throbber
Behavior and Motor Activity Response
`in Hyperactive Children and
`Plasma Amphetamine Levels Following
`a Sustained Release Preparation
`
`Gerald L. Brown, M .D., Michael H. Ebert, M.D.,
`Edwin j. Mikkelsen, M.D., and Robert D. Hunt, M.D.
`
`Abstract. Amphetamine has been clearly documented to be an efficacious treatment for
`tablet, and sustained-release
`hyperactive children. Recently, pharmacokinetics of elixir,
`pr ep arations have been studied in hyperactive children. Sustained release has been
`thought to prov ide a prolonged clinical response. In this study , nin e hyperactive children,
`selected by specific exclusion-inclusion criteria, were admi nistered single oral do ses of
`sustained -release d-amphetamine and placebo ; plasma levels, beh avioral response, and mo(cid:173)
`tor activity were observed in double-blind design . The results, as with the earlier studies, in(cid:173)
`d icate that significant clinical response is not observed beyond 4 hou rs and that responses
`occur onl y du ring the abso rptio n phas e and are not correlate d with specific plasma levels of
`d- amphetarnine.
`
`J ournal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 19:225-239, 1980
`
`Amphetamines have been used for over 40 years (Bradley, 1937) to
`treat child ren with aggressive , impulsive behavioral disturbances.
`Double-blind placebo-controlled studies of the effects of d-am(cid:173)
`phetamine on hyperactive children (HAC) have confir med its ef(cid:173)
`ficacy (Greenberg et al., 1972; Arnold et al., 1972; Conners et al.,
`
`Dr. Brown is in the Biological Psychiatry Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda,
`Maryland. Dr. Ebert is in the Laboratory of Clinical Science, Nat ional Institute of Mental Health,
`Bethesda, Maryland . Drs. Mikk elsen and Hunt are currently in the Child Study Center, Yale Univer(cid:173)
`sity, New Haven, Connecticut; f ormerly, Biological Psychiatry Branch and Laboratory of Clinical
`Science, NIMH, Bethesda, Maryland .
`R eprints may be requestedfro m Dr. Gerald L. Brown, Nat ional Institutes of Health, Clinical Center,
`Room 3N204, 9000 Rockville Pike, Bethesda, MD 20205.
`Acknowledgment: Assistance in analysis of data, Marcia D. M inichiello, R esearch Assistant, and
`Kathleen j. Powers, Laboratory Assistant.
`0002-7138/80/1902-0225 $0 1.30 Q 1980 Ameri can Academy of Child Psychiatry.
`
`225
`
`Page 1
`
`SHIRE EX. 2065
`KVK v. SHIRE
`IPR2018-00293
`
`

`

`226
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`1972; Huestis et al., 1975 ) and its use as a standard for compari(cid:173)
`son in pharmacotherapeutic trials.
`In normal prepubertal boys,
`Rapoport et al. (1978) ha ve described cogn itive and behavioral
`responses to d-amphetamine similar to th ose observed in HAC.
`Recent studies have reported some of th e pharmacokinetic charac(cid:173)
`teristics oj d-amphetamine in HAC; e .g., apparent elimination
`half-life, X ± SEM, 6.8 ± 0.5 h (Brown et aI., 1977 , 1978, 1979a,
`1979b). Sustained-release d-amphetamine has been shown to be re(cid:173)
`leased at a slower rate than tablets and to ha ve a similar half-life in
`adults and animals (Bec ke tt and Tucker, 1966; Rosenet aI., 1967 ;
`Brown et aI., 1979b); this characteristic ha s been thought to be rel(cid:173)
`evant to attaining a prolonged clinical response in HAC (Wender,
`1971; Gross and Wilson, 1974; Safer and Allen, 1976; Ross and
`Ross, 1976; Cantwell and Carlson, 1978). This study was under(cid:173)
`taken to review pharmacokinetic differences between tablets and
`sustained-release d-amphetamine following single-dose administra(cid:173)
`tion. Clinical responses from tablets and elixir by similar methodol(cid:173)
`ogy also are compared to these results.
`
`METHODS
`
`Male child re n, ages 60 to 144 month s, were evaluated at the Na(cid:173)
`tional Institute of Mental Health (N IH Clinical Center, Bethesda,
`Maryland) for impulsive, maladaptive social behavior, hyperactivity
`and learning disability. We obtained the community teachers'
`ratings off-me d icatio n by using the 39-item Conners'
`behavior
`Teacher Rati ng Scale (CTRS) (1969). Children and families were
`assessed in a preadmission screening. All research methods and
`procedures were a pproved by the NIMH Institutional Review
`Board (IRB) and the NIH Medical Board.
`i.e.,
`Exclusion cr iteria were:
`(1) "hard" neurological findings ,
`clinical seizure di sorder or any other medical disorder (we scored
`all children for neurological "soft signs" using PANESS)
`(Guy,
`1976); (2) borderline psychosis-as determined by Creak (1961)
`and the proposed DSM-III criteria (197 8); and (3) IQ < 80 on
`WISC-R. Inclusion cr iter ia for the study group were two standard
`deviations (SD) or more, above publish ed (Werry et aI., 1975)
`norms for similarly aged boys on Factor I (conduct problem) or
`Factor IV (hy peractivity) of the CTRS by comm unity teachers' rat(cid:173)
`ings; children were also rated by the NIH t eacher. Eight of nine
`child ren had Facto r I scores> 2 SD ; two of nine had Factor IV
`scores > 1 SD < 2 SD-one of th ese had a Factor II (attention)
`
`Page 2
`
`

`

`Behavior Response in Hyperactive Children
`
`227
`
`score> 2 SD and the other> 1 SD < 2 SD. Thus, though Factor I
`behavior is prominent in these children, there were no children
`who showed only this behavior. Each child further received a com(cid:173)
`plete medical and psychiatric examination to confirm hyperactivity
`and rule out other conditions during a 5-day inpatient assessment
`before research procedures were initiated. This study group (N =
`9) may be described (X ± SD) as follows: age, 97 ± 25 mos.; weight,
`28.0 ± 11.8 kg; WISC-R IQ (full scale), 97 ± 7; and family in(cid:173)
`come, $17,278 ± $10,814.
`A low monoamine,
`low xanthine, normal salt diet was main(cid:173)
`tained. The controlled diet was necessary: (1) for urinary metabo(cid:173)
`lite studies not reported in this paper; (2) for minimizing variations
`in urinary pH for pharmacokinetic studies of amphetamine; and
`(3) for a standard state of hydration (Beckett et al., 1969; Rowland,
`1969; Axelrod, 1970). Instructions for this diet were given to par(cid:173)
`ents for weekends and those nights when the child was at home.
`Urinary pH was monitored by Ames Dipstix on days when blood
`samples were obtained for d-amphetamine analyses. The mean
`(± SEM) urinary pH was 6.3 ± 0.4 from an average of 2.7
`determinations/patient (range 5.0-7.0). To minimize the number of
`venipunctures, a heparin-lock and armband were utilized to collect
`serial blood samples. Usually a single venipuncture sufficed for
`eight h, though occasionally more than one venipuncture was nec(cid:173)
`essary either for initiating and/or continuing the procedure. Lunch
`was at a fixed time in relation to blood sampling. A standard
`breakfast (9 g protein, 26 g carbohydrate, 15 g lipid), usually 75%
`consumed, was provided between 8:30 and 9:00 A.M. prior to
`baseline blood at 9:00-9:30 A.M. Placebo and amphetamine were
`given in varying order with at
`least three intervening drug-free
`days. Unlike an earlier study (Brown et al., 1979a), plasma samples
`for norepinephrine (NE) and dopamine-,8-hydroxylase were drawn
`at h 1-3 with small sham drawings at h 4-6, thus allowing a similar
`placebo day in which plasma samples were being obtained. We at(cid:173)
`tempted to keep the dose of d-amphetamine as near as possible
`to 0.5 mg/kg, using 5 mg sustained-release capsules (0.48 ± .01
`mg/kg). Environmental variables during the 6 h following single(cid:173)
`dose administration included dissimilar school material and differ(cid:173)
`ent, numbers of children on the unit. Scheduled activities were
`similar, i.e., 1 h for initiating the study, 2 h in the classroom fol(cid:173)
`lowed by three intervals, each for 1 h, for lunch, occupational
`therapy, and art therapy, respectively.
`Double-blind behavioral ratings by the same research assistant
`
`Page 3
`
`

`

`228
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`using the 10-item abbreviated CTRS (ABCTRS) (Conners, 1973)
`were done on all blood sampling days between 15-30 min following
`the hourly blood drawing. Likewise, on all study days motor
`activity was automatically and continuously recorded with a newly
`developed acceleration-sensitive device (Colburn et al., 1976a,
`1976b). Unlike the initial tablet study (Brown et al., 1979a) with 1 h
`activity summations, but similar to the elixir study (Brown et al.,
`1977), the motor activity summations in the current study were 15
`min at baseline and following each hourly blood sampling. Motor
`activity counts for consecutive 7.5 min intervals were stored in a
`memory cell for periods up to 32 h and read out from a PDP-II
`minicomputer. The interval
`length and sensitivity of this device
`may be adjusted in order to adapt it to the level and kind of move(cid:173)
`ment disorder being studied or to a specific protocol design. An
`impulse generated by a movement is amplified, routed, and stored
`in a time-sequence memory location. The entire apparatus is 4 x 6
`x 1 ern and weighs 75 g. Activity monitors can be calibrated with
`each other. The ambulatory motor activity monitor was worn in a
`vest pocket over the thoracic dorsal area (special vests fitted to
`each child). Motor activity data intervals were taken consistently
`during each hour.
`Levels of d-amphetamine in plasma were determined by a
`radioimmunoassay (RIA). The assay relies on competitive bind(cid:173)
`ing between amphetamine (in the plasma of the subject) and
`radiolabeled amphetamine to an antibody raised to a metham(cid:173)
`phetamine-bovine serum complex. The assay was adapted by Ebert
`et al. (1976) from a technique of Cheng et al. (1973). The antibody
`to methamphetamine does not distinguish amphetamine and
`methamphetamine. Neither does it cross-react significantly with
`the major metabolites formed from amphetamine in man (benzoic
`acid, hippuric acid, parahydroxyamphetamine, norephedrine, or
`parahydroxyephedrine). The midpoint of the standard curve was
`1.6 ng, and the minimal detectable dose was 100 pg. Assays were
`performed directly, in duplicate, with 0.1 cc of plasma. The intra(cid:173)
`assay coefficient of variation is 6.9% and the interassay coefficient
`of variation is 12.3%. In the same laboratory, both RIA and gas
`chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) were used to mea(cid:173)
`sure amphetamine plasma levels; a highly significant multiple cor(cid:173)
`relation coefficient was found (R = 0.98223). RIA samples were
`run in duplicate, while GC-MS samples were run singly. Since
`more error is inherent
`in RIA values, RIA was regressed on
`
`Page 4
`
`

`

`Behavior Response in Hyperactive Children
`
`229
`
`GC-MS. Although GC-MS was 1.9 times more sensitive than RIA,
`for most purposes, the convenience of the RIA method outweighs
`the technical superiority of the GC-MS (Powers and Ebert, 1979).
`
`RESULTS
`
`The results of the CTRS behavior ratings performed by the chil(cid:173)
`dren's community teachers, for whom the scale was intended, are
`shown in table 1. The community teachers' ratings on Factors I, II,
`and IV were generally higher than the NIH teachers' ratings. HAC
`group ratings by community teachers were significantly different
`from norms on Factors I, II, and IV (p < .001); but ratings by
`the NIH teachers were significantly different only for Factor IV
`(p < .01) (two-tailed t-tests). Factor II has been shown to have a
`smaller z score (2.16) than Factors land IV (2.54 and 3.22, respec(cid:173)
`tively) (Werry et al., 1975) in HAC vs. normals and might thus be
`thought to be less sensitive in differentiating HAC from normal
`children. As a further evaluation of the exclusion-inclusion criteria
`used for this study and other studies in this research program, we
`obtained a Spearman rank-order correlation relating community
`and NIH teachers' ratings from 42 children screened for several
`studies. Correlation for both Factors I and IV are significant (Fac(cid:173)
`tor I, rs = 0.43, P < .01; Factor II, r s = -0.04, ns; Factor IV, r, =
`
`Table I
`
`Behavioral Rating Characteristics
`Connors's Teacher Rating Scale Factor Scores (X ± SD)
`
`N H
`
`3 9
`
`43
`
`I
`
`Factor
`II
`
`IV
`
`1.21 (.39)
`
`1.60 (.58)
`
`1.56 (.65)
`
`2.53 (.51)'
`1.34 (.48)
`
`2.85 (.65)'
`1.54 (.48)
`
`3.48 (.60)'
`2.20 (.65)·
`
`0.43·
`
`-0.04
`
`0.37.1
`
`Norms'
`
`Study Group"
`Community Teacher
`NIH Teacher
`
`Community Teacher versus
`NIH Teacher (r,)·
`
`a Werry et aI. (1975).
`two-tailed t-test.
`b Study Group versus Norms,
`c Spearman rank-order correlation coefficients.
`.I p < .05.
`e P < .01.
`f P < .001.
`
`Page 5
`
`

`

`230
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`0.39, P < .01). The data reported here are consistent with the z
`scores cited above.
`Peak plasma levels occurred between 3-8 h (X ± SEM = 65.7 ±
`7.1,70.2 ± 7.9,65.8 ± 7.8,64.8 ± 8.8,68.6 ± 7.6, and 64.1 ± 9.5
`ng/ml, respectively). Plasma levels of d-amphetamine in individual
`children differed threefold at 1 h, fivefold at 2 h, threefold at h 3
`and 4, and two and one-halffold thereafter. The coefficients of
`variation (CV = SD/X expressed as a percent) of d-amphetamine
`plasma levels during the absorption phase at h 1-8 are 38, 42, 32,
`34,36,38,31, and 39%, respectively.
`Figure 1 shows the behavioral response to a single dose of
`sustained-release d-amphetamine compared with placebo. There
`was a significant difference between drug and placebo ABCTRS
`t = 3.00, P < .01).
`ratings at h 2 (paired t-tests:
`In HAC,
`the
`
`AMPHETAMINE BLOOD LEVELS AND BEHAVIOR RATINGS
`ON SUSTAINED RELEASE
`
`80
`
`OJm
`
`I~
`
`2.5 <5
`:xl
`:xl
`2.0 ~
`
`ZG
`
`>
`
`1.5 CJ)s
`
`OJ
`1.0 (")
`-l
`:xl
`~
`
`N=9
`
`1 Placebo
`I
`--- ---- '"
`I
`,,---- <., 1
`
`1-....--.-:*':7:*-",-__
`
`'
`
`I
`
`- - - --
`
`234
`TIME (hours)
`
`5
`
`6
`
`EE70
`
`zo~
`
`60
`
`l
`
`~~
`
`50
`
`uzo
`u 40wz
`
`30
`
`~~
`
`Ia.
`~ 20«
`10
`
`«~
`
`~a
`
`.
`
`Page 6
`
`

`

`

`

`232
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`All t-tests were one-tailed because a large majority of HAC have
`a positive response to amphetamine (Millichap, 1973) or no re(cid:173)
`sponse. The very few children who have a negative response are
`often considered borderline psychotic (excluded from this study)
`prior to medication (Wender, 1971).
`
`DISCUSSION
`
`The mean (± SEM) apparent half-lives for tablet d-amphetamine
`and for sustained-release d-amphetamine (identical doses) obtained
`from an earlier group of seven children studied on two separate
`occasions were not significantly different (6.6 ± .05 and 8.4 ± 1.2
`h, respectively) (fig. 3, Brown et aI., 1979b). In both, apparent
`half-lives were calculated from a least squares linear regression
`analysis done on the plasma disappearance curve of each patient
`over a 30-h interval. For tablet, peak values occurred between h 3
`and 4 (63.6 ± 5.3 and 63.3 ± 6.1 ng/ml, respectively); and for the
`sustained release, peak values occurred between h 3-6 (60.7 ± 7.8,
`62.2 ± 6.2, 61.7 ± 2.2 ng/ml at h 3, 4, and 6, respectively). Sus(cid:173)
`tained release thus gave a somewhat more plateaulike blood level
`during this peak level period. Plasma levels of d-amphetamine in
`individual children differed fourfold at h 1, two and one-halffold
`at h 2, twofold at h 3, and less than twofold at h 4 after tablet and
`threefold at hi, and about twofold at h 2-4 after sustained release.
`Thus, the early absorption of tablets was somewhat more variable
`than that of sustained-release capsules; whereas the variability in
`plasma levels following both was somewhat
`less during h 6-30
`(elimination) as compared to h 1-6 (absorption). The CV of
`d-amphetamine plasma levels during the absorption phase at h 1-4
`were 66, 31, 22, and 24%, respectively, for tablet, and 47,30,34,
`and 25%, respectively, for sustained release. When plasma values
`obtained from tablets and from sustained release were compared
`for similarity in these seven children,
`the intraclass correlation
`coefficients (ICC) of serial plasma d-amphetamine levels on the
`two study days were significant in five of the seven individuals
`.98, P < .01); however, a chi-square test indicat(cid:173)
`(ICC range,
`.52 -
`ed that the seven ICC's did not differ significantly from one an(cid:173)
`other. The pooled ICC was .83. The interchangeability of sets of
`plasma amphetamine values is quite high for a given child, whether
`he receives tablet or sustained release;
`the interchangeability of
`these sets of values is only modestly less than those reported when
`
`Page 8
`
`

`

`Behavior Response in Hyperactive Children
`
`233
`
`individual children are given tablets on two separate occasions
`(Brown et al. , 1979a, 1979b). Thus,
`to the degree that differing
`plasma pharmacokinetics from these two preparations could be ex(cid:173)
`pected to effect differing clinical responses, one would predict only
`modest differences, if any,
`in clinical response. The plasma data
`from this study group are similar to the earlier HAC group given
`sustained-release d-amphetamine. The standard breakfast given to
`the sustained-release study group here reported (and not to the
`earlier group) appears to have little effect on the CV.
`levels of
`The
`apparent
`elimination
`half-life of
`plasma
`d-amphetamine for a goup of 16 HAC (6.8 ± 0.5 h) who received
`tablets (Brown et al., 1979a, 1979b) was considerably less than that
`reported for eight depressed adults (19.4 ± 4.6 h) (Ebert et al.,
`1976; van Kammen and Murphy, 1975). Both groups attained
`peak levels at
`the same time; and both showed the maximal
`behavioral response at
`the same time, h 1-4). The findings of
`Ebert et al. (1976) are consistent with other adult studies when uri(cid:173)
`nary pH is considered (Davis et al., 1971; Kreuz and Axelrod,
`1974). More recent work (Gershon et al., 1979; Nurnberger et al.,
`1979) indicates that normal adult twins show an elimination half(cid:173)
`life of 10.5 h when the diet is controlled and the urinary pH is
`more similar to that of the HAC here reported. Though urinary
`pH differences could account for some of the half-life differences
`between the depressed adults and the HAC, it is very unlikely to
`account for
`the differences between the HAC and the normal
`adults. If HAC do eliminate d-amphetarnine more rapidly than
`normal adults, no explanation is clear currently. The half-life of
`methylphenidate in HAC is 2.6 h, SD ± 0.16 (Hungund et al.,
`1979) as compared to normal adults (approximately 2 h) (Faraj et
`al., 1974). The metabolism of amphetamine is more dependent
`upon liver enzymes than that of methylphenidate. Children and
`adults apparently absorb d-amphetamine and respond behaviorally
`in a similar fashion with regard to time, but adults may eliminate
`the drug more slowly.
`The earlier study of the response of HAC to a single dose of
`d-amphetamine (0.45 ± .02 mg/kg) also showed that significant
`behavioral and motor activity responses occurred during the ab(cid:173)
`sorption phase (h 1-4) and did not correlate with plasma am pheta(cid:173)
`mine levels (Brown et al., 1979a). A later replication study of the
`response of a different group of 14 HAC to a single dose of
`d-amphetamine elixir (0.5 mg/kg) with armboard, heparin-lock,
`
`Page 9
`
`

`

`234
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`and placebo blood study days (as in this study) showed results simi(cid:173)
`lar to the tablet study as well as reliable results within the same
`group of children (similar drug study conditions on two separate
`occasions) (Brown et al., 1977). ABCTRS ratings are somewhat de(cid:173)
`pendent upon interactions between children;
`though the ratings
`themselves were done consistently on both amphetamine and pla(cid:173)
`cebo study days, interactions between children were not consistent
`across all intervals on a given study day. Lack of behavioral effects
`postpeak amphetamine levels (h 5 and 6) could possibly be due to
`the decreased behavioral rating on placebo days at those time inter(cid:173)
`vals, whereas the return of disruptive behavior on amphetamine
`days is less than baseline for the postpeak intervals; but the likeli(cid:173)
`hood of a type II error in this small N sample is reduced by our
`having used one-tailed t-tests. The preferable statistical analysis to
`employ in such a study would be an analysis of variance to assess
`drug effect,
`time effect, and their possible interaction, as well as
`the avoidance of a possible occasional significant difference by
`paired t-test from chance alone. However, difficulties in obtaining
`com plete serial sets of data for all children studied (particularly be(cid:173)
`havior) determined the choice of paired t-tests.
`When the earlier absorption-elimination study of tablet and
`sustained-release amphetamine is compared to the current study
`(see figs. 1, 2, and 3), it is clear that the peak plasma level occurs
`later and lasts longer with sustained-release (up to h 8), though this
`later occurrence and more plateaulike peak plasma level
`is not
`accom panied by a longer period of significant response to the
`medication (in fact, the significant response appears to be shorter).
`Additionally, the earlier and more significant responses to tablet,
`and particularly to elixir, may further indicate that clinical re(cid:173)
`sponse is related to absorption. The pharmacogenetic studies of
`Gershon et al. (1979) in normal adult twins indicate that the period
`of maximal behavioral change occurs within h 1 after intravenous
`d-amphetamine despite a mean elimination half-life of greater
`than 10 h. Behavioral response thus appears unlikely to be a sec(cid:173)
`ondary response to plasma amphetamine level. These authors
`suggest that genetic variation in the amount of releasable cate(cid:173)
`cholamines, susceptibility of cellular and vesicular membranes to
`amphetamine, or sensitivity of postsynaptic receptor sites may, in
`part, explain this profile of response. Pharmacokinetic data for
`methylphenidate in HAC mayor may not be consistent with that
`of d-amphetamine. Hungund et al.
`(1979) suggest that the low
`
`Page 10
`
`

`

`

`

`236
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`"behavioral" and pharmacological half-life of methylphenidate is
`coincidental. Whether the differences in behavioral parameters be(cid:173)
`ing measured contribute to an understanding of the mechanism of
`response for methylphenidate and d-amphetamine is unclear
`(Sprague and Sleator, 1977).
`Studies in rats of the effect of methamphetamine on NE metabo(cid:173)
`lism and behavior demonstrate that methamphetamine (5.0 mg/kg)
`inhibits reuptake and increases normetanephrine levels in the first
`several hours postdrug administration. In these experiments, in(cid:173)
`creases
`in normetanephrine, a metabolite that
`reflects extra(cid:173)
`neuronal metabolism of NE, correlate highly with the behavioral
`response to methamphetamine (Cook and Schanberg, 1970). When
`rhesus monkeys were assessed behaviorally after single oral doses
`of d-amphetamine (0.32 and 1.0 mg/kg), maximal behavioral ef(cid:173)
`fects were seen prior to the peak plasma level of amphetamine at
`times when the plasma level was twofold lower than that which was
`subsequently attained (Downs and Braude, 1977). The mechanism
`of the decreased response to later similar levels of d-amphetarnine
`(from a single dose) in plasma may be related to depletion of
`catecholamine stores, to replacement by a "false neurotransmitter"
`metabolite of amphetamine (Kopin, 1968a, 1968b), or to alteration
`in receptor sensitivity (Bunney and Murphy, 1975).
`to whether
`The most
`important
`clinical question relates
`sustained-release d-arnphetamine actually does lead to a prolonged
`clinical response. Despite the literature th at raises this prolongation
`as a possibility, these data would not support such a conclusion, nor
`would the pharmacokinetic-clinical response data in previous stud(cid:173)
`ies. Gross and Wilson (1974) suggest
`that variable absorption
`- "delayed and incomplete"-may account for some of the varia(cid:173)
`tion in clinical response; however, our data suggest modest varia(cid:173)
`tion within the group during the absorption phase. Earlier data
`suggest less variation during the early absorption phase for sus(cid:173)
`tained release than for tablet, as well as more plateaulike plasma
`levels during the peak level period; both preparations show some(cid:173)
`what less variability during h 6-30, as compared to the early hours
`during the absorption phase. Thus,
`the variability in absorption
`and elimination between sustained release and tablet appear to be
`either unrelated to the observed clinical response in this study, or
`the slower rate of absorption for the sustained release may relate to
`a later onset and less significant clinical response vs. the response
`observed after tablet. Safer and Allen (1976) found that at least
`
`Page 12
`
`

`

`Behavior Response in Hyperactive Children
`
`237
`
`10% of HAC who take 15 mg sustained-release spansules as a
`beginning dose will experience insomnia. Our study did not pro(cid:173)
`vide for systematically observing differences between tablet and
`spansule response in the evening hours. Insomnia has sometimes
`been a clinical complaint on the evening of the several single-dose
`studies; such appears to have been dose-related, but not prepara(cid:173)
`tion-related. The period during which absorption and elimina(cid:173)
`tion are in equilibrium (peak plasma level) clearly does last longer
`for the spansules than for the tablets at the same dose. Thus, for
`some children, this prolonged peak plasma level might relate to in(cid:173)
`somnia,
`though one could also hypothesize less of a "rebound
`effect"-an effect that is sometimes felt to be observable clinically,
`but which has not been documented in a controlled study. Should
`the clinical response be related to a necessary rate of absorption
`and be unrelated to the peak plasma level per se and its duration,
`then the earlier studies would not lead one to predict a prolonged
`response. These data would not suggest that there is any general
`advantage in single-dose sustained-release capsule vs. a tablet at
`8:00 A.M. and 12:00 P.M., since there is no evidence that any
`pharmacologically induced therapeutic effect can be obtained be(cid:173)
`yond 4 hours from the oral administration of a single dose (0.5
`mg/kg) of d-amphetamine to HAC.
`In conclusion,
`this study of sustained-release d-amphetamine,
`like earlier single-dose amphetamine studies in hyperactive chil(cid:173)
`dren, shows significant behavior and motor activity responses
`to the medication only during the absorption phase, and these
`responses are not correlated with specific plasma levels of d(cid:173)
`amphetamine. Furthermore, despite pharmacokinetic differences
`between elixir, tablet, and sustained release, all at the same dose,
`there is no evidence that a prolonged clinical response results
`from the use of the sustained-release preparation.
`
`REFERENCES
`
`ARNOLD, L. E., WENDER, P. H., MCCLOSKEY, K., & SNYDER, S. H. (1972), Levoampheta(cid:173)
`mine and dextroamphetamine. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 27:816-822.
`AXELROD, J. (1970), Amphetamine. In: Amphetamine and Related Compounds, ed. E. Costa & S.
`Garattini. New York: Raven Press, pp. 207-216.
`BECKETT, A. H., SALMON, J. A., & MITCHORD, M. (1969), The relations between blood levels
`and urinary excretion of amphetamine under controlled acidic and under fluctuating uri(cid:173)
`nary pH values using (14C) amphetamine.]. Pharm. Pharmacol., 21:251-259.
`
`Page 13
`
`

`

`238
`
`Gerald L. Brown et al.
`
`- - - & TUCKER, G. T. (1966), A method for the evaluation of some oral prolonged-release
`forms of dexamphetamine in man, using urinary excretion data.). Pharm. Pharmacol., 18
`(Suppl): 72S-75S.
`(1937), The behavior of children receiving benzedrine. Amer. J. Psychiat.,
`BRADLEY, C.
`94:577-585.
`BROWN, G. L., EBERT, M. H., & HUNT, R. D. (1978), Plasma d-amphetarnine absorption and
`elimination in hyperactive children. Psychopharm. Bull., 14:33-35.
`- - - - - - MIKKELSEN, E. J., BUNNEY, W. E., JR., & KOPIN, I. J. (l979a), Plasma levels of
`d-amphetamine in hyperactive children. Psychopharmacology, 62:133-140.
`- - - - - - - - - & HUNT, R. D. (l979b), Clinical pharmacology of d-amphetamine in
`hyperactive children. In: Pharmacokinetics of Psychoactive Drugs, ed. L. A. Gottschalk. New
`York: Spectrum, pp. 137-153.
`- - - - - - - - - KOPIN, I. J., & BUNNEY, W. E., JR. (1977), Reliability of behavior and
`activity responses in hyperactive children following single doses of d-amphetamine, Read
`at the American Psychiatric Association Annual Meeting, Toronto.
`BUNNEY, W. E., JR. & MURPHY, D. L.
`(1975), Strategies for
`the systematic study of
`neurotransmitter receptor function in man.
`In: Pre- and Post-Synaptic Receptors, ed. E.
`Usdin & W. E. Bunney, Jr. New York: Marcel Dekker, pp. 283-311.
`CANTWELL, D. P. & CARLSON, G. A. (1978), Stimulants. In: Pediatric Psychopharmacology, ed. J.
`S. Werry. New York: Brunner/Mazel, pp. 171-207.
`CHENG, L. T., KIM, S. Y., CHUNG, A, & CASTRO, A. (1973), Amphetamine. FEBS Lett.,
`36:339-342.
`COLBURN, T. R., SMITH, B. A., & GUARINI, J. J. (1976a), An ambulating activity monitor with
`solid state memory. Biomed. Sci. Instruments, 12: 117-122.
`- - - - - - - - - & SIMMONS, N. N. (l976b), An ambulatory activity monitor with solid
`state memory.ISA Trans., 15:149-154.
`CONNERS, C. K. (1969), A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. Amer.],
`Psychiat., 126:152-156.
`- - - (1973), Rating scales for use in drug studies with children. Psychopharm. Bull. (Special
`Issue-Pharmacotherapy of Children):24-29.
`- - - TAYWR, E., MEO, G., KURTZ, M. A., & FOURNIER, M. (1972), Magnesium pemoline
`and dextroamphetamine. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.), 26:321-336.
`COOK, J. D. & SCHANBERG, S. M. (1970), The effects of methamphetamine on behavior and
`on
`the uptake,
`release,
`and metabolism of norepinephrine. Biochem. Pharmacol.,
`19:1165-1179.
`CREAK, M. [Chairman, Progress Report of a Working Party] (1961), Schizophrenic syndrome
`in childhood. Brit. Med.)., 2:889-890.
`DAVIS, J. M., KOPIN, I. J., LEMBERGER, L., & AXELROD, J. (1971), Effects of urinary pH on
`amphetamine metabolism. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 179:493-50 I.
`DOWNS, D. A. & BRAUDE, M. C. (1977), Time-action and behavioral effects of amphetamine,
`ethanol, and acetylmethadol. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav., 6:671-676.
`EBERT, M. H., VAN KAMMEN, D. P., & MURPHY, D. L. (1976), Plasma levels of amphetamine
`and behavioral response. In: Pharmacokinetics of Psychoactive Drugs, ed. L. A. Gottschalk &
`S. Merlis. New York: Spectrum Publications, pp. 157-169.
`FARAJ, B. A., ISRAIL!, Z. H., PEREL, J. M., JENKINS, M. L., HOLTZMAN, S. G., CUCINELL, S. A.,
`& DAYTON, P. G. (1974), Metabolism and studies in man and animals.). Pharm. Exp. Ther.,
`191:535-547.
`GERSHON, E. S., NURNBERGER, J. N., SITARAM, N., & GILLIN, J. C. (1979), Pharmacogenetics
`and the pharmacologic challenge strategy in clinical
`research.
`In: Progress in Neuro(cid:173)
`psychopharmacology, ed. B. Saletu. Oxford: Pergamon Press, pp. 75-83.
`GREENBERG, L. M., DEEM, M. A., & McMAHON, S. (1972), Effects of dextroamphetamine,
`chlorpromazine, and hydroxyzine on behavior and performance in hyperactive children.
`Amer.], Psychiat., 129:532-539.
`
`Page 14
`
`

`

`Behavior Response in Hyperactive Children
`
`239
`
`GROSS, M. B. & WILSON, W. C. (1974), Minimal Brain Dysfunction. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
`GUY, W. (1976), Physical and neurological examination for soft signs. In: ECDEU Assessment
`Manual, 384-393. Rockville, Md.: DHEW/ADAMHAlNIMH, Psychopharmacology Re(cid:173)
`search Branch.
`HUESTIS, R. D., ARNOLD, L. E., & SMELTZER, D. J. (1975), Caffeine versus methylphenidate
`and d-amphetarnine in minimal brain dysfunction. Amer.]. Psychia., 132:868-870.
`HUNGUND, B. L., PEREL, J. M., HURWIC, M. J., SVERD, J., & WINSBERG, B. G.
`(1979),
`Pharmacokinetics of methylphenidate in hyperactive children. Read at the annual meeting
`of Society of Biological Psychiatry, Chicago.
`KOPIN, I. J. (1968a), False adrenergic transmitters. Ann. Rev. Pharmacol., 8:377-394.
`- - - (1968b), The influence of false neurotransmitters on adrenergic transmission. In:
`Adrenergic Neurotransmission, ed. G. E. W. Wolstenholme & M. O'Conner. Boston: Little,
`Brown, pp. 95·-104.
`KREUZ, D. S. & AXELROD, J. (1974), Amphetamine in human plasma. Science, 182:420-421.
`MILLICHAP, J. G. (1973), Drugs in management of minimal brain dysfunction. Ann. N.Y.
`Acad. Sci., 205:321-334.
`NURNBERGER, J. I., GERSHON, E. S., EBERT, M. H., & BUCHSBAUM, M. S. (1979), Pharma(cid:173)
`cogenetics of amphetamine in normal twins. Read at the annual meeting of Society of Bio(cid:173)
`logical Psychiatry, Chicago.
`(1979), Comparison of RIA and GC-MS assays
`POWERS, K. G. & EBERT, M. H.
`d-amphetamine. Biomed. Mass. Spectrometry, 6:187-190.
`RAPOPORT, J. L., BUCHSBAUM, M. S., ZAHN, T. P., WEINGARTNER, H., LUDLOW, C., &
`MIKKELSEN, E. J. (1978), Dextroamphetamine. Science, 199:560-562.
`ROSEN, E., ELLISON, T., TANNENBAUM, P., FREE, S. M., & CROSLEY, A. P., JR. (1967), Com(cid:173)
`parative study in man and dog of the absorption and excretion of dextroamphetamine-I''C
`j. Pharm. Sci.,
`sulfate in sustained-release and nonsustained-release dosage forms.
`56:365-369.
`Ross, D. M. & Ross, S. A. (1976), Hyperactivity. New York: John Wiley.
`ROWLAND, M.
`(1969), Amphetami

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket