throbber
Technical Report UMTRI-98-4
`
`June, 1998
`
`Map Design: An On-the-Road Evaluation of
`the Time to Read Electronic Navigation Displays
`
`Christopher Nowakowski and Paul Green
`
`umtri
`
`HUMAN FACTORS
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`2. Government Accession No.
`
`1. Report No.
`UMTRI-98-4
`4. Title and Subtitle
`Map Design: An On-the-Road Evaluation of the Time
`to Read Electronic Navigation Displays
`
`Technical Report Documentation Page
`
`3. Recipient’s Catalog No.
`
`5. Report Date
`June, 1998
`6. Performing Organization Code
`account 364781
`8. Performing Organization Report No.
`UMTRI-98-4
`10. Work Unit no. (TRAIS)
`
`11. Contract or Grant No.
`
`13. Type of Report and Period Covered
`5/96 - 6/98
`14. Sponsoring Agency Code
`RCE #939421
`
`7. Author(s)
`Christopher Nowakowski and Paul Green
`9. Performing Organization Name and Address
`The University of Michigan
`Transportation Research Institute (UMTRI)
`2901 Baxter Rd, Ann Arbor, Michigan
`48109-2150 USA
`12. Sponsoring Agency Name and Address
`University of Michigan
`ITS Research Center of Excellence
`200 Engineering Programs Building, 2609 Draper Drive
`Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 USA
`15. Supplementary Notes
`The ITS Research Center is supported by fees from industrial members and contracts
`from government agencies. Toyota was the primary sponsor for this project.
`16. Abstract
`This report covers the third of four experiments examining the time to read electronic
`maps while driving. Three factors were varied: (1) the number of streets displayed
`(12 street and 24 street maps), (2) the street label text size (10, 12, and 14 point), and
`(3) the time of day (day vs. night driving). Sixteen drivers (ages 18-30 and >65, both
`men and women) drove a test vehicle on public roads. Subjects were given three
`tasks while driving: (1) find the name of the street being driven, (2) find the name of a
`cross street ahead, and (3) find the location of a particular street on the map.
`
`The largest effect was age which increased task response times by 40 to 80%. Each
`additional labeled street increased the response time by 7 to 140 ms depending on
`the task (up to 30%). Using 14 point reduced response time by 200 ms (up to 10%).
`
`Subjective ratings by the drivers revealed uneasiness about their ability to drive
`safely when the task required more than 5 seconds to complete. To avoid this
`discomfort, using 14 point text and no more than 12 labeled streets is recommended.
`
`Further, average response times from on-the-road were within 15% of the mean for
`the same task in a previous simulator experiment. The pattern of results (factors
`significant, their relative impact) were also similar, validating the simulator results.
`17. Key Words
`18. Distribution Statement
`ITS, intelligent transportation systems,
`No restrictions. This document is
`human factors, ergonomics, driving,
`available to the public through the
`electronic maps, navigation systems,
`National Technical Information Service,
`route guidance, driver interface
`Springfield, Virginia 22161
`19. Security Classify. (of this report)
`20. Security Classify. (of this page)
`21. No. of pages
`(None)
`(None)
` 121
`Form DOT F 1700 7 (8-72)
`Reproduction of completed page authorized
`
`22. Price
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`ii
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Map Design: An On-the-Road Evaluation
`of the Factors Affecting the
`Time to Read Electronic Maps
`UMTRI Technical Report 98-4
`University of Michigan
`Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA
`Christopher Nowakowski and Paul Green
`
`ISSUES
`
`1
`On-the-Road Evaluation
`1. When do drivers feel that map reading is unsafe while driving?
`2. How many streets should be displayed on an in-vehicle navigation system?
`3. What size text should be used for the street labels?
`4. What is the effect of time of day (day vs. night) on map reading?
`
`Simulator Validation
`1. How do the previous simulator results compare with the on-the-road results?
`
`2
`
`MAP TASKS
`
`Sample Map
`
`Task 1 - On Street
`What street are you on?
`Subject Finds: Edward
`Subject Responds: male M(
`
`key)
`
`Task 2 - Cross Street
`What is the 3rd Cross Street?
`Subject Finds: Suzanne
`( key)
`F
`Responds: female
`What is the 4th Cross Street?
`Subject Finds: only 3 streets
`(
`0
`key)
`Responds: not there
`
`Task 3 - Where is?
`Where is Jonathan?
`Response: behind
`Where is Florence?
`Response: left
`Where is David?
`Response: right
`Where is Albert?
`Response: ahead
`Where is Tammy?
`Response: not there
`
`( )
`
`( )
`
`( )
`
`( )
`
`( )
`0
`
`DAVID
`
`DEBORAH
`
`SUZANNE
`
`EDWARD T
`
`ALBERT
`
`HENRY
`
`JANET
`
`FRANK
`
`FLORENCE
`
`GRACE
`
`HERESA
`
`JONATHAN
`
`Response Keypad
`Task 3 Directional Keys
`right
`ahead
`left
`behind
`0 not there
`Tasks 1 and 2
`Response Keys
`0 not there
`male
`female
`
`0
`
`M F 0
`
`M F
`
`iii
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`3
`
`METHOD
`
`Expressway Driving Scenario
`
`Subjects
`Session
`1
`2
`Day
`Night
`Night
`Day
`
`Age 18 - 30
`Men Women
`2
`2
`2
`2
`
`Age >65
`Men Women
`2
`2
`2
`2
`
`Map
`
`Response
`Keypad
`
`Main Test Conditons
`Text Size
`12 Streets 24 Streets
`10 POINT
`12 POINT
`14 POINT
`
`(cid:214) (cid:214)(cid:214) (cid:214)(cid:214) (cid:214)
`
`4
`
`ON-THE-ROAD EVALUATION RESULTS
`
`Cumulative (%)
`
`100
`75
`50
`5
`
`02
`
`Task 2 - Cross Street
`20
`15
`10
`
`x = 3.4 s
`
`05
`
`Frequency (%)
`
`0
`
`5
`10
`15
`Response Time (s)
`
`Mature
`
`Young
`
`Error Rates
`1. Age Effect
`25
`20
`15
`10
`
`Error Rate (%)
`
`05
`
`2
`Task
`2. Number of Streets Effect
`20
`15
`10
`
` 24
`Streets
`
`12 Streets
`
`1
`
`2
`Task
`
`3
`
`05
`
`Error Rate (%)
`
`1
`
`3
`
`Cumulative (%)
`
`100
`80
`60
`40
`0
`
`02
`
`Cumulative (%)
`
`100
`75
`50
`5
`
`02
`
`Task 1 - On Street
`25
`20
`15
`10
`
`Frequency (%)
`
`6
`3
`0
`21
`4 5
`7 8
`Response Time (s)
`
`05
`
`x = 1.8 s
`
`Task 3 - Where is?
`15
`
`x = 5.2 s
`
`10
`
`05
`
`Frequency (%)
`
`0
`
`5
`15
`20
`10
`Response Time (s)
`
`Issue 1 - When is map reading unsafe?
`
`0
`
`5
`1
`8
`6
`4
`2
`3
`7
`Response Time (s)
`
`9
`
`iv
`
`5 4 3 2 1
`
`Very Unsafe
`Unsafe
`Sometimes Unsafe
`Safe
`
`Very Safe
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Issue 2 - How many streets to display?
`Response time increase
`per each displayed street
`150
`
`10
`
`Task 3 Streets Effect
`
`Mature
`
`Recommendation:
`
`Display £12 Streets
`
`Young
`
`24
`12
`Streets displayed
`
`2468
`
`(s)
`
`1
`
`2
`Task
`
`3
`
`100
`(ms)
`
`50
`
`0
`
`Issue 3 - What text size to use?
`Task 1 Point Size Effect
`2.0
`1.9
`1.8
`1.7
`1.6
`1.5
`
`24 st.
`
`Task 2 Point Size Effect
`4.5
`4.0
`3.5
`3.0
`2.5
`
`12 st.
`
`10
`
`12
`Point Size
`
`14
`
`Recommendations:
`
`1. When possible,
` Use 14 point.
`
`2. Do not use <12 point
`
`(s)
`
`24 st.
`
`(s)
`
`12 st.
`
`10
`
`12
`Point Size
`
`14
`
`Issue 4 - Day/Night Effects
`Task 1 - Night learning
` is more difficult
`
`Task 3 - Night increases
` response time
`
`(s)
`
`2.1
`2.0
`1.9
`1.8
`1.7
`1.6
`
`(s)
`
`6.0
`5.5
`5.0
`4.5
`4.0
`Night
`Day
`
`1
`
`Session
`
`2
`
`1
`
`2
`
`Session
`
`Recommendation:
`
`Issues of color,
`luminance, and contrast
`for night-use maps
`need to be addressed
`in further reasearch.
`
`Response Time Regression Equations (ms)
`RT = 6710 + 325*(A) + 6.67*(S) + 33.75*(P)2 - 832.50*(P)
`Task 1 - On Street
`+ 9.58*(C)
`
`Task 2 - Cross Street
`
`RT = 1210 + 575*(A) + 370*(X) + 40.83*(S)
`+ 8.08*(P-12)*(SL)+ 40.83*(S-12)*[MINIMUM(1,X-2)]
`
`Task 3 - Where is?
`
`RT = [1630 + 1235*(A) + 380*(T) + 136*(S) + 27*(A)*(SL)
`+ 475*(L)]*(SR)
`
`v
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Regression Equations Terms
`-1 if young
`{
`A = Age
`+1 if mature
`S = Number of streets (S ‡ 1)
`P = Point size (10 £ P £14)
`0 if £ 12 point
`{
`(P-12)(S-12.52) if > 12 pt.
`-1*(24-S) if S £ 12
`+1*(S-12) if S > 12
`
`SL = Street level
`
`{
`
`C = Clutter
`
`T = Time of day
`
`-1 for day
`{
`+1 for night
`X = Target cross street (X ‡ 1)
` -1 if ahead
`{
` 0 if not there
`+1 if behind, left or right
`
`L = Target location
`
`SR = Search result
`
`{
`
`1 if found
`1.70 if not found
`
`5
`
`SIMULATOR VALIDATION RESULTS
`
`Subjects
`
`Experiment
`
`Simulator
`On-the-Road
`
`Age 65<
`Age 18 - 30
`Men
`Women Men Women
`5
`5
`5
`5
`4
`4
`4
`4
`
`Trials per Subject
`Task 1
`Task 3
`6
`24
`24
`48
`
`Response Time Comparison
`Task 1 - On Street
`
`Cumulative (%)
`
`100
`75
`50
`25
`0
`75
`50
`25
`0
`
`Task 3 - Where is?
`
`Simulator
`x = 5.96 s
`
`On-the-Road
`x = 5.20 s
`
`0
`
`20
`15
`10
`5
`Response Time (s)
`
`246
`
`0246
`
`Frequency (%)
`
`Cumulative (%)
`
`100
`75
`50
`25
`0
`75
`50
`25
`0
`
`Simulator
`x = 1.64 s
`
`On-the-Road
`x = 1.79 s
`
`0
`
`4
`3
`2
`1
`Response Time (s)
`
`5
`
`20
`15
`10
`
`05
`
`15
`10
`
`05
`
`Frequency (%)
`
`Conclusions:
`The optimal point size
`was also dependent
`upon the display
`resolution and location
`(character visual angle).
`
`Differences in Experimental Findings
`Task 1 - Point Size Effects
`Task 3 - Point Size Effects
`2500
`7000
`
`Simulator
`
`6000
`
`5000
`
`4000
`
`(ms)
`
`On-the-Road
`
`Simulator
`
`10
`
`12
`Point Size
`
`14
`
`2000
`
`1500
`
`1000
`
`(ms)
`
`On-the-Road
`
`10
`
`12
`14
`Point Size
`
`vi
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`PREFACE
`
`This research was funded by the University of Michigan Intelligent Transportation
`Systems (ITS) Research Center for Excellence, formerly the IVHS Research Center for
`Excellence. The program is a consortium of companies and government agencies,
`working with the University, whose goal is to advance ITS research and
`implementation.
`
`The current sponsors are:
`
`• Ann Arbor Transit Authority
`• Automobile Association of America (AAA)
`• Chrysler Corporation
`• Federal Highway Administration (FHWA)
`• Ford Motor Company
`• General Motors Corporation
`• Hewlett Packard
`• Michigan Department of Transportation
`• Nissan Motors
`• NOVA Laboratories
`• Orbital Sciences
`• Road Commission of Oakland County
`• Ryder Trucks
`• Siemens Automotive
`• Toyota Motor Corporation
`
`We would like to thank the lead corporate sponsor for this project, Toyota Motor
`Corporation, for their support. Originally Cale Hodder, and now Jim Bauer (both from
`Toyota), have served as project technical monitors.
`
`Electronic maps are commonplace in automotive navigation systems in Japan, and
`soon will be common in the U.S. and Europe. To make such maps safe and easy to
`use while driving, it is important to know how engineering, individual, and task factors
`affect reading time, and how reading time can be minimized. The more time drivers
`spend looking in the vehicle, the less time they spend looking at the road, increasing
`the opportunity for crashes. Given the almost complete absence of literature on the
`time to read maps prior to this project, two specific issues were addressed.
`
`Issue 1: How long does it take to read an electronic local map as a function of label
`size and orientation, the number of streets shown, the percentage of streets
`labeled, display location, and the driver's task?
`
`Issue 2: When do drivers desire area maps instead of turn (intersection) displays?
`
`These issues were examined in 5 reports summarized on the next page:
`
`vii
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Green, P. (1998). Reading Electronic Area Maps: An Annotated Bibliography ,
`(Technical Report UMTRI-98-38).
`
`This report contains a collection of abstracts generated by the author.
`Primary articles concerned performance differences in reading street
`names due to font, how people follow directions using street maps, etc.
`There were no articles in the literature that methodically considered how
`factors related to street map design affect task completion time.
`Secondary articles considered color coding, symbols for tourist
`information, etc.
`
`Authors to be determined (1998). Preliminary Examinations of the Time to Read
`Electronic Maps: The Effects of Text and Graphic Characteristics , (Technical Report
`UMTRI-98-36).
`
`This report summarizes the initial series of simulator experiments
`concerning reading electronic maps. Included were efforts to identify
`representative maps and street names for testing and a pilot experiment
`concerning the subjective legibility of various map typefaces. In the main
`experiment, the time to read the electronic maps was found as a function
`of text size, the number of streets, text orientation, and grid-likeness.
`
`Brooks, A. and Green, P. (1998). Map Design: A Simulator Evaluation of the Factors
`Affecting the Time to Read Electronic Navigation Displays , (Technical Report
`UMTRI-98-7).
`
`This report describes a simulator experiment that was an extension of the
`first main experiment. This extension examined situations when only
`some of the street names were labeled, small text sizes, and the effect of
`map location in the vehicle.
`
`Nowakowski, C. and Green, P. (1998). Map Design: An On-the-Road Evaluation of the
`Time to Read Electronic Navigation Displays , (Technical Report UMTRI-98-4).
`
`This report summarizes an on-the-road study that was run in parallel with
`the previous report and examined similar factors. The same text sizes
`and number of streets were used, but all the streets were labeled and the
`effect of day and night was studied. These results were used to bridge
`the laboratory results to real, on-the-road situations.
`
`Brooks, A., Nowakowski, C., and Green, P. (1998). Turn-by-Turn Displays versus
`Electronic Maps: An On-the-Road Comparison of Driver Glance Behavior , (Technical
`Report UMTRI-98-37).
`
`This report describes an on-the-road study that examined when and how
`often drivers look at turn-by-turn and electronic map displays in route
`guidance. Factors examined included road type (residential, freeway,
`etc.) and the distance to the next turn/decision point.
`
`viii
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`TABLE OF CONTENTS
`
`INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................1
`Overview..............................................................................................................................1
`Issues....................................................................................................................................2
`
`TEST PLAN ...............................................................................................5
`Test Participants..............................................................................................................5
`Test Activities and Their Sequence .......................................................................5
`Map Construction ............................................................................................................7
`Task Descriptions ...........................................................................................................7
`Practice 1: Keypad Practice (Male and Female) .....................................7
`Task 1: What Street Are You On? ....................................................................8
`Task 2: What Is the nth Cross Street? ..........................................................9
`Practice 2: Keypad Practice (Arrow Keys)................................................10
`Task 3: Where Is the Target Street?............................................................11
`Test Vehicle .....................................................................................................................11
`Test Route.........................................................................................................................14
`
`RESULTS................................................................................................17
`Data Reduction...............................................................................................................17
`
`Task 1: What Street Are You On? ........................................................................18
`Errors ...........................................................................................................................18
`Response Time........................................................................................................18
`Subject Effects (Age and Gender)....................................................................19
`Context Effects (Road, Time of Day, and Map Template).............................20
`Map Design Effects (Number of Streets and Point Size)..............................20
`Task 1 Response Time Prediction Model ...................................................21
`Driving Performance.............................................................................................22
`
`Task 2: What Is the nth Cross Street? ...............................................................22
`Errors ...........................................................................................................................22
`Subject Effects (Age and Gender)....................................................................22
`Context Effects......................................................................................................24
`Map Design Effects..............................................................................................24
`Response Time........................................................................................................25
`Subject Effects (Age and Gender)....................................................................26
`Context Effects (Road, Session, and Time of Day)........................................26
`Map Design Effects (Condition and Point Size).............................................27
`Task 2 Response Time Prediction Model ...................................................28
`Driving Performance.............................................................................................29
`
`ix
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Task 3: Where Is the Target Street?...................................................................31
`Errors ...........................................................................................................................31
`Factors Influencing Error Rate...........................................................................33
`Response Time........................................................................................................34
`Subject Effects (Age and Gender)....................................................................35
`Context Effects (Road, Time of Day, and Response Location)....................36
`Map Design Effects (Number of Streets and Point Size).............................37
`Task 3 Response Time Prediction Model ...................................................38
`Driving Performance.............................................................................................39
`
`Subjective Task Ratings ............................................................................................41
`Driver Eye Movements................................................................................................43
`
`CONCLUSIONS.......................................................................................45
`
`SIMULATOR VALIDATION.......................................................................49
`Validation Method .........................................................................................................49
`
`Validation Results .........................................................................................................49
`Task 1: What Street Are You On? .................................................................49
`Context Effects......................................................................................................51
`Subject Effects......................................................................................................51
`Map Design Effects..............................................................................................52
`Task 3: Where is the Target Street?............................................................54
`Context Effects......................................................................................................56
`Subject Effects......................................................................................................57
`Map Design Effects..............................................................................................57
`
`Simulator Validation Conclusions ........................................................................59
`
`REFERENCES.........................................................................................61
`
`APPENDIX A - Participant Consent Forms..............................................63
`APPENDIX B - Subject Biographical Form..............................................65
`APPENDIX C - Instructions to Subjects...................................................67
`APPENDIX D - Map Examples.................................................................75
`APPENDIX E - Experimental Conditions by Task....................................81
`APPENDIX F - Test Vehicle Illustrations.................................................87
`APPENDIX G - Test Routes......................................................................89
`APPENDIX H - Listing of Invalid Trials and Outliers by Task..................93
`APPENDIX I
`- Lane Crossing Benchmark Images..................................95
`APPENDIX J - Table of Task 1 Error Trials.............................................97
`APPENDIX K - Task 1 Response Time ANOVA Table..............................99
`APPENDIX L - Task 2 Response Time ANOVA Table............................101
`APPENDIX M- Task 3 Error ANOVA Table............................................103
`APPENDIX N - Task 3 Response Time ANOVA Table............................105
`APPENDIX O - Simulator Validation Task 1 ANOVA Table ...................107
`APPENDIX P - Comparison of Display Positions ..................................109
`APPENDIX Q - Simulator Validation Task 3 ANOVA Table ...................111
`
`x
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Introduction
`
`Overview
`
`INTRODUCTION
`
`Recently there has been an influx of in-vehicle navigation systems into the United
`States, both as standard equipment (on the Acura RL and several Lexus vehicles) and
`as aftermarket products such as the Rockwell (now Magellan) PathMaster, the Alpine
`voice navigation system, and the Philips (now VDO) Carin system. A major concern is
`that the in-vehicle maps provided by navigation systems may be difficult to read,
`distracting drivers from attending to the road ahead. Such distractions could provide
`increased opportunities for crashes (Green, 1997) as is suspected for cellular phones
`(Goodman, Bents, Tijerina, Wierwille, Lerner, and Benel, 1997).
`
`In recognition of this safety concern, Paul Green is developing a Society of Automotive
`Engineers (SAE) standard, a precursor to an International Standards Organization
`(ISO) standard, on what drivers should be permitted to do with a navigation system
`while a vehicle is in motion. Of considerable value in developing such a standard
`would be baseline data on how long it takes to read a map as a function of its content
`and the task to be completed. Accordingly, this project is a timely coincidence.
`
`There have been numerous studies concerning matters relating to human factors and
`the design of navigation displays. (See Green, 1992 for a review.) Almost all of the
`prior research has considered issues relating to the modality to be used for navigation
`information and attentional demands of particular implementations (Dingus, McGehee,
`Hulse, Jahns, Manakkal, Mollenhauer, and Fleischman, 1995; Green, Hoekstra, and
`Williams, 1993; Green, Williams, Hoekstra, George, and Wen, 1993), not the impact of
`specific map characteristics such as is addressed by this series of experiments.
`Therefore, this project breaks new ground.
`
`The first report of this project was an annotated bibliography of the research on
`reading electronic area maps (Green, 1998). The report verified the a priori notion that
`relevant literature was limited. The vast majority of studies concern how colors should
`be assigned to areas on a map (states or provinces) so no two adjacent areas have
`the same color.
`
`Several significant pilot studies were conducted as part of the first experiment. Those
`studies identified typical content of electronic maps (number of streets, street name
`length, etc. for the United States) and developed a task set representative of what
`drivers do. The first major laboratory experiment, conducted in a driving simulator,
`examined the time to read electronic maps as a function of numerous map display
`factors (Green, 1998). A total of 20 drivers (10 under 30 years of age and 10 over 65
`years of age) operated a driving simulator while performing one of three tasks: (1)
`identifying the street being driven, (2) identifying a particular cross street, or (3)
`locating a particular street on a map. These same three representative tasks have
`been used consistently in this project. Further, they are interesting experimentally as
`they vary in complexity and completion time.
`
`1
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Introduction
`
`Variables examined in the first experiment included: (1) the number of streets shown
`(6 through 36), (2) label point size (12 and 18 point), (3) the street configuration (grid
`verses nongrid), and (4) the street label orientation (horizontal, vertical, and vertical
`stacked).
`
`The results were that the response times increased as the number of streets shown
`increased. For the label size, the reading time for 18 point text was significantly slower
`(by 11 percent) than for 12 point, especially with a large number of streets on the map.
`Large point sizes combined with many visible streets created a cluttered map which
`dramatically increased the drivers’ response times. Additionally, drivers were able to
`read the maps faster when the maps were based on a grid layout, rather than a less
`structured, more random arrangement. The regularity of the grid facilitated search.
`Finally, the best label orientations were horizontal for horizontal streets and vertical for
`vertical streets, even though horizontal text is normally easier to read. In this case, the
`vertical text facilitated the association of each label with a particular line representing a
`street.
`
`Following the initial laboratory experiment, a second laboratory experiment was
`conducted in parallel with this on-the-road study to reexamine some of the map
`display factors (Brooks and Green, 1998). Using the same three tasks as the first
`experiment, the variables examined in the second experiment included: (1) the
`number of streets (12 through 36), (2) label point size (10, 12, and 14 point), (3)
`percentage of streets labeled (33, 66, 100 percent), (4) display location (high or low on
`the center console).
`
`The results of the second laboratory experiment showed that labeled streets increased
`the response times more than the unlabeled streets for most of the tasks, and a
`maximum of 12 labeled streets should be used on the map to optimize search
`performance. The experiment also found that the use of 10 point text increased the
`response times for all tasks, especially for older drivers. In general 14 point text was
`preferred, although clutter effects were seen when using that point size on maps
`containing more than 16 labeled streets. The effect of display location was only tested
`using the first task, identify the street being driven. The higher location produced
`slightly faster response times (by 10 percent).
`
`Issues
`
`All of the initial work was conducted in a driving simulator to provide consistent test
`conditions and reduce cost. However, once the relationships of the key factors were
`established, it was necessary to determine the necessary adjustment of the laboratory
`data to predict on-the-road performance. Consequently, some of the test conditions
`from the previous simulator study (plus some additional conditions of interest) were
`explored on the road.
`
`2
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Introduction
`
`There were four key issues:
`
`1. When do drivers feel that map reading is unsafe while driving?
`
`Maps should not be so complex that they require an excessively long time to read. The
`major concern is that if drivers are not looking at the road, the risk of a crash increases.
`Prior to this research, there was a minimum of data on what drivers thought to be
`excessive. (See Hada, 1994 as an example.) One way to determine when drivers
`began to feel uncomfortable was to ask them to rate the tasks on a scale with regard to
`interference with driving and perceived safety.
`
`2. What size text and how many streets should be displayed?
`
`The first experiment tested only 12- and 18-point text sizes for street names, both of
`which are larger than the text used in some U.S. navigation systems. Unresolved was
`the impact of smaller font sizes on map reading times. Since the first experiment
`showed a large street’s effect and an interaction between streets and text size, the
`number of streets displayed was also included as a factor in the on-the-road
`experiment.
`
`3. How does the ambient lighting (time of day) affect map reading?
`
`Altering the ambient light changes both the display contrast and its overall luminance
`level. The lighting levels in the UMTRI simulator approximated dusk. Although some
`adjustments can be made, limits of the scene projector output make simulating a wide
`range of conditions, especially daylight, difficult. (This is true of all driving simulators of
`which the authors are aware.) Accordingly, on-road driving was used to examine the
`effects of ambient lighting. Readers should remember that changes in scene
`luminance and interior illumination occur together, and furthermore, day-night
`differences were accompanied by changes in traffic volume.
`
`4. How do the simulator results compare with the on-the-road results?
`
`Experiments were conducted, for the most part, in the driving simulator because the
`conditions could be well controlled, especially traffic, leading to more stable results.
`Further, Michigan winters made collecting on-the-road data impossible. Rain in the
`spring and fall can likewise be problematic leading to schedule delays. Since the data
`was collected to predict on-the-road performance, differences between the two
`contexts were examined experimentally by replicating a subset of the test conditions
`from the driving-simulator experiment in the on-the-road experiment.
`
`As an aside, the fourth experiment, which is in the planning stages as this report is
`being written, will consider when drivers want maps and when turn displays should be
`provided. The rationale is that for reasons of space and cost, only a single display
`may be available. The ideal situation would be for the navigation computer to "know"
`at any given moment which display format a driver might desire, and automatically
`present it, rather that forcing a driver to press a key each time a different format display
`should appear.
`
`3
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`4
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`FORD EX. 1021
`
`

`

`Test Plan
`
`Test Participants
`
`TEST PLAN
`
`A total of 16 licensed drivers (8 men and 8 women equally drawn from two age
`groups) participated in the experiment. The subjects were randomly selected from the
`UMTRI human factors subject database of past subjects, friends of the staff, and those
`recruited through newspaper advertisements. Only those who had not participated in
`a previous map study or any other recent UMTRI study were recruited. Subjects were
`tested on one of two roads, M-14 or I-94, and in one of two time sequences, either day
`before night or night before day, with a minimum of three days between sessions.
`(See Table 1.) The eight subjects tested on M-14 were each paid $20 for each
`session with a bonus of $15 for completing both sessions. The I-94 subjects were paid
`an extra $10 paid to compensate for the longer drive to the test site. Three additional
`subjects were tested on M-14 but discarded to preserve a balanced design after the
`unexpected start of road construction required relocation of the test site.
`
`Table 1. Subjects.
`
`Road
`
`M-14
`
`I-94
`
`Time Sequence
`(session 1, 2)
`
`Day, night
`Night, day
`Day, night
`Night, day
`
`Young (19 - 25)
`(mean=22)
`Men
`Women
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`
`Mature (65 - 75)
`(mean=68)
`Men
`Women
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`1
`
`Corrected visual acuity over all subjects ranged from 20/15 to 20/100 with means of
`20/18 for younger subjects and 20/37 for older subjects. Seventy-five percent of the
`subjects wore either glasses or contacts.
`
`Subjects drove an average of 12,000 miles per year with no differences due to age or
`gender. Only one subject had seen or used an in-vehicle navigation system, having
`participated in an on-road study of a navigation system four years ago (Green,
`Williams, Hoekstra, George, and Wen, 1993). Subjects reported, on average, that they
`had used maps 3 to 4 times over the past 6 months and that their computer use was
`daily, except for a few older subjects whose computer use was infrequent or never.
`
`Test Activities and Their Sequence
`
`Each subject began by completing a consent form (Appendix A), a biographical form
`(Appendix B), and having their vision checked. See Appendix C for the complete
`instructions to subjects.
`
`Each experimental session consisted of two practice tasks and three experimental
`tasks. (See Table 2.) During each 8-minute, 24-trial sequence, subjects drove
`approximately 8 miles. An example or two of each task was shown to the subject
`before the start of the experiment while pa

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket