throbber
1
`
`APPLE 1032
`
`APPLE 1032
`
`1
`
`

`

`THE
`
`PSYCHOLOGY
`
`OF EVERYDAY
`
`THINGS
`
`Donald A. Norman
`
`E BasicBooks
`A Division 9’ Hamlinsl’ublisben
`
`2
`
`

`

`
`
`3
`
`

`

`CH\|‘Il|{ ()\I
`
`THE
`
`PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
`
`OF EVERYDAY
`
`THINGS
`
`.*_
`
`.
`
`q
`
`”Kenneth Olsen, the engineer who founded and
`
`still runs Digital Equipment Corp, confessed at
`the annual meeting that he can ’1‘ figure out how to
`heat a cup of cofiee in the company’s microwave
`oven. ”1
`
`You Would Need an Engineering Degree
`to Figure This Out
`
`"You would need an engineering degree from MIT to work this,”
`someone once told me, shaking his head in puulement over his brand
`new digital watch. Well,
`I have an engineering degree from MIT.
`(Kenneth Olsen has two of them, and he can’t figure out a microwave
`oven.) Give me a few hours and I can figure out the watch. But why ‘
`should it take hours? I have talked with many people who can’t use all
`the features of their washing machines or cameras, who can’t figure out
`how to work a sewing machine or a video cassette recorder, who
`habitually turn on the wrong stove burner.
`Why do we put up with the frustrations of everyday objects, with
`objects that we can’t figure out how to use, with those neat plastic-
`wrappecl packages that seem impossible to open, with doors that trap
`people, with washing machines and dryers that have become too con-
`
`4
`
`

`

`
`
`\
`
`1.1 Carelman's C'offeepot for Maso-
`chists. The French artist Jacques Carel-
`man in his series of books Catalogue
`d bljtls inhvumblzs (Catalog 0/ Malinda“: ob-
`ject} provides delightful examples of
`everyday things that are deliberately
`unworkable, outrageous, or otherwise
`ill-formed.
`lacques Carelman:
`"Cof—
`feepot for Masodusts." Copyright ©
`1969-76—80 by Jacques Carelman and
`A. D. A. G. P. Paris. From Jacques Carel-
`man, Catalog 0/ Unfina'able Objects, Balland,
`éditeur, Paris-France. Used by pen-nis—
`sion of the artist.
`
`fusing to use, with audio—stereo-television-video-cassette-recorders
`that claim in their advertisements to do everything, but that make it
`almost impossible to do anything?
`The human mind is exquisitely tailored to make sense of the world.
`Give it the slightest clue and off it goes, providing explanation, ration-
`alization, understanding. Consider the objects—books, radios, kitchen
`appliances, office machines, and light switches—that make up our ev-
`eryday lives. Well-designed objects are easy to interpret and under-
`stand. They contain visible clues to their operation. Poorly designed
`objects can be difficult and frustrating to use. They provide no clues—
`or sometimes false clues. They trap the user and thwart the normal
`process of
`interpretation and understanding. Alas, poor design
`predominates. The result is a world filled with frustration, with objects
`that cannot be understood, with devices that lead to error. This book
`is an attempt to change things.
`
`3‘
`
`'1
`
`$3
`
`0‘1
`
`The Frustrations
`of Everyday Life
`
`If I were placed in the cockpit of a modern jet airliner, my inability to
`perform gracefully and smoothly would neither surprise nor bother me.
`But I shouldn't have trouble with doors and switches, water faucets
`and stoves. "Doors?” I can hear the reader saying, ”you have trouble
`
`2
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`
`
`opening doors?” Yes. I push doors that are meant to be pulled, pull
`doors that should be pushed, and walk into doors that should be slid.
`Moreover, I see others having the same troubles—unnecessary trou-
`bles. There are psychological principles that can be followed to make
`these things understandable and usable.
`Consider the door. There is not much you can do to a door: you can
`open it or shut it. Suppose you are in an office building, walking down
`a corridor. You come to a door. In which direction does it open? Should
`you pull or push, on the left or the right? Maybe the door slides. If so,
`in which direction? 1 have seen doors that slide up into the ceiling. A
`door poses only two essential questions: In which direction does it
`move? On which side should one work it? The answers should be given
`by the design, without any need for words or symbols, ce ainly with-
`out any need for trial and error.
`K 6".)"an“
`9‘54
`3°"
`A friend told me of the time he got trapped in the doorway ofa post
`ofice in a European city. The entrance was an impasingrow ofperhaps
`six glass swing'ng doors, followed immediately by a second, identical
`row. That’s a standard design: it helps reduce the airflow and thus
`maintain the indoor temperature of the building.
`My friend pushed on the side of one of the lefmost pair of outer
`doors. It swung inward, and he entered the building. Then, before he
`couldget to the next row ofdoors, he was distracted and tamed around
`for an instant. He didn ’t realize it at the time, buthe had moved slightly
`to the right. So when he came to the next door and pushed it, nothing
`happened. ”Hmrn, ” he thought, ”must be locked. ”50 he pushed the
`side of the adjacent door. Nothing. Puzzled, my friend decided to go
`outside again. He turned around andpushed against the side ofa door.
`Nothing. He pushed the adjacent door. Nothing. The door he hadjust
`entered no longer worked He turned around once more and tried the
`inside doors again. Nothing. Concem, then mildpanic. He was trapped!
`just then, a group ofpeople on the other side of the entranceway (to
`my friend’s right) passed easily through both sets of doors. My friend
`hurried over to follow their path.
`How could such a thing happen? A swinging door has two sides.
`One contains the supporting pillar and the hinge, the other is unsup-
`ported. To open the door, you must push on the unsupported edge. If
`you push on the hinge side, nothing happens. In this case, the designer
`aimed for beauty, not uu'lity. No distracting lines, no visible pillars, no
`visible hinges. So how can the ordinary user know which side to push
`
`ONE: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`5
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`1.2 A Row of Swinging Glass Doors in a Boston Hotel. A similar problem to
`the doors from that European post office. On which side of the door should you
`push? When I asked people who had just used the doors, most couldn't say. Yet
`only a few of the people I watched had trouble with the doors. The designers had
`incorporated a subtle clue into the design. Note that the horizontal bars are not
`centered: they are a bit closer together on the sides you should push on. The design
`almost works—but not entirely, for not everyone used the doors right on the first
`try.
`
`on? Mile distracted, my friend had moved toward the (invisible)
`supporting pillar, so he was pushing the doors on the hinged side. No
`wonder nothing happened. Pretty doors. Elegant. Probably won a de-
`sign prize.
`
`The door story illustrates one of the most important principles of
`designm The correct parts must be visible, and they must con-
`vey the correct message. With doors that push, the designer must
`provide signals that naturally indicate where to push. These need not
`destroy the aesthetics. Put a vertical plate on the side to be pushed,
`nothing on the other. Or make the supporting pillars visible. The
`vertical plate and supporting pillars are natural signals, naturally inter—
`preted, without any need to be conscious of them. 1 call the use of
`natural signals natural design and elaborate on the approach throughout
`this book.
`
`Visibility problems come in many forms. My friend, trapped be-
`tween the glass doors, suffered from a lack of clues that would indicate
`what part of a door should be operated. Other problems concern the
`; J
`‘
`between what you want to do and what appears to be possible,
`another topic that will be expanded upon throughout the book. Con-
`sider one type of slide projector. This projector has a single button to
`control whether the slide tray moves forward or backward. One button
`to do two things? What is the mapping? How can you figure out how
`to control the slides? You can’t. Nothing is visible to give the slightest
`hint. Here is what happened to me in one of the many unfamiliar places
`I’ve lectured in during my travels as a professor:
`
`Hie Leitz slide projector illustrated in figure 1.} has shown up sev—
`eral times in my travels. The first time, it led to a rather dramatic
`inddent. A consa'entious student was in charge of showing my slides.
`I started my talk and showed the first slide. When I finished with the
`first slide and asked for the next, the student carefully pushed the
`control button and watched in dismay as the tray backed up, slid out
`of the projector and plopped off the table onto the floor, spilling its
`entire contents. We had to delay the lecture fifteen minutes while I
`struggled to reorganize the slides. It wasn’t the student’s fault. It was
`the fault of the elegant projector. With only one button to control the
`slide advance, how could one switch from forward to reverse? Neither
`of us could figure out how to make the control work.
`All during the lecture the slides would sometimes go forward, some-
`times backward. Afterward, we found the local technician, who ex-
`plained it to us. A brief push of the button and the slide would go
`
`Taste (7) fiir Diawechsel am Gerat
`Diawechsel vorwarts = kurz driicken,
`Diawechsel riickw'a'rtz = linger driicken.
`
`
`
`Button (7) for changing the slides
`Slide change forward = short press,
`Slide change backward = longer press.
`
`Slide
`Pravodit
`1.3 Leitz
`I
`finally tracked
`Projector.
`down the instruction manual
`for that projector. A photo-
`graph of the projector has its
`parts numbered. The button
`for changing slides is number
`7. The button itself has no la~
`bels. Who could discover this
`operation without the aid of
`the manual? Here is the entire
`text related to the button,
`in
`the original German and in my
`English translation:
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`ONE: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`5
`
`6
`
`

`

`
`
`
`forward, a long push and it would reverse. (Pity the conscientious
`student who kept pushing it hard—and long—to make sure that the
`switch was making contact.) What an elegant design. Why, it managed
`to do two functions with only one button! But liow was a first-tine
`user of the projector to know this?
`.
`
`As another example, consider the beautiful Amphitheatre Louis-
`Laird in the Paris Sorbonne, which is filled with magnificent paintings
`ofgreat figures in French intellectual history. (The mural on the ceiling
`shows lots of naked women floating about a man who is valiantly
`trying to read a book. The painting is right side up only for the lec-
`turer—it is upside down for all the people in the audience.) The room
`is a delight to lecture in, at least until you ask for the projection sa'een
`to be lowered. ”Ah, ” says the professor in charge, who gestures to the
`technician, who runs out of the room, up a short tlight of stairs, and
`out of sight behind a solid wall. The saeen comes down and stops.
`’Wo, no, ” shouts the professor, ”a little bit more.” The screen comes
`down again, this time too much. ”No, no, no!” the professor jumps up
`and down andgestures wildly. It’s a lovelyroom, with lovelypaintings.
`But why can ’t the person who is trying to lower or raise the screen see
`what he is doing?
`
`New telephone systems have proven to be another excellent exam-
`ple of incomprehensible design. No matter where I travel, I can count
`upon finding a particularly bad example.
`
`then I visited Basic Books, the publishers of this book, I noticed a
`new telephone system. I askedpeople how they liked it. The question
`unleashed a torrent of abuse. ”It doesn’t have a hold function,” one
`woman complained bitterly—the same complaintpeople at my univer—
`sity made about their rather dilferent system. In older days, business
`phones always had a button labeled ”hold. ” You couldpush the button
`and hang up the phone without losing the call an yourline. Then you
`could talk to a colleague, orpick up another telephone call, or even pick
`up the call at anotherphone with the same telephone number. A light
`on the hold button indicated when the function was in use. It was an
`in valuable tool for business. Why didn ’t the newphones at Basic Books
`orin my university have a hold function, ifit is so essential? Well, they
`did, even the very instrument the woman was complaining about. But
`there was no easy way to discover the fact, nor to learn how to use it.
`I was visiting the University of[Michigan and I asked about the new
`
`6
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`the Telephones at
`Dial ot
`the University of Michigan.
`These inadequate instructions
`are all
`that most users see.
`(The button labeled ”TAP" at
`the lower
`right
`is used. to
`transfer or pick up calls—it is
`pressed whenever the instruc-
`tion plate says "TAP." The
`light on the lower left comes
`on whenever the telephone
`rings-l
`
` 1.4 Plate Mounted Over the
`
`system there. ”Yechl” was the response, ”and it doesn’t even have a
`hold function!” Here we go again. Mat is going on? The answer is
`simple: first, look at the instructions for hold. At the University of
`[Michigan the phone company provided a little plate that fits over the
`keypad and reminds users of the functions and how to use them. I
`carefully unhooked one of the plates from the telephone and made a
`photocopy (figure 1.4}. Can you understand how to use it? I can ’t.
`There is a ”call hold” operation, but it doesn’t make sense to me, not
`for the application that I just described.
`
`The telephone hold situation illustrates a number of different prob-
`lems. One of them is simply poor instructions, especially a failure to
`relate the new functions to the similarly named functions that people
`already know about. Second, and more serious, is the lack of visibility of
`the operation of the system. The new telephones, for all their added
`sophistication, lack both the hold button and the flashing light of the old
`ones. The hold is signified by an arbitrary action: dialing an arbitrary
`sequence of digits ('8, or '99, or what have you: it varies from one
`phone system to another). Third, there is no visible outcome of the
`operation.
`Devices in the home have developed some related problems: func-
`tions and more functions, controls and more controls. I do not think
`that simple home appliances—stoves, washing machines, audio and
`television sets—should look like Hollywood’s idea of a spaceship con-
`trol room. They already do, much to the consternation of the consumer
`who, often as not, has lost (or cannot understand) the instruction
`
`one: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`7
`
`

`

`manual, so—faced with the bewildering array of controls and dis—
`plays—simply memorizes one or two fixed settings to approximate
`what is desired. The whole purpose of the design is lost.
`\
`
`In England I visited a home with a fancy new Italian washer-drier
`combination, with super-duper mum-symbol controls, all to do every—
`thing you ever wanted to do with the washing and drying of clothes.
`The husband (an engineering psychologist) said he refused to go near
`it. The wife (a physia'an) said she had simply memorized one setting
`and tried to more the rest.
`Someone went to a lot of trouble to create that design. I read the
`instrucb'on manual. That machine took into account everything about
`today’s wide variety of synthetic and natural fabrics. The designers
`worked hard; they really cared. But obviously they had never thought
`of trying it out, or of watching anyone use it.
`If the desiyr was so bad, if the controls were so unusable, why did
`the couple purchase it? If people keep buying poorly designed pro-
`ducts, manufacturers and designers wiU tlu'nk they are doing the right
`dung and continue as usual.
`
`The user needs help. Just the right things have to be visible: to
`indicate what parts operate and how, to indicate how the user is to
`interact with the device. Visibility indicates the mapping between in-
`tended actions and actual operations. Visibility indicates crucial dis-
`tinctions—so that you can tell salt and pepper shakers apart, for exam-
`ple. And visibility of the effects of the operations tells you if the lights
`have turned on properly, if the projection screen has lowered to the
`correct height, or if the refrigerator temperature is adjusted correctly.
`It is lack of visibility that makes so many computer-controlled devices
`so difficult to operate. And it is an excess of visibility that makes the
`gadget-ridden, feature—laden modern audio set or video cassette re-
`corder (VCR) so intimidating.
`
`The Psychology
`of Everyday Things
`
`This book is about the psychology of everyday things. POET empha-
`sizes the understanding of everyday things, things with knobs and
`dials, controls and switches, lights and meters. The instances we have
`just examined demonstrate several principles, including the importance
`
`of visibility, appropriate clues, and feedback of one’s actions. These
`principles constitute a form of psychology—the psychology of how
`people interact with things. A British designer once noted that the
`kinds of materials used in the construction of passenger shelters af-
`fected the way vandals responded. He suggested that there might be
`a psychology of materials.
`
`-
`fit is a)
`”In one case, the reinforced glass used to panel shelters (for railroad
`passengers) erected by British Rail was smashed by vandals as fast as
`it was renewed M’hen the reinforced glass was replaced by plywood
`boarding, however, little further damage occurred, although no extra
`force would have been required to produce it. Thus British Rail
`managed to elevate the desire for defacement to those who could write,
`albeit in somewhat limited terms. Nobody has, as yet, considered
`whether thereis a kind ofpsychology ofmaterials. But on the evidence,
`there could well be!”
`
`There already exists the start of a psychology of materials and of
`things, the stud of affordances of objects. When used in this sense,
`
`thetaw'
`
`W (see figures 1.5 and 1.6). A chair
`affords ("is for") support and, therefore, affords sitting. A chair can also
`be carried. Glass is for seeing through, and for breaking. Wood is
`normally used for solidity, opacity, support, or carving. Flat, porous,
`smooth surfaces are for writing on. So wood is also for writing on.
`Hence the problem for British Rail: when the shelters had glass, van-
`dals smashed it; when they had plywood, vandals wrote on and carved
`it. The planners were trapped by the affordances of their materials.3
`Affordances provide strong clues to the operations of things. Plates
`are for pushing. Knobs are for turning. Slots are for inserting things
`into. Balls are for throwing or bouncing. When affordances are taken
`advantage of, the user knows what to do just by looking: no picture,
`label, or instruction is required. Complex things may require explana-
`tion, but simple things should not. When simple things need pictures,
`labels, or instructions, the design has failed.
`A psychology of causality is also at work as we use everyday things.
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`ONE: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`8
`
`

`

`
`
`1.5 Affordances of Doors. Door hardware can signal whether to push or pull
`without signs. The flat horizontal bar of A (above left) affords no operations except
`pushing: it is excellent hardware for a door that must be pushed to be opened. The
`door in B (above right) has a different kind of bar on each side, one relatively small
`and vertical to signify a pull, the other relatively large and horizontal to signify a
`push. Both bars support the affordance of grasping: size and position specify
`whether the grasp is used to push or pull—though ambiguously.
`
`1.6 When Affordances Fail. i had to tie a string around my cabinet door to afford
`pulling.
`
`
`
`Something that happens right after an action appears to be caused by
`that action. Touch a computer terminal just when it fails, and you are
`apt to believe that you caused the failure, even though the failure and
`your action were related only by coincidence. Such false causality is the
`basis for much superstition. Many of the peculiar behaviors of people
`using computer systems or complex household appliances result from
`such false coincidences. When an action has no apparent result, you
`may conclude that the action was ineffective. So you repeat it. In earlier
`days, when computer word processors did not always show the results
`of their operations, people would sometimes attempt to change their
`manuscript, but the lack of visible effect from each action would make
`them think that their commands had not been executed, so they would
`repeat the commands, sometimes over and over, to their later astonish-
`ment and regret. It is a poor design that allows either kind of false
`causality to occur.
`
`TWENTY THOUSAND EVERYDAY THINGS
`
`There are an amazing number of everyday things, perhaps twenty
`thousand of them. Are there really that many? Start by looking about
`you. There are light fixtures, bulbs, and sockets; wall plates and screws;
`clocks, watches, and watchbands. There are writing devices (I count
`twelve in front of me, each different in function, color, or style). There
`are clothes, with different functions, openings, and flaps. Notice the
`variety of materials and pieces. Notice the variety of fasteners—but-
`tons, n‘ppers, snaps, laces. Look at all the furniture and food utensils:
`all those details, each serving some function for manufacturability,
`usage, or appearance. Consider the work area: paper clips, scissors, pads
`of paper, magazines, books, bookmarks. In the room I’m working in,
`I counted more than a hundred specialized objects before I tired. Each
`is simple, but each requires its own method of operation, each has to
`be learned, each does its own specialized task, and each has to be
`designed separately. Furthermore, many of the objects are made of
`many parts. A desk stapler has sixteen parts, a household iron fifteen,
`the simple bathtub-shower combination twenty-three. You can't be—
`lieve these simple objects have so many parts? Here are the eleven basic
`parts to a sink: drain, flange (around the drain), pop—up stopper, basin,
`soap dish, overflow vent, spout, lift rod, fittings, hot-water handle, and
`cold-water handle. We can count even more if we start taking the
`faucets, fittings, and lift rods apart.
`
`10
`
`Hie Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`ONE: The Psychopathology of Everyday Things
`
`‘11
`
`9
`
`

`

`
`
`The book What's M/haf: A Visual Glossary 0/ the Physical World has inore
`than fifteen hundred drawings and pictures and illustrates twenty-
`three thousand items or parts of items.‘ Irving Biederman, a psycholo-
`gist who studies visual perception, estimates that there are probably
`”30,000 readily discriminable objects for the adult.’.’5 Whatever the
`exact number, it is clear that the difficulties of everyday life are ampli-
`fied by the sheer profusion of items. Suppose that each everyday thing
`takes only one minute to learn; learning 20,000 of them occupies
`20,000 minutes—333 hours or about 8 forty-hour work weeks. Fur-
`thermore, we often encounter new objects unexpectedly, when we are
`really concerned with something else. We are confused and distracted,
`and what ought to be a simple, effortless, everyday thing interferes
`with the important task of the moment.
`How do people cope? Part of the answer lies in the way the mind
`works—in the psychology of human thought and cognition. Part lies
`in the information available from the appearance of the objects—the
`psychology of everyday things. And part comes from the ability of the
`designer to make the operation clear, to project a good image of the
`operation, and to take advantage of other things people might be ex-
`pected to know. Here is where the designer’s knowledge of the psy-
`chology of people coupled with knowledge of how things work
`becomes crucial.
`
`
`
`1.7 Carelman's Tandem ”Convergent Bicycle (Model for Fiancés).” Jacques
`Carelman: ”Convergent Bicycle" Copyright © 1969-76—80 by Jacques Carelman
`and A. D. A. G. P. Paris. From lacques Carelman, Cnlalog o/Unfindablt Objects, Balland,
`éditeur, Paris-France. Used by permission of the artist.
`
`the scissors still work. You can figure out the scissors because their
`operating parts are visible and the implications clear. The conceptual
`model is made obvious, and there is efiective use of affordances and
`constraints.
`
`As a counterexample, consider the digital watch, one with two to
`four push buttons on the front or side. What are those push buttons
`for? How would you set the time? There is no way to tell—no evident
`relationship between the operating controls and the functions, no con-
`straints, no apparent mappings. With the scissors, moving the handle
`makes the blades move. The watch and the Leitz slide projector provide
`no visible relationship between the buttons and the possible actions,
`no discernible relationship between the actions and the end result.
`
`Principles of Design
`for Understandability and Usability
`
`We have now encountered the fundamental principles of designing
`for people: (1) provide a good conceptual model and (2) make things
`visible.
`
`PROVIDE A GOOD CONCEPTUAL MODEL
`
`WW
`
`ithout a good model we operate by rote, blindly; we do operations
`as we were told to do them; we can’t fully appreciate why, what effects
`to expect, or what to do if things go wrong. As long as things work
`properly, we can manage. When things go wrong, however, or when
`
`r,
`
`x-j
`l he)”.
`97
`
`ONE: The Psychopathology of Everyday firings
`
`13
`
`Consider the rather strange bicycle illustrated in figure 1.7. You know
`it won’t work because you formW
`mYou can do the simulation because the
`parts are visible and the implications clear.
`Other clues to how things work come from their visible structure—
`in particular from aflordunm, mnshaints, and mappings. Consider a pair of
`scissors: even if you have never seen or used them before, you can see
`that the number of possible actions is limited. The holes are clearly
`there to put something into, and the only logical things that will fit are
`fingers. The holes are affordances: they allow the the fingers to be
`inserted. The sizes of the holes provide” to limit the possible
`fingers: the big hole suggests several fingers, the small hole only one.
`The mapping between holes and fingers—the set of possible opera-
`tions—is suggested and constrained by the holes. Moreover, the opera-
`tion is not sensitive to finger placement: if you use the wrong fingers,
`
`12
`
`The Psychology of Everyday firings
`
`10
`
`10
`
`

`

`we come upon a novel situation, then we need a deeper understanding,
`a good model.
`For everyday things, conceptual models need 'not be' very complex.
`After all, scissors, pens, and light switches are pretty simple devices.
`There is no need to understand the underlying physics or chemistry of
`each device we own, simply the relationship between the controls and
`the outcomes. When the model presented to us is inadequate or wrong
`(or, worse, nonexistent), we can have difliculties. Let me tell you about
`my refrigerator.
`
`
`
` My house has an ordinary, two-compartment refn’gerator—nothing
`rnesn FOOD
`mm”
`COOLING UNIT
`
`
`very fancy about it. The problem is that I can ’t set the temperature
`properly. Hiere are only two things to do: adjust the temperature ofthe
`freezer compartment and adjust the temperature of the fresh food
`5! compartment. And there are two controls, one labeled ”freezer,” the
`Vi
`other ”fresh food. ” What’s the problem?
`You try it. Figure 1.8 shows the instruction plate from inside the
`refrigerator. Now, suppose thefreezeris too cold, thefresh foodsection
`justright. You want to make the fieezer warmer, keeping the fresh food
`constant. Co on, read the thstmctions, figure them out.
`
`K
`Q8.
`
`1-9 ‘1'“, (3011091)“!!! Modfls to: MY Refflsmmh The model A (above) is
`provrded by the system image of the refrigerator as gleaned from the controls and
`instructions; 8 (below) is the correct conceptual model. The problem is that it is
`impossible to tell in which compartment the thermostat is located and whether the
`two controls are in the freezer and fresh food comp
`ent or vice versa
`
`1.8 My Refrigerator. Two compartments—fresh food and freezer—and two con-
`trols (in the fresh food unit). The illustration shows the controls and instructions.
`Your task: Suppose the freezer is too cold, the fresh food section just right How
`would you adjust the controls so as to make the freezer warmer and keep the fresh
`food the same.7 (From Norman, 1986.)
`
`NORMAL SETTINGS
`COLDER FRESH FOOD
`COLDEST FRESH FOOD
`COLDER FREEZER
`WARNER FRESH FOOD
`OFF (FRESH FD & FHZ)
`
`1 SE? BOTH OONmLS
`2 ALLOW 24 HOURS
`TO STABILIZE
`
`COOLING UNIT
`
`
`
`14
`
`The Psychology of Everyday Things
`
`ONE 7779 PsychOPatholoxy of Everyday Things
`
`15
`
`11
`
`11
`
`

`

`Oh, perhaps I’d better warn you. The two controls are not indepen-
`dent. The freezer control afi'ects the fresh food temperature, and the
`fresh food control aliects the freezer. And don ’t forget to wait twenty-
`four hours to check on whether you made the n'ghtadiustment, ifyou
`can remember what you did.
`
`Control of the refrigerator is made difficult because the manufac-
`turer provides a false conceptual model. There are two compartments
`and two controls. The setup clearly and unambiguously provides a
`simple model for the user: each control is responsible for the tempera-
`ture of the compartment that carries its name. Wrong. In fact, there is
`only one thermostat and only one cooling mechanism. One control
`adjusts the thermostat setting, the other the relative proportion of cold
`air sent to each of the two compartments of the refrigerator. This is
`why the two controls interact. With the conceptual model provided by
`the manufacturer, adjusting the temperatures is almost impossible and
`always frustrating. Given the correct model, life would be much easier
`(figure 1.9).
`7
`Why did the manufacturer present the wrong conceptual model.
`
`1.10 Conceptual Models. The design model is the designer's conceptual model. The
`user’s model is the mental model developed through interaction with the system. The
`system image results from the physical structure that has been built (including docu-
`mentation, instructions, and labels). The designer expects the user’s model to be
`identical to the design model. But the designer doesn't talk directly with the
`user—all communication takes place through the system image. If the system
`image does not make the design model clear and consistent, then the user will end
`up with the wrong mental model. (From Norman, 1986.)
`
`
`
`Perhaps the designers thought the correct model was too complex, that
`the model they were giving was easier to understand. But with the
`wrong conceptual model, it is impossible to set the controls. And even
`though I am convinced I now know the correct model, I still cannot
`accurately adjust the temperatures because the refrigerator design
`makes it impossible for me to discover which control is for the thermo-
`stat, which control is for the relative proportion of cold air, and in
`which compartment the thermostat is located. The lack of immediate
`feedback for the actions does not help: with a delay of twenty-four
`hours, who can remember what was tried?
`The topic of conceptual models will reappear in the book. They are
`part of an important concept in design“ the models people
`have of themselves, others, the environment, and the things with
`which they interact. People form mental models through experience,
`training, and instruction. The mental model of a device is formed
`largely by interpreting its perceived actions and its visible structure. I
`call the visible part of the device the system image (figure 1.10). When
`the system image is incoherent or inappropriate, as in the case of the
`refrigerator, then the user cannot easily use the device. If it is inc
`-
`
`30" , w 4 4,
`plete or contradictory, there will be trouble.
`9/" \v‘
`’0'"
`(\w RS W"
`‘ ”Luella, w"
`x”)
`MAKE THINGS VISIBLE W'
`'
`I
`L
`Va; A
`.
`:1 \m“
`\,.
`A.
`.i.
`l
`'
`.
`..'~'
`,y
`‘~
`The problems caused by inadequate attention to visibility are all neatly
`demonstrated with one simple appliance: the modern telephone.
`
`1 stand at the blackboard in my ofiice, talking with a student, when
`my telephone rings. Once, twice it rings. Ipause, trying to complete my
`sentence before answering. The ringing stops. ”I’m sorry,” says the
`student. ”Not your fault, ” I say. "But it’s no problem, the call now
`transfers to my secretary’s phone. She’ll answer it. ” As we listen we
`we
`hear her phone start to ring. Once,
`twice. I look at my watch. Six
`wt“
`o’clock: it’s late, the ofi‘ice staff has left for the day. l rush out of my »
`office to my secretary’s phone, but as Iget there, it stops ringing. ”Ah, ”
`I think, ”it’s being transferred to another phone. ” Sure enough, the
`phone in the adjacent oli‘ice now starts ringing Irush to that office, but
`it is locked. Back to my ofiice to get the key, out to the locked door,
`fumble with the lock, into the ofice, and to the now quietphone. Ihear
`a teleph

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket