced, the two observers and one
`of the other authors (PJM, an experienced user ofclinical
`grading scales) discussed grading strategies and
`compared the bulbar redness grades assigned to a series
`of human subjects. No measurement of the inter-observer
`agreement was made before data collection. Trained
`observers have better inter- and intra-observer
`agreement.*”* In a similar study on clinical grading of
`the upper palpebral conjunctiva of non-contact lens
`wearers,’ the inter-observer 95% limits of agreement at
`the beginning was 0.76 units, but improved to 0.24 units
`at the end of the study. In another similar study on
`corneal staining,” the inter-observer 95% limits of
`agreement was 0.36 units, and they reported no
`differences in agreement between the start and end of the
`study. In studies on grading bulbar redness of
`photographs, Papas™ found an inter-observer 95% limits
`of agreement of 0.8 units for seven experienced
`observers, and Chonget al** found inter-observer 95%
`limits of agreements of 0.32-0.42 units for five
`experienced observers. Thus, our 0.4-unit inter-observer
`95% limits of agreement is comparable to two previous
`studies that also used real eyes,““* and similar toor
`smaller than™” studies that used photographs to assess
`inter-observer agreement.
`Intra-observer agreementis also important in the
`assessment of grading scales. Inter-observer agreement
`
`compares two (or more) independent observers, whereas
`intra-observer agreement describes the repeatability of
`an observer, the ability to give the same result at each
`time of assessment. Both can be used to interpret changes
`in grading scale scores and to determine sample sizes
`necessary for future studies. Intra-observer 95% limits of
`agreements have been reported ranging from 0.78 to 1.52
`units in studies using photographs,°47°"*! values that
`appear to be larger than comparable studies of inter-
`observer agreement. As intra-observer agreement was
`not found in our study, the significance of a change
`between observations of real eyes made by a single
`observer is not known.
`
`In conclusion, although the bulbar conjunctival
`hyperaemia of a white eye may be redder than expected,
`this probably reflects the normal physiological detail
`visible by slit-lamp microscopy and not an error in the
`design of the grading scale. Given that normal bulbar
`redness can range from 1.3 to 2.6 units, it is more
`important that the clinician make note of the baseline
`appearance, as a change in bulbar redness score of 0.4
`units or more may be significant.
`
`Acknowledgements
`
`The CCLRU grading scale is available in the UK from
`Johnson and Johnson Vision Care, or worldwide from
`the Institute for Eye Research Ltd in Australia
`{enquiry@ier.org.au) or www.ierorg.au.
`
`References
`
`1. Albietz JM. Conjunctival histologic findings of dry eye and
`non-dry eye contact lens wearing subjects. CLAG J 2001; 27:
`35-39.
`
`2 Bloch-Michel E. Chronic allergic conjunctivitis. Int
`Ophthalmol Clin 1988; 28: 321-323.
`3 Donshik PC. Allergic conjunctivitis. It Ophthalmol Clin
`1988; 28: 294-301.
`4 Williams CPR, McGill JI. Allergic eye disease: an overview.
`CE Optom 2001; 4: 97-100.
`5 Soni PS, Horner DG, Ross J. Ocular response to lens care
`systems in adolescent soft contact lens wearers. Optom Vis
`Set 1996; 73: 70-85.
`6 Wolffsohn JS. Contact lens related red eye. CE Optom 2001;
`4: 110-112.
`
`7 Marzulli FN, Simon ME. Eyeirritation from topically
`applied drugs and cosmetics: pre-clinical studies. Aim J
`Optom Arch Amt Acad Optom 1971; 48: 61-79.
`8 Chen PC, Li 5G, Zweifach BW. Comparisons of
`morphometric alterations in human bulbar conjunctiva
`microvascular network in hypertension and diabetes. [rt J
`Microcive Clin Exp 1986; 5: 200.
`9 Bain KE. Differential diagnosis of the red eye. CE Optom
`2001; 4: 105-109.
`10 Buckley RJ. Inflammatory disease of the anterior eye. CE
`Optom 2001; 4: 101-104.
`
`Page 5 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`ae
`
`Page 5 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

`

`638
`
`How red is a white eye?
`P) Murphy et af
`
`11
`
`12
`
`13
`
`14
`
`15
`
`16
`
`17
`
`18
`
`19
`
`20
`
`21
`
`22
`
`23
`
`24
`
`25
`
`26
`
`27
`
`28
`
`29
`
`30
`
`Millis E. Contact lenses and the red eye. Covttact Lens Ant Eye
`1997; 20(Suppl): 5-10.
`Paton D. The conjunctival sign of sickle cell disease. Arch
`Ophthalmol 1961; 66: 90-94.
`Sloan PG. Ocular side effects of systemic medication. Art J
`Optom Arch Am Acad Optom 1962; 39: 459-470.
`van Herick W, Shaffer RN, Schwartz A. Estimation of width
`of angle of anterior chamber: incidence andsignificance of
`the narrow angle. Am | Ophthalmol 1969; 68: 626-629.
`Teich SA, Walsh JB. A grading system foriris
`neovascularization: prognostic implication for treatment.
`Ophthalmology 1981; 88: 1102-1106.
`Niessen AGJE, van den Berg TJTP, Langerhorst CT,
`Bossuyt PMM. Grading of retinal nerve fibre layer with a
`photographic reference set. Am J Ophthalmol 1995; 120:
`577-586.
`
`Boehm AG, Bowd C, Vasile C, El-Beltagi TA, Booth M,
`Zangwill LM et al. Comparison of two grading methods to
`evaluate focal narrowing of retinal arterioles in glaucoma.
`Graef Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 2002; 240: 810-815.
`Kinyoun JL, Martin DC, Fujimoto WT, Leonetti DL.
`Ophthalmoscopy versus fundus photographs for detecting
`and grading diabetic retinopathy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
`1992; 33: 1888-1893.
`KanskiJJ. CHnical Ophthalmology: A Systematic Approach, 5th
`ed. Butterworth-Heinemann: Oxford, 2003.
`Lofstrom T, Anderson JS, Kruse A. Tarsal abnormalities: a
`new grading system. CLAO J 1998; 24: 210-215.
`Chylack LT, Leske MC, 5perduto R, Khu P, McCarthy D.
`Lens opacities classification system. Arch Ophthalmol! 1988;
`106: 330-344.
`
`McMonnies CW, Chapman-Davies A. Assessment of
`conjunctival hyperemia in contact lens wearers. Part 1. Am J
`Optom Physiol Opt 1987, 64: 246-250.
`Woods RL. Quantitative slit lamp observations in contact
`lens practice. {| BCLA Sei Meetings 1989; 12: 42-45.
`Cornea and Contact Lens Research Unit (CCLRU) Grading
`Scales (1996). University of New South Wales, School of
`Optometry, Sydney, Australia.
`Terry RL, Schnider CM, Holden BA, Cornish R, GrantT,
`Sweeney D et al. CCLRU standards for success of daily
`and extended wear contact lenses. Optom Vis Sci 1993; 70:
`234-243.
`
`Efron N. Clinical application of grading scales for contact
`lens complications. Optician 1999; 5604(213): 26-35.
`Holden BA, Sweeney DF, Swarbrick HA, Vannas A,
`Nilsson KT, Efron N. The vascular response to long-term
`extended contact lens wear. Clin Exp Optom 1986; 69:
`112-119.
`
`McMonnies CW, Chapman-Davies A. Assessment of
`conjunctival hyperemia in contact lens wearers. Part 2. Am }
`Optom Physiol Opt 1987; 64: 251-255.
`Begley CG, Barr JT, Edrington TB, Long WD, McKenney
`CD, Chalmers RL. Characteristics of corneal staining in
`hydrogel contact lens wearers. Optom Vis Sci 1996; 73:
`193-200.
`
`Guillon M, Shah D. Objective measurement of contact
`lens-induced conjunctival redness. Optom Vis Sci 1996; 73:
`595-605.
`
`31
`
`32
`
`33
`
`34
`
`35
`
`36
`
`37
`
`38
`
`39
`
`40
`
`41
`
`42
`
`43
`
`44
`
`45
`
`46
`
`47
`
`48
`
`49
`
`50
`
`51
`
`Papas EB, Vajdic CM, Austen R, Holden BA. High oxygen
`transmissibility soft contact lenses do not induce limbal
`hyperaemia. Curr Eye Res 1997; 16: 942-048.
`Bailey IL, Bullimore MA, Raasch TW, Taylor HR. Clinical
`grading and the effects of scaling. Invest Ophthalinol Vis Sci
`1991; 32: 422-432.
`Sparrow JM, Frost NA, Pantelides EP, Laidlaw DAN.
`Decimalization of the Oxford Clinical Cataract Classification
`
`and Grading System. Ophthal Epidemiol 2000; 7: 49-60.
`Papas EB. Key factors in the subjective and objective
`assessment of conjunctival erythema. Invest Ophthalinol Vis
`Set 2000; 41: 687-691.
`Villumsen J, Ringquist J, Alm A. Image analysis of
`conjunctival hyperemia. Acta Ophthalmol 1991; 69: 536-539.
`Willingham FF, Cohen KL, Coggins JM, Tripoli NK, Ogle
`JW, Goldstein GM. Automatic quantitative measurement of
`ocular hyperemia. Curr Eye Res 1995; 14: 1101-1108.
`Owen CG, Fitzke FW, Woodward EG. A new computer
`assisted objective method for quantifying vascular changes
`of the bulbar conjunctivae. Ophthal Physiol Opt 1996; 16:
`430-437.
`
`Efron N, Morgan PB, Jagpal R. Validation of computer
`morphs for grading contact lens complications. Ophthal
`Physiol Opt 2002; 22: 341-349,
`Fieguth P, Simpson TL. Automated measurementof bulbar
`redness. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sei 2002; 43: 340-347.
`Wolffsohn J5, Purslow C. Clinical monitoring of ocular
`physiology using digital image analysis. Corttact Lens Ant
`Eye 2003; 26: 27-35.
`Barbeito R, Simpson TL. Should level of measurement
`considerations affect the choice of statistic? Optom Vis Sci
`1991; 68: 236-242.
`Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for assessing
`agreement between two methods of clinical measurement.
`Lancet 1986; 1(8476): 307-310.
`Dundas M, Walker A, WoodsRL. Clinical grading of corneal
`staining of non-contact lens wearers. Ophthal Physiol Opt
`2001; 21: 30-35.
`MacKinven J, McGuinness CL, Pascal E, Woods RL. Clinical
`grading of the upper palpebral conjunctiva of non-contact
`lens wearers. Optom Vis Sci 2001; 78: 13-18.
`McMonnies CW, Ho A. Conjunctival hyperaemia in non-
`contact lens wearers. Acta Ophthalmol 1991; 69: 799-801.
`McMonnies CW, Chapman-Davies A, Holden BA. The
`vascular response to contact lens wear. Am J Optom Physiol
`Opt 1982; 59: 795-799,
`Efron N, Morgan PB, Jagpal RK. The combined influence of
`knowledge, training and experience when grading contact
`lens complications. Ophthal Physiol Opt 2003; 23: 79-85.
`Chong T, Simpson TL, Fonn D. The repeatability of discrete
`and continuousanterior segment grading scales. Optom Vis
`Sei 2000; 77: 244-251.
`Efron N. Grading scales for contact lens complications.
`Ophthal Physiol Opt 1998; 18: 182-186.
`Lloyd M. Lies, statistics and clinical significance. J Br
`Contact Lens Assoc 1992; 15: 67-70.
`Efron N, Morgan PB, Katsara 55. Validation of grading
`scales for contact lens complications. Ophthal Physiol Opt
`2001; 21: 17-29.
`
`Fye
`
`Page 6 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`Page 6 of 6
`
`SLAYBACK EXHIBIT 1026
`
`

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket