throbber
VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0001
`
`TCL 1023, Page 1
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 1
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0001
`
`

`

`
` nu| !il
`
`Camegie Library of Pittsburgh
`
`is a free public brary, maintoined by the
`City of Pittsburgh and the County of Alle-
`gheny, with
`supplemental
`opproprictions
`from the State of Pennsylvania
`
`495
`| ok87
`f 19865
`
`(1)
`
`TCL 1023, Page 2
`
`Carnegie Library of Pittsburgh
`4400 Forbes Ave.
`Tel. 622-3131
`
`FINES: Five cents o day on juvenile ond adult cords for
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 2
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0002
`
`

`

`rae
`ey
`
`
`
`HAZEL ROSSOTTI
`COLOUR
`
`—
`
`Princeton University Press
`Princeton, New Jersey
`
`TCL 1023, Page 3
`
`
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 3
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0003
`
`

`

`Published by Ponccton Liniversity Press,
`4! William Street, Princeton, New Jersey 08540:
`
`Copyright © 1983 by Hazel Rossotti
`All nghts reserved
`First Pelican original edition, 1983
`First Princeton Paperback printing, with corrections, 1985
`
`LOC 84-1145]
`ISBN 0-691 -08369-%
`ISBN 0-691 - 02386 -7 (pbk.)
`
`Reprinted by arrangementwith Penguin Books Ltd,
`Made and printed in Great Britain
`by Richard Clay (The Chaucer Fress) Lid,
`Bungay, Suffolk
`Seain VIP Times
`
`Clothbound editions ofPrinceton University Press books are printed
`on acid-free paper, and binding materials are chosen for strength and durabilily.
`Paperbacks, while satisfactory for personal collections, are not usually suitable
`for library rebinding.
`
`
`
`CONTENTS
`
`List of Text Figures
`Foreword
`Preface to the Princeton Edition
`Introduction
`
`Part One: Light and Dark
`
`hoe
`
`. Light Particles
`. White Light on Clear Glass
`
`Part Two: Lights and Colours
`
`Ata&&
`
`2o~)
`
`. Steady Colours
`, Shimmering Colours
`. Special Effects
`. Lights
`
`Part Three: The Natural World
`
`. Air and Water
`. Earth and Fire
`. Vegetable Colours
`. The Colours of Animals
`
`Part Four: Sensations of Colour
`
`11.
`12.
`13.
`14.
`13:
`
`Light and the Eye
`Anomalous Colour Vision
`Colour Vision in Animals
`The Eye and the Brain
`Sorting and Recording
`
`Part Five: Technology
`
`16.
`17.
`
`Colour Reproduction
`Added Colour
`
`11
`12
`13
`
`19
`26
`
`37
`
`48
`55
`
`65
`77
`84
`91
`
`109
`122
`126
`130
`143
`
`169
`185
`
`TCL 1023, Page 4
`
`
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 4
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0004
`
`

`

`
`
`Contents
`
`Part Six: Uses and Links
`
`18.
`19,
`20.
`ok.
`
`Imparting Information
`Communicating Feelings
`Colour, Music and Movement
`Words and Colours
`
`Index
`
`Acknowledgements
`
`203
`209
`220
`ee di
`
`231
`
`239
`
`LIST OF TEXT FIGURES
`
`OoOo~]ohUroetHhome
`
`. Light waves
`. Prismatic colours
`. A home-made prism
`. From a rose window
`. How light travels through glass
`. How light bounces off glass
`. The dark eyes of houses
`. A single atom of matter
`. Glass
`. A metal
`. Interference of light
`. Printer’s blue and blood red
`. Oil patches and soap bubbles
`. Diffraction by twoslits
`. Diffraction by a grating
`. Polarization oflight
`. Three combs
`. A highly ordered fluid
`. Light sources
`. Laser light
`. Colours from white light
`. White light and tiny particles
`. Distant blue
`. White light and droplets
`. Reflected colour in clouds
`. The Blue Grotto
`. Sunset
`. Colours from a dewdrop
`. A pomary rainbow
`. A double rainbow
`. The Brocken spectre
`. How a glory occurs
`. Shiny graphite
`
`21
`
`23
`24
`27
`28
`29
`31
`32
`33
`33
`41
`46
`49
`50
`51
`52
`53
`57
`
`61
`65
`66
`67
`68
`69
`69
`71
`72
`72
`74
`75
`79
`82
`
`TCL 1023, Page 5
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 5
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0005
`
`

`

`
`
`List of Text Figures
`
`» Leaf green
`. Carrot orange
`- Delphinium biue
`38. The colourful juice of pickled red cabbage
`. Butterfly wings
`. Insect iridescence
`. Blue birds
`. How animals change colour
`. The human eye
`. A light twist
`. Changesin the retina
`. Brightness by night
`- Brightness by day
`. Cones and colour: a possible mechanism
`. Blue-green ambiguity
`- A bee’s-eye view?
`. Colours which recede or advance
`. The corner of the eye
`; Connections for contrast
`. Stereo
`. Brain waves
`, Coloured tops
`. Sidney Harry's top
`. Colours in space
`. Ostwald’s coloursolid
`. Munsell’s tree
`. Comparison of saturation
`» Mixing lights
`, Labelling with filters
`: Tops for colour measurement
`- A good match?
`. Maxwell's triangle
`. ‘Negative’ colours
`. CIE tongue diagram
`. Purple and white
`. Dominant wavelength
`. Colours and complementaries
`. Brightness
`73.
`Some common colours
`74,
`Colour and temperature
`75.
`Theflight of colours
`76.
`Colourtelevision
`
`153
`155
`156
`157
`159
`161
`163
`
`165
`166
`173
`
`List of Text Figures
`
`77
`78
`79
`80
`81
`82
`83
`
`. Phosphordots
`. Colour negatives and prints
`. Code for resistors
`. Coloured signals
`. Evolution of colour words
`. Heraldic tinctures
`. Names for colours
`
`175
`178
`206
`208
`223
`225
`226
`
`TCL 1023, Page 6
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 6
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0006
`
`

`

`
`
`142
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`15
`
`SORTING AND RECORDING
`
`him from the donkey at that particular moment than to the average colour
`which the donkey would have if viewed by a laymanin diffuse light.
`Itis not surprising that the ability to look analytically can be cultivated by
`willpower and training; nor that those without training often prefer a
`naturalistic picture which represents colours as they remember them to
`those in which theartist tries to reproduce the actual light coming from the
`object. In much the same way, an observer will often select, as the most
`‘natural’ colour photograph, one in which the colours are actually brighter
`than those in the origina! scene, Colours selected to match memories from
`dreams, however, are often paler than those of similar objects we observe
`when awake.
`Colour sensations may often be even more dramatically altered by, or
`even produced by, factors other than light. Some forms of hysteria and
`hallucinogenic drugs enhance appreciation of colour: hallucinogens often
`provide experience of brightly coloured patterns which bearlittle relation
`to the real world, and those about to suffer attacks of epilepsy or migraine
`may see coloured rays and geometrical shapes before an attack. The blind,
`particularly if they are old, may have similar sensations, akin to seeing
`‘golden rain’ and coloured patterns. In childhood, a pressure on a closed
`eye was enough to produce patterns as exotic as Catherine wheels or
`peacock tails. In adult life, a firmer touch, preferably on the upper part of
`the eye, is needed to produce an inferior but none the less impressive result.
`Electrical and mechanical stimulation ofthe optic nerve and the visual areas
`of the brain can make us see colours; socan some acuteillnesses, andevena
`strong magnetic field. And so, of course, can memofy and dreams. But
`although we can experience an immense varicty of colour sensations pro-
`duced in these different, non-visual, ways, we have as yet only a negligible
`understanding of any of the mechanisms involved.
`
`‘To what extent can we record a colour? Can we impose any order on our
`rich vanety of colour sensations? If we can, would our scheme be entirely
`personal, or could we use it to communicate information about a colour?
`Howcan webesttell someone the exact colour we should like the new door
`to be?
`As we shall see in Chapter 21, there are manydifficulties in trying to
`describe colours with words. A request to paint the door turquoise would be
`likely to produce a fairly bright door in the greenish-blue (or might it be
`bluish-green?} range, neithervery pastel nor very murky. Perhaps we could
`get nearer to the colour we have in mind by a request to paint the wall the
`same colour as the curtains. But, though the two may s¢em a good match in
`one light, they may clash horribly in another (see page 152). And as they
`will have different textures, they will have different highlights; so although
`the colours of the two may ‘go’ very satisfactorily, they will certainly not
`match all over, even if the light falling on them happensto be identical. It is
`safer to choose from samplesof the actual paint, on the manufacturer's own
`colour card. But maybe the exact colour required is not available. ‘Please
`mix me...’ How does one continue? ‘Something between these two’;
`“Something like this, only lighter’; ‘A more subtle shade ofthat’; ‘A bluer
`version of this one.’ It is difficult to know howto specify, or how to produce,
`the colour required. And it may even be difficult to envisage precisely any
`colours which are not on the colour card.
`Perhapsit would help if we could arrange colours in somerational oruer
`and label them appropriately. We could then refer to them in much the
`same way as we can pinpoint a place by a map reference. Many child-hours
`must be passed in just this way, rearranging crayons, pastels or embroidery
`threads.It is easy enough to makealine of the rainbow colours, and join the
`TCL 1023, Page 7
`ends, through purple, to give a circle. But problems soon arise. Do black,
`grey and white count as colours? If they are to be included, where should
`they go? What should be done with pale colours: primrose, duck-egg blue,
`salmon? And what about the browns? One can imagine a cross-roads at
`yellow, with primrose leading to white on one side, and ochre leading to
`khaki, brown and black on the other. Perhaps, however, black and white
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 7
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0007
`
`

`

`LOWES 1023, Page 8
`
`brightness (or value). The so-called ‘natural’ or ‘achromatic’ colours, black,
`the firsi. It seems we cannot arrange our coloured objects onaflat surface,
`but need some three-dimensional scheme.
`grey and white, are of zero saturation, and differ from each other only in
`Maybe it would be better to seek a more‘scientific’ classification than
`brightness. A series obtained by adding one hue, say blue, to white differs
`any subjective arrangement of coloured materials? We might try to specify
`only in saturation, as does a series obtained by adding a blue pigmentto a
`the colour of a sample by irradiating it with the light of a large number of
`grey one. If the same pigment were added to a white one in the same
`very narrow bands of wavelength and measuring the percentage of each
`proportions, the dusky, pale blue would differ from the clear pale blue only
`which the sample reflects. These measurements can be made extremely
`in brightness.
`easily, given the appropriate equipment. But the light which enters the eye
`There are many three-dimensional arrangements of colours, the best
`dependson the lighting as well as on the sample, so we would also need to
`known being those devised by Munsell and by Ostwald. Both are based on
`know the composition of the illumination. Even this does not tell us the
`the colour circle formed by joining the two ends of the spectrum through
`colour of the sample unless we know how the eye reacts to light of different
`purple (see Figure 58}. So the hue changes around the circumference of the
`wavelengths. Two materials may match exactly under one type of ililumina-
`circle, much as the hours progress around the face of a clock. Through the
`tion even if they sendlight oftotally different composition to the eye; we
`centre of the clock face. and perpendicularto it, like the axle of a wheel,
`know that many yellows can be matched by mixtures of red and greenlight.
`runsthe line representing the neutral colours, usually with white at the top,
`We might, however, combine, for each narraw band of wavelength,
`changing, through deepening greys, to black at the bottom. Radially, like
`measurements of the reflecting powers of the sample, and the composition
`spokes on a wheel, the saturation increases towardsthe rim,to give a space
`of the light source with our knowledge of the response of the retina.
`which can be filled in by different colours, according to which system is
`Although this procedure still needs laboratory equipment, it relates the
`being used.
`scientific measurements of the light reaching us to our perceptions of
`colour, assuming that the observer has normal colour vision, adapted for
`daylight viewing and uninfluenced by the effects (such as after-images,
`contrast of near-by colours, memory, expectation) which we discussed in
`the previous chapter. So we are attempting to chart,not just the stimulus of
`thelight entering the eye, but a normal observer's response to it. The idea of
`attempting to measure colour in this way soundsattractive, if somewhat
`complex. But do its advantages always outweigh those of map references
`with an ordered arrangement, albeit subjectively chosen and represented in
`three dimensions? Since both systems are usedin practice, we shall look at
`each in more detail.
`If we are to arrange a numberofcolours in any systematic order, we must
`decide what qualities we shall use to sort them. Letusfirst recall the waysin
`which light can vary. The sensation of colour depends primarily on the
`composition of the light, and partly on the intensity; and the composition
`may be usually described as a mixture, in a certain proportion, of whitelight
`with a ‘coloured’ light of a particular dominant wavelength within the
`visible spectrum. (For purple light, the ‘coloured’ componentis itself a
`mixture.) We can describe the primary sensation of colour in termsof hue,
`which refers to the greenness, blueness and so forth, and varies with any
`change in the dominant wavelength. The extent to which this wavelengthin
`fact dominates the light is known as saturation (or chroma). As the domin-
`ant wavelength is diluted with white light, the saturation decreases. An
`increase in intensity in ight of a particular composition increases the
`
`144
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`145
`
`Pale colour
`Murky colour
`
`Vivid colour
`
`Black
`
`Figure 58. Colours in space The skeleton ofa colour solid. The ‘achromatic’ colours
`form the vertical backbonefram which the different hues radiate; red in one direction,
`green in the opposite one, and the others in between. For any one hue, the colour
`becomes more vivid the farther it is fromthe centre.
`
`TCL 1023, Page 8
`
`Ostwald arranged his colours in a double cone, based on twenty-four
`different hues, arranged around the circumference (sce Figure 59). Each
`hue is combined, in a numberof fixed proportions, with each of eight
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 8
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0008
`
`

`

`
`
`Hue (2)
`colourf°(compiementary to {1)}
`
`146
`{a}
`
`Sensations of Colaur
`
`Planes of constant dominant
`wavetength {hue}
`
`Full saturation atedge
`
`ib)
`
` Full
`
`Grey ans
`
`Figure 59, Ostwald’'s colour solid (a) Exterior view, (b} Vertical section.
`(Adapted, with permussion, from G. J. Chamberlin and D, G. Chambertin, Colour: lts
`Measurement, Computation and Appheation, Heyden, London, 1980.)
`equally spaced neutral colours from white to black. The resulting colours
`are arrangedso that brightness decreases vertically towards the bottom of
`the diagram, while saturation decreases towards the centre. Thus Ostwald’s
`colour solid consists of twenty-four triangles (one for each hue), arranged
`radially so that a vertical section through it gives two such triangles, for
`complementary hues, fused at the centre, Each position within the solid is
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`147
`
`numbered ona grid system, so that any colour contained by thesolid can be
`specified by a map-reference.
`In Munseli's arrangement, saturation is increased by a serics of visually
`equal steps rather than by adding a fixed proportion of pigment; and there
`are nine neutral colours, rather than eight. As the numberof equalsteps of
`saturation at a particular hue and brightness depends on the hue, the arms
`are of different length in different parts of the solid. Munsell’s solid is
`therefore much fess regular than Ostwaid’s, and on accountof its untidy
`appearance is known as a colour‘tree’. A typical vertical section through it
`is shown in Figure 60. Each position in the tree, as in Ostwald’s cone, is
`encoded, thereby allowing colours to be specified. And the tree has one
`great advantage over Ostwald’s solid: whenever some new, dazzling pig-
`ment is made, it may be incorporated by extending an existing branch.
`
`Maximal
`
`
`
`saturation Hue (t}
`
`Black
`
`Hue (2)
`(complementaryto (1})
`
`Figure 60. Munsell's tree Vertical section (cf. Figures 39(b), page 146, and 83, page
`226).
`
`The two systems resemble each other in that the circumference is divided
`arbitrarily into hues, and the vertical axis is graduated into visually equal
`steps, which are obtained by asking large numbers of observers to estimate
`equal differences in brightness. But the two solids are based on diffcrent
`ways of varying saturation. While Ostwald used the ratio of neutral pigment
`to saturated pigment, Munsell again invoked visual assessment by the
`average observer.
`When Ostwald devised his colour solid, he was an old man whose sensi-
`tivity to blue was doubtless declining, which accounts for the slight com-
`pression in the blue region of the circumference. Swedish workers have
`attempted to correct this defect in Ostwald’s system by placing each of
`the four psychological primaries at right angles to one another on the
`
`TCL 1023, Page 9
`
`
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 9
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0009
`
`

`

`149
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`
`
`Horzontalacan
`
`
`
`MovingeyepieceM
`
`fe}
`
`{qd
`
`tb)
`
`
`Transparentpiungar——.
`
`FM
`
`=ny
`
`
`
`FixedeyepleceF|
`
`ee
`
`(a)
`
`-
`£ 4
`i 5
`3s
`
`circumference and dividing the segments between them into visually equal
`steps, constituting a more ‘natural’ colour circle for use as the basis of a
`three-dimensional colour solid.
`Although a colour solid is a useful concept, and may even be constructed
`asa display object of great visual and intellectual appeal, either swatches or
`books are more convenient for everyday use. Colour atlases, such as the
`Munseil Book of Colour, often represent vertical sections of a colour solid,
`cut through cach hue represented. For more specialist use, a restricted
`Tange of colours, varying by smaller gradations, may be reproduced as in
`collections for those who wishto specify the precise colourof arose petal or
`a sample of human skin or tooth.
`Such visual matching of colour is a stepwise process, a placing of the
`sample of unknown specifications between two standard colours whose
`specifications are known. But how can wetry to measure the colourspecifi-
`cations of a material if we have no standard colour which matches it? What
`can we do to try to measure ‘colour’, to produce specifications of hue,
`saturation and brightness?
`Since colour is a sensation, there is a lot to be said for the measurements
`being made by the eye. The humaneyeis, in fact, an excellent detector of
`differences of hue, and many people can assess the percentageof red, blue
`and yellow in a pigment with surprising precision. But human estimates of
`saturation, and of brightness, are much less reliable.
`The most precise visual methodsof attempting to specify colours,like the
`use of a colour atlas, involve matching. The simplest are those devised
`merely to measure saturation, as in the determination of the concentration
`of a single coloured componentin a liquid (see Figure 61).
`If the two solutions appear to be the same colour, the ratio of their
`concentrations is simply related to the ratio of the length of the two
`solutions through which the light has passed; and this mayeasily be found
`using a simple comparator involving either a plunger or a wedge.
`
`Figure 61. Comparison of saturation A fixed depth ofthe sample is viewed with one
`eye and a variable length ofstandard with the other. The geometry ofthe instrument is
`adjusted until the same depth of colour is observed by each eye.
`(a) Sampte.
`{b} Variation af depth of standard by means oftransparent plunger.
`
`fe) Variation ofdepth ofstandardby use ofwedge.
`
`(d) Split field, one halffrom each eyepicce.
`in the top two diagrams, the same depth used for sample and standard gives a darker
`field on the right. In the lower diagram, the length (1) ofstandard has been adjusted so
`that the two fields are indistinguishable In this exampie, d =2I, indicating that the
`Saiuration of the sample is half that of the standard.
`
`TCL 1023, Page 10
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 10
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0010
`
`

`

`
`
`150
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`For many purposes, however, we need to know the hue, as well as the
`saturation. We again compare our sample with a colour which we can
`specify. But how can we vary this colour whilststill being able to specify it?
`One way is to mix coloured lights of known wavelength in known pro-
`portions. A simple arrangementis shown in Figure 62. The amountof red,
`blue and green light can be varied by horizontal and vertical movementof
`the filter assembly over the source of light, and the three lights are then
`mixed by diffusion and multiple reflections. More suphisticated devices,
`used mainly for
`research on colour,
`involve six lights of varying
`wavelengths. In each case the mixture of coloured lights, shone on to a
`white background, is matched with the unknown sample,illuminated from
`a standard source of white light.
`Alternatively, the sample may be matched by a patch of colouredlight
`which has been obtained by passing white light through three filters, one
`
`Bive fitter
`
`OOK x
`
`TiSNS35088reeatatet
`Bess
`
`
`
`Green hitter
`
`Red filter
`
`Figure 62. Mixing lights The required mixture of red, blue and green itght may be
`oeenee byadjustingthat areaofcachfilter which lies over theaperture to the mixing
`
`Sorting and Recording
`151
`magenta, oneyellow and one blue-green (see Figure 63). Sets of suchfilters
`are available commercially for use in an instrument equipped with a stan-
`dardlight source and knownas the Lovibond ‘Tintometer. Thefull range of
`250filters of different depth for each of the three huesallows nearly nine
`million different colours to be obtained,
`including the full range of
`achromatic colours from white to black. The colourof the sampleis readily
`specified in terms of the three filters used to match it.
`MBG Y¥
`
`
`StandardENI iUy
`
`Sample
`Figure 63. Labelling withfilters The colour ofthe sample can be matched with that of
`light which has passed through three Lovibondfilters, magenta (M), blue-green (BG)
`and yellow (Y} ofspecified strength. A purplish bluesample, for example, would need
`a deep magenta (to absorb most ofthe green), a medium blue-green (to absorb some,
`bu: not all, ofthe red), and yellow ofappropriate depth to reduce the intensity ofthe
`colour to that of the sample.
`Instead of matching the light which reaches us from a sample with that
`from an unknown, we can exploit the phenomenon of persistence of vision
`and match only the sensations. Split discs, coloured in saturated blue, green
`and red, are placed on a revolving platform in such a way that the pro-
`portions of the three colours can be varied (see Figure 64). When the
`platform is spun at high speed, the sensations merge, just as if three
`coloured lights were superimposed, The areas of the three colours are
`adjusted until the colour of the spinning disc exactly matches that of the
`sample, which can then be expressedin terms of the proportionsof primary
`colours used.
`Nowadays, there is an increasing tendency to use photoelectric instru-
`ments for colour matching, instead of the eye of one or two individual
`observers. Such instruments monitor narrow bands over the whole visible
`
`TCL 1023, Page 11
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 11
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0011
`
`

`

`
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`153
`
`152
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`the sample, and then converting this information into the size of the
`stimulus which bombards eachof the three cone systems in a normal human
`eye. Finally, the responses to these three stimuli are combined to give the
`colour experienced by the ‘standard observer’. Inescapably, these instru-
`ments give results based on sensations experienced as a consequence of
`humanvision. But the vision js that, not of a few individuals, but of the
`‘standard observer’ built up from observations made by a large number of
`individuals selected for their normality ofvision. But even when the match-
`ing is done, rather approximately, by eye, it is seldom left for a single
`individual: more often two, or even three, observers are used.
`
`(a)
`
`(b) ae
`
`
`
`Figure 64. Tops for colour measurement (a) Split circular disc ofpaper ofstandard
`colour. (b} Stavhdardgreen, blue and red interlocked, exposing known areas ofeachfor
`colour mixing when the disc is rotated.
`
`Matching, whether by eye or machine, often gives different resu{ts with
`different sourcesoflight; and as matching implies identity only of response,
`this is no surprise. Two extreme examplesof identical colours produced by
`hght of very different composition were given in Table 3 (page 119).
`Imagine a pair of yellow pigments, onereflecting light of only 580nm and
`one reflecting only light of 540 nm and 630 nm,each at half the intensity of
`the first pigment. If both were illuminated with light containing equal
`intensities of the three wavelengths, the two pigments would match exactly,
`But if they were illuminated with light containing a slightly higher pro-
`portion of longer wavelengths, the second pigment would look redder than
`the first. Such ‘metameric’ pigments, which match only under one light
`source, are the bane of those who try to match clothes and accessories, or
`paint and fabrics (see Figure 65). Metamerism accounts for much of the
`
`whouses dyes such that any changein illumination causes almost the same
`change in colour for the different materials.
`Colour solids and atlases can give us no information about the compost-
`tion of the light which causesa particular colour, For charts relating colour
` 016
`
`Relative
`
`012}
`
`O04
`
`reflectance
`
`ates,
`
`i
`-
`i
`L
`i —
`400
`7
`500
`600
`700
`
`Figure 65. A good match? Thereflectance spectra oftwefabrics which match perfecily
`in daylight. (Reproduced, with permission, from W. D. Wright, The Measurement of
`Colour, 4th edition, Adam Hilger, London, 1969.)
`Green
`
`Wavelength (nim)
`
`
` Unsaturated greenlsh-yellow
`Blue-green
`
`Biue-violel
`Magenta
`Orange-red
`Figure 66. Maxwell's triangle Shows how many, burnorall, colours canbe represented
`as a mixture of three primary coloured lights. The nearer a point is to an apex 0.fthe
`triangle the higheris thepropartionoflight ofthe colour represented by that apex. The
`point X (50 per cent green, 35 per cent orange-red and 15 per cent blue-violet)
`
`TCL 1023, Page 12
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 12
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0012
`
`

`

`
`
`i534
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`lo composition, we turn to the second, more‘scientific’ approach. Just as
`many coloursolids are based on a ring of spectral colours, joined through
`purpie, a triangle usually forms the basis of attempts to chart the colours
`produced by the mixing of lights. As early as 1855, Maxwell found thata
`great numberof colours could be produced by mixing lights of only the
`three ‘primary’ colours: orange-red, green and blue-violet. The colour
`resulting from a particular mixture can be represented by a point on a
`triangular grid (sce Figure 66). Many colourscan be specified in this way:
`but not all. Whichever three primary sources we choose, there are always
`some colours (including many pure spectral ones) which cannot be rep-
`resented by a point in, or on, the triangle; which confirms that we cannot
`always match one colour by a mixture of three others unless we allow
`ourselves the option of mixing one of the primary colours with the sample
`and then matching the result with a mixture of the two otherlights. Figure
`67 gives the recipe for obtaining a match for every visible wavelength with
`three primaries, Thus vivid yellow (570 nm) (cf. page 119} can never be
`exactly matched by red (700 nm) and green (546 nm); but if a little blue
`(436 nm) is added to the yetlow, a perfect match can be made. We can
`express this algebraically by stating that vivid yellaw can be matched by red,
`green and a small negative amountof blue. But since there is no scope for
`plotting negative contributions on a Maxwell triangle, colours such as vivid
`yellow cannot be represented onit.
`It is too bad that we cannot choose any three wavelengths which, when
`themselves mixed together, will produce alf visible colours. But there is
`nothing to stop us imagining that such ideal primary colours might exist;
`‘and if they did, they could be mixed in such a way as to produce three
`convenient heal primaries, such as Maxwell used. So we could draw a
`mathematical modification of Maxwell's triangle, with our three imaginary
`primaries at the corners. Points for the real ones, and for all other colours,
`would then be within it. Three such imaginary primaries have indeed been
`devised, such that any real colours can be represented as a mixture ofthe
`appropriate amounts of the three of them, and plotted as an idealized
`version of the Maxwell triangle. But not everyone is used to triangular
`graphs and the ruled paper may not be easy to obtain. Could we not usé
`squared paper instead? How many variables do we need? Since we have
`decreed that a real colour R can be matched by a mixture of our three
`Imaginary Primary Lights — say, | units of one, P units of the second and Lof
`the third -it might look as though we need the three variables,I, P, and L, to
`specify R. But we could also say that the brightness ofthe light is the sum S$
`of the three primaries (so S=1+P+L), mixed in the ratio of 1/S:P/S:L/S.
`And if we know I/S and P/S we already know L/S, because
`I/S+P/S+L/S=1. Since we are often more interested in colour than in
`
`425
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`
`
`=
`Blue (436)
`
`+40
`
`+05
`
`0
`
`155
`
`These ‘positive’ Colours
`when mixed together
`
`MATCH
`
`mibdure.of pure Batch and
`these ‘negathvd colours
`
`Red (700) 415
`
`
`
`700
`400
`500
`600
`Wavelength of pure patch of fight (nm}
`Figure 67. ‘Negative’ colours Many pure spectral colours can be exactly matched with
`threeprimaries only by use of‘negative’ colours. When oneprimary is mixed with the
`‘pure’ patch oflight, this mixture can be matched by some mixture ofthe other two. For
`theprimary lights used here, the only colour which can be matched directly by the three
`primaries is a greenish yellow in the region of550.nm. (Reproduced, with permission,
`from F. W. Billmeyer and M. Saltzman, Principles of Color Technology, Interscience,
`New York, 1966, p. 33.)
`brightness, we could concentrate on the two quantities [/S and P/S which
`specify the colour. We could then use ordinary squared graph paper. lf we
`ever needed to know L/S,it would be easy enoughto calculateit. Andif we
`decided that, after all, we wanted to specify the brightness, we could
`representit on an axis rising vertically, oul of the paper. The final diagram
`would be much like a plan, with the two specifications of colour running
`north-south and east-west: of,if brightness is added, like a map which also
`shows heights, or isobars (or any third variable) superimposed onthe plan
`as a series of contours.
`a
`So all that we need in order to specify the colour of an object is a
`knowledge of the composttion of the light falling on it, of the modification
`
`TCL 1023, Page 13
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 13
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0013
`
`

`

`
`
`156
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`of the light by it, of the response of the normal humaneye, and of the
`quantities of three imaginary primary lights which would produce the same
`response; and a piece of ordinary graph paper. The Commission Inter-
`nationale de l' Eclairage (C1E), in 1931, defined the standard observer and
`three possible standard sources; and they produced tables showing the
`relationship between the observers’ response and the quantities of the
`imaginary primary tights which would in theory be needed to produce them.
`The tongue-shaped curve (see Figure 68) in the graph showsthe specifica-
`100% primary P
`‘ideal greary
`
`
`
`P
`
`157
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`
`
`Figure 49. Purple and white The point £ for p =0-33, i =0-33 (and sal =k- 0-33) is
`‘equal energy’ white. Purples lie along the base line (see text}.
`
`100%primaryL
`raa
`Figure 68. CIE tongue diagram The tougue enclosesall visible colours, with thepure
`spectral ones lying along its curved edge. The inner triangle encloses those colours
`obtainable by mixing real primaries 436 nm, 546m, 700-nm (i.e. those enclosed in
`Maxwell's triangle of Figure 66, page 153).
`
`tions of the pure spectral colours as their values of i=1/S and p=P/S.
`(Modified definitions of the standard observer and the standard sources,
`eB introduced by the CIE in 1967, change only-details on the graph.) The
`100%primary| ane?
`diagram, known as a CIE chromaticity curve, has been used to depict
`colours, and the relationship between them, in a wide variety of situations.
`The enormous ceeress rf the ar diagrams percee ye
`aecan repressDud mixture of
`two lg us r pie
`vac :
`H3
`points which specily them. Sa purples, formed by mixing red and blue, he on
`the base-line of the ‘tongue’ (see Figure 69). A mixture of two parts red
`
`TCL 1023, Page 14
`
`LOWES 1023, Page 14
`
`VIZIO Ex. 1023 Page 0014
`
`

`

`
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`139
`
`o8
`
`C6
`
`Od ~
`
`158
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`(770.nm) and onepart violet (380 nm) would lie at point X, twice as near to
`the red point as to the violet point. Since all the colours formed by mixing
`real lights tie inside the area enc

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket