throbber
ournal of
`
`lull V
`
`TUP 1-8111.1110V1
`
`February 1996
`
`re;
`
`HEALTH SCIENUES LIBRARI
`
`inical FEB U7 1996
`chopharmacology
`
`Editors-in-Chief
`Richard I. Shader, M.D.
`David J. Greenblatt, M.D.
`Assistant Editors
`
`Lawrence G. Miller, M.D.
`Domenic A. Ciraulo, M.D.
`Tufts University School of Medicine
`Boston, Massachusetts
`
`Barbara Kern, Managing Editor
`
`Editorial Board
`David J. Kupfer, M.D., Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
`Louis Lasagna, M.D., Boston, Massachusetts
`V. Markku I. Linnoila, M.D., Ph.D.,
`Bethesda, Maryland
`Herbert Y. Meltzer, M.D., Cleveland, Ohio
`Jack H. Mendelson, M.D., Belmont, Massachusetts
`Roger E. Meyer, M.D., Washington, D.C.
`Robert E. Rakel, M.D., Houston, Texas
`Donald S. Robinson, M.D., Melbourne, Florida
`Alan F. Schatzberg, M.D., Stanford, California
`Joseph J. Schildkraut, M.D., Boston, Massachusetts
`Edward M. Sellers, M.D., Ph.D., Toronto, Ontario
`Joe P. Tupin, M.D., Sacramento, California
`Richard J. Wyatt, M.D., Washington, D.C.
`
`Cole, M.D., Belmont, Massachusetts
`'is, M.D., Chicago, Illinois
`▪ T, M.D., Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
`▪ ledel, M.D., Birmingham, Alabama
`-7- hon, M.D., Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
`-
`. Glassman, M.D., New York, New York
`4- ' M.D., Stanford, California
`
`E , Goodwin, M.D., Bethesda, Maryland
`towsky, M.D., Chapel Hill, North Carolina
`Tik, M.D., Ph.D., Los Angeles, California
`r.- :-. lein, M.D., New York, New York
`
`0
`
`re,
`
`L
`•
`* • C.
`* • o
`* 0 OW
`
`*
`*
`*
`* CIN
`*
`$,-
`*
`r —
`*
`
`9
`
`.c
`U) G)
`
`OLI-N3
`U)'
`r.-1. .1
`
`›—
`* •
`f—
`•— c0 X<
`*
`OW
`*
`* a.. ==.0v)
`
`Ross J. Baldessarini, M.D., Belmont, Massachusetts
`
`Magda Campbell, M.D., New York, New York
`
`Bernard Carroll, M.D., Ph.D., Durham, North Carolina
`
`Page 1
`
`SHIRE EX. 2059
`KVK v. SHIRE
`IPR2018-00293
`
`

`

`JOURNAL OF
`CLINICAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY
`
`VOLUME 16
`
`FEBRUARY
`
`NUMBER 1
`
`CONTENTS
`1 Editorial. Twice May Be Too Many: Redundant Publications. Richard I. Shadpr, David,J. Green-
`blatt
`
`2 Mitchell B. Baiter Award: Call for Papers
`
`2 Editors' Note
`
`3 Paroxetine as a- Treatment for Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. Kimberly A. Yonkers,
`Christina Gullion, Anita Williams, Kimberly Novak, A. John Rush
`
`9 Fluvoxamine in Prevention of Relapse in Bulimia Nervosa: Effects on Eating-Specific Psy-
`chopathology. Manfred M. Ficther, Ralf Kruger, Winfried Rief, Robert Holland, Jutta Dohne
`
`19 A Study of the Effect of Age and Gender on the Pharmacokinetics of Nefazodone After Sin-
`gle and Multiple Doses. Rashmi H. Barbhaiya, Akshay B. Buch, Douglas S. Greene
`
`26 Investigation of Pharmacokinetic and Pharmacodynamic Interactions After Coadministra-
`tion of Nefazodone and Haloperidol. Rashmi H. Barbhaiya, Umesh A. Shukla, Douglas S. Greene,
`Hans-Peter Breuel, Kamal K. Midha
`
`35 Relatively Low Doses of Cisapride in the Treatment of Nausea in Patients Treated With
`Venlafaxine for Treatment-Refractory Depression. Johnna L. Russell
`
`38 Antipsychotic and Anxiolytic Properties of Rispgridone, Haloperidol, and Methotrimeprazine
`in Schizophrenic Patients. Olivier Blin, Jean Miche Azorin, Philippe Bouhours
`
`45 Prediction of Haloperidol Steady-State Levels in Plasma After a Single Test Dose. Javaid I.
`Javaid, Philip G. Janicak, Rajiv P. Sharma, Anne M. Le ch, John M. Davis, Zhengyu Wang
`
`51 Multiple Drug Use and Psychiatric Comorbidity in Patients Admitted to the Hospital With
`Severe Benzodiazepine Dependence. Usoa E. Busto, Myroslava K. Romach, Edward M. Sellers
`
`58 Buprenorphine Versus Methadone in the Treatment of Opioid Dependence: 'Self-Reports,
`Urinalysis, and Addiction Severity Index. Eric C. Strain, Maxine L. Stitzer, Ira A. Liebson, George
`E. Bigelow
`
`Brief Reports
`68 Lithium and Angiotensin-Converting Enzyme Inhibitors: Evaluation of a Potential Inter-
`action. Patrick R. Finley, John G. O'Brien, Robert W. Coleman
`
`72 Acute Tolerance to Subjective but not Cardiovascular Effects of d-Amphetamine in Nor-
`mal, Healthy Men. Lisa H. Brauer, John Ambre, Harriet de Wit
`
`JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY (ISSN 0271,0749) is published bimonthly, in Feb., Apr., June, Aug., Oct., and Dec., by
`Williams & Wilkins, 351 West Camden Street, Baltimore, MD 21201-2436. Subscription rates $114.00 ($149.00 foreign); institutions $174.00
`($209.00 foreign); in-training $57.00 ($92.00 foreign); single copy $24.00 ($26.00 foreign). (Prices subject to change). The GST number for Cana-
`dian subscribers is 123394371. Second class postage paid at Baltimore, MD, and at additional mailing offices. POSTMASTER: Send address
`changes to JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, 351 West Camden Street, Baltimore, MD 21201-2436. Indexed by Current
`Contents (Clinical Medicine, Life Sciences, Science Citation Index, Research Alert, ISI/BioMed), Index Medicus, Excerpta Medica, BIOSIS,
`Psychlnfo and Toxicology Abstracts. Printed in U.S.A. Copyright © 1996 by Williams & Wilkins.
`
`Page 2
`
`

`

`0271-0749/96/1601-0072603.00/0
`Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology
`Copyright © 1996 by Williams & Wilkins
`
`Vol. 16, No. 1
`Printed in U.S.A.
`
`Acute Tolerance to Subjective but not Cardiovascular
`Effects of d-Amphetamine in Normal, Healthy Men
`
`LISA H. BRAUER, PilD,1.4 JOHN AMBRE, MD, PHD,2 AND HARRIET DE WIT, PHD3
`'Postdoctoral Fellow, Department of Psychiatry, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois;
`2Director, Department of Toxicology and Drug Abuse, American Medical Association,
`Chicago, Illinois; 3Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, University of Chicago,
`Chicago, Illinois; and 4Research Associate, Department of Psychiatry, Duke University
`Medical Center, Durham, NC •
`
`This is a descriptive report on the relationship
`between the pharmacokinetics and plarmacody-
`namics of d-amphetamine in healthy, normal vol-
`unteers. Six men, aged 22 to 31, attended two ex-
`perimental sessions during which they received
`single oral doses of 20 mg- of d-amphetamine.
`Plasma levels Qf drug and measures of drug effect
`were collected predrug and at regular intervals
`Is
`for 24 hours after drug administration. P1 ma
`drug levels peaked at 4 hours and remained a de-
`tectable levels for 24 hours after drug admini tra-
`tion. Subjective ratings, including "feel drug" 1.nd
`"feel high" peaked at 1% to 2 hours and returned
`to baseline levels by 3 to 4 hours. Evaluation of
`phase plots (i.e., drug effect vs. drug concentra-
`tion) indicated, that acute tolerance developed to
`the subjective but not to the cardiopressor effects
`of d-amphetamine. This finding, implies that indi-
`viduals who repeatedly administer the drug to
`maintain certain levels of subjective effects may
`increase plasma drug levels and physiologic ef-
`fects to toxic levels. (J Clin Psychopharmacol
`1996;16:72-76)
`
`THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN drug concentration
`
`in plasma and drug response is important to inves-
`tigate because understanding pharmacokinetic-pharma-
`codynamic relationships may improve our knowledge of
`the basic mechanisms by which drugs produce their ef-
`fects. For example, these relationships may reveal the
`extent to which observed drug effects are directly re-
`lated to receptor occupancy or to the effects of metabo-
`lites.1 The relationship between drug concentration and
`drug effect is particularly important in the study of drugs
`that are abused, because it may influence repeated drug
`
`Received January 10, 1995, and accepted June 22, 1995.
`Address requests for reprints to: Harriet de Wit, PhD, Department
`of Psychiatry, University of Chicago, MC3077, 5841 South Maryland
`Ave., Chicago, IL 60637.
`
`administration within an episode of drug-taking. A ma-
`jor factor believed to maintain repeated ingestion of a
`drug is its mood-altering, or subjective effects.2 The drug
`effects that appear to be most desirable to drug abusers
`are those experienced during the onset of the drug effect
`(e.g., the "rush") 3' 4 After this initial effect, acute tolerance
`may develop to the mood-altering effects of the drug. That
`is, after the drug produces its initial effects on mood,
`these effects may rapidly dissipate, even though plasma
`levels of the drug are still increasing. However, tolerance
`to other effects of the drug, such as the cardiovascular ef-
`fects, may not develop at the same rate. Consequently, as
`individualS repeatedly self-administer a drug to maintain
`.desired mood effects, they may inadvertently escalate
`plasma concentrations and cardiovascular effects to
`toxic levels.
`Several-investigators have examined the pharmacoki-
`netic and pharmacodynamic profiles of cocaine in co-
`caine abusers,5-9 and the observed relationship betweeri
`drug concentrations in plasma and drug effects has been
`inconsistent across studies. For example, Javaid and
`colleagues' found that the times to peak for subjective
`and physiologic effects of single doses of cocaine corre-
`sponded well with plasma levels, but that subjective and
`physiologic effects had returned to baseline values be-
`fore plasma levels declined. These findings suggest that
`acute tolerance developed to both subjective and phys-
`iologic effects of cocaine. Fischman and colleagues'
`also demonstrated acute tolerance to both subjective
`and physiologic effects of single doses of cocaine using
`different procedures and different measures of drug ef-
`fect. Other studies have shown that acute tolerance to
`the subjective and certain physiologic effects of co-
`caine may develop at different rates.8, s For example,
`Foltin and associatess examined subjective and physio-
`logic responses to repeated doses of 96 mg of intranasal
`cocaine. They found that blood pressure increases cor-
`responded closely with increases in plasma levels of co-
`caine but that heart rate and subjective responses
`
`Page 3
`
`

`

`Acute Tolerance to d-Amphetamine
`
`J CLIN PSYCHOPHARMACOL, VOL 16/No 1, FEBRUARY 1996
`
`73
`
`Procedures
`
`view. Subjects were screened by a clinical psychologist
`reached their peak before plasma levels and declined
`much more rapidly. This pattern of results suggested 1.--ancl-a.earfliologist to rule out any psychosocial or med-
`that acute tolerance developed to the subjective, but
`ical condition that might contraindicate participation in
`not to the pressor, effects of cocaine. However, a sub-
`the study. Candidates with past or current serious med-
`sequent reanalysis of their data using more quantitative
`ical conditions, including cardiac or liver disease, high
`methods showed that tolerance, in fact, developed to
`blood pressure, or abnormal electrocardiograms, or
`the pressor effects of cocaine as well.'°
`who met criteria for past or current major axis I disor-
`Few studies have investigated the relationship be-
`ders (excluding nicotine dependence; DSM-III-R) were
`tween plasma levels of d-amphetamine and drug effects
`excluded.
`in normal, healthy volunteers.11-13 In general, studies
`with normal volunteers have found dissociation be-
`tween plasma levels and drug effects. For example, An-
`grist and coworkers13 administered 0.25 mg/kg oral d-
`amphetamine to normal subjects and measured
`subjective and cardiovascular effects and plasma levels
`over a 5-hour period. They found that, while plasma lev-
`els peaked at 2 to 3 hours after drug administration, car-
`diovascular and subjective responses peaked at 1 and 2
`hours, respectively. Both cardiovascular and subjective
`effects of d-amphetamine had declinedby 4 hours after
`drug administration, while blood levels remained sig-
`nificantly elevated.
`Studies with normal volunteers are important to ex-
`amine tolerance without the possible influence of vari-
`ables related to repeated drug use, such as conditioned
`responses and neuroadaptation, which may alter the
`pharmacodynamic profiles of drugs in drug abusers.
`Thus, the purpose of this descriptive report is to extend
`previous findings by examining the relationship be-
`tween plasma d-amphetamine levels and drug effects
`over a longer period of time (i.e., 24 hours) and on a
`broader range of dependent measures in normal volun-
`teers. By examining this relationship over a 24-hour pe-
`riod, both ascending and descending limbs of the plasma
`drug level- and drug effect-time curves can be charac-
`terized. Six male subjects attended two sessions during
`which they received 20 mg of oral d-amphetamine.
`Plasma levels and subjective and physiologic responses
`were measured predrug and for 24 hours thereafter.
`
`Data were collected as part of another study designed
`to investigate interactions between d-amphetamine and
`the dopamine antagonist pimozide. Because pimozide
`had no detectable effect on any measure of response to
`d-amphetamine (unpublished .data; see below), the re-
`sults are presented as the mean of the two sessions with
`d-amphetamine. Each subject attended two sessions
`separated by 1 week. Sessions were conducted in the
`University of Chicago Clinical Research Center •(CRC)
`and lasted from 6:30 a.m. until 9:45 a.m. the following
`day. During each session, subjects received a:capsule
`containing 20 mg of d-amphetamine. There was no
`placebo control condition in this study because the
`variable of interest was plasma d-amphetamine levels
`over time. Subjects were told that the capsules might
`contain a stimulant/appetite suppressant, sedative/mi-
`nor tranquilizer, major tranquilizer, or placebo. Sub-
`jects gave written informed consent before participa-
`tion, This study was approved by the University of
`Chic go Institutional Review Board.
`P s of subjects were admitted to the CRC at 6:30
`a.m.
`er an overnight fast. Subjects were provided with
`one glass of clear fruit juice upon arrival, but no other
`food or drink was available until 1.p.m., when they ate a
`light lunch. At 7 am., a baseline blood sample was ob-
`tained from an intravenous catheter placed in the sub-
`jects! riondominant arms. Subjects were then allowed to
`relax and acclimate to the catheter and the surround-
`ings. At 7:20 a.m. they completed baseline mood ques-
`tionnaires, and physiologic and behavioral measures
`were obtained (see below). These measures were col-
`lected again at 8:30 and at 9:20 a.m. At 9:30 am., subjects
`ingested a capsule containing 20 mg ofd-amphetamine
`(Dexedrine; Smith, Kline and French, Philadelphia,
`PA). This dose of d-amphetamine has been shown in
`previous studies in our laboratory to produce reliable
`effects on mood without producing adverse physiologic
`effects.15 Blood samples (10 ml) were collected before
`d-amphetamine administration (9:25 a.m.) and at 10,
`10:30, 11, -11:30, 12 p.m., 12:30, 1, 1:30; 2:30, 3:30, 9:30
`p.m. and 9:,30 a.m. Subjective and behavioral measures
`were obtained at the same times as blood samples,
`whereas physiologic measures were only collected at
`
`Methods
`
`Subjects
`
`.
`
`Six healthy men aged 22 to 31 (mean = 27 years) were
`recruited from the university community with adver-
`tisements and posters. To minimize possible pharma-
`cokinetic variability related to gender differences, only
`men were tested.'4 Interested participants were initially
`screened over the telephone. Individuals who were
`within 10% of normal body weight (mean = 74.2 .kg,
`range = 65.4-84.1), reported drinking at least one alco-
`holic beverage per week (mean = 4.0, range = 1-8), were
`high school graduates, and were native English speak-
`ers were asked to come to the laboratory for an inter-
`
`Page 4
`
`

`

`74
`
`J CLIN PSYCHOPHARMACOL, VOL 16/NO 1, FEBRUARY 1996
`
`hourly intervals until 3:30 p.m. and then at 9:30 p.m. and
`9:30 the next morning. When subjects were not com-
`pleting questionnaires, they relaxed in the testing room.
`They were allowed to read, watch television, play
`games, talk; or engage in other leisure activities. After
`completing the study, subjects attended a debriefing in-
`terview and were paid $160.
`
`Plasma d-Amphetamine
`
`•
`
`I
`
`50
`40
`30
`20
`10
`
`o 1 2
`
`3 4 5 6 12 24
`Hour
`
`50
`40
`30
`20
`10
`0
`0
`
`Brauer and Associates
`
`"Feel Drug"
`
`I 2
`
`.4 5
`'3
`Hour
`
`X A,
`6 12 24
`
`Dependent measures
`
`Subjective effects were assessed with the Profile of
`Mood States,16,17 the 49-item Addiction Research Center
`Inventory (ARCI18,19), a locally developed Drug Effects
`Questionnaire (DEQ; unpublished), and several Visual
`Analog Scales. Each of these questionnaires has been
`shown to be sensitive to the mood effects of a variety of
`psychoactive drugs, including stimulants.2° Only data
`from the MBG (euphoria) and A (stimulant-like effects)
`scales of the ARCI, and the "feel drug," "like drug," and
`"feel high" scales of the DEQ will be presented here.
`Physiologic measures included blood pressure and
`heart rate and were obtained using a Dynamap Vital
`Signs Monitor (Critikon, Inc., Palatine, IL). Plasma sam-
`ples were analyzed for d-amphetamine using positive
`, ,chemical ionization gas chromatography mass \ pec-
`
`
`trometry (Center for Human Toxicology, Salt Lake City;
`UT) using procedures described elsewhere.21,22
`
`Data analyses
`
`Measures obtained immediately before d-ampheta-
`mine administration (9:25 a.m.) were used -as baseline
`in all analyses. Because analyses of variance revealed
`that there were no significant differences between the
`two sessions on any measure, 'either between the first
`and second sessions or between sessions when sub-
`jects did or did not receive pimozide, the two determi-
`nations for each subject were averaged. Data for each
`variable were plotted, both across time and as phase
`plots, and inspected to determine whether the pharma-
`cokinetic and pharmacodynamic profiles of d-ampheta-
`mine covaried (see Figs. 1 and 2).
`
`Results
`
`The time course of plasma levels arid several subjec-
`tive and physiologic effects of d-amphetamine are
`shown in Figure 1. Plasma levels were detectable 1 hour
`after drug administration and reached a peak level of 40
`ng/ml at 4 hours. Plasma levels plateaued and then be-
`gan to decline 5 hours after drug administration but re-
`mained detectable even at 24 hours.
`Subjective effects produced a different profile. In
`general, subjective effects of d-amphetamine reached
`peak levels at 1% to 2 hours and declined significantly
`within the first 6 hours of the session. Ratings of "feel
`
`Systolic Blood Pressure
`
`"Like Drug"
`
`140
`130
`120
`110
`100
`
`f
`
`•
`•
`•
`•
`0 1 2 3 4 5 672 24
`Hour
`
`Heart Rate
`
`90
`80
`70
`60
`50
`0 1 2 3 4 5 67224
`Hour
`
`5.5
`4.5
`3.5
`2.5
`1.5
`0
`
`co
`
`i
`
`2 3 4 5
`Hour
`
`6 12 24
`
`FIG. 1. Mean (SE) plasma levels and drug effects of 20 mg d-amphet7
`amine as a function of time. The 0 time point shows values obtained
`immediately before drug administration (9:25 a.m.), and each subsel
`quent point shows values obtained at various intervals thereafter:
`"Feel drug" and "like drug" scores range from 0 to 100, and Addiction
`Research Center Inventory A scale scores range from 0 to 11.
`
`drug," "like drug," "feel high," and "euphoria" (ARCI
`MBG) reached peak levels at 114 hours, while stimulant
`like effects (ARCI A) peaked at 2 hours. Ratings of "feel
`drug," "like drug," and "feel high" had returned to base-
`line levels by hour 5, and scores on the> A and MBG
`scales of the ARCI reached baseline levels at 12 hours
`postdrug (Fig. 1).
`Figure 1 also shows the time course of cardiovascu-
`lar responses to d-amphetamine. In general, changes in
`systolic blood pressure paralleled changes in plasma
`drug concentration over time, but blood pressure was
`still slightly elevated when plasma levels were begin-
`ning to decline. In contrast, heart rate remained low
`while plasma levels were increasing and began to rise 4
`hours after drug administration. Heart rate continued to
`increase for the duration of the session.
`Phase plots of plasma drug concentration versus
`drug effect are shown in Figure 2. This figure illustrates
`that the subjective effects of d-amphetamine show con-
`siderable clockwise hysteresis: the relationship be-
`tween plasma concentration and subjective response
`changes over the rising and falling phases of the drug
`concentration curve. This pattern of results suggests
`the development of acute tolerance. In contrast, the car-
`
`• ••• -.4••••••••
`
`•••••••••••••••..-.
`
`Page 5
`
`

`

`Acute Tolerance to d-Amphetamine
`
`J OLIN PSYCHOPIIARMACOL, VOL 16/NO 1, FEBRUARY 1996
`
`75
`
`40
`
`30
`
`20
`
`10
`
`0
`0
`
`E
`
`40 .
`30
`20
`10
`- Plasma 4-Amphetamine
`(ng/ml)
`
`0
`
`20
`10
`40
`30
`Plasma 4-Amphetamine
`(ng/ml)
`
`a.
`2
`02
`
`90
`
`80
`
`70
`
`60
`
`50
`0
`
`155
`145
`a '135
`125
`U vs 115
`105
`0
`
`1
`
`40
`30
`20
`10
`Plasma 4-Amphetamine
`(ng/m1)
`
`40
`30
`20
`10
`Plasma 4-Amphetamine
`(ng/ml)
`
`Plasma 4-Amphetamine
`(ng/ml)
`
`FIG. 2. Phase plots of drug concentration versus drug effect for sys-
`tolic blood pressure and for representative subjective responses to c/-
`amphetamine. Subjective responses to d-amphetamine (left panel)
`show marked clockwise hysteresis, suggestive of acute tolerance to
`these effects. In contrast, the panel on the right shows no evidence of
`hysteresis, and thus of acute tolerance, to the cardiopressor effects
`of d-amphetamine. (ARCI, Addiction Research Center Inventory).
`
`diopressor effects of d-amphetamine show little or no
`hysteresis: the maximal effect on blood pressure is pro-
`duced by plasma d-amphetamine concentration of
`aboUt 15 to 20 ng/ml and is sustained throughout the ex-
`perimental session. Thus, there is no evidence of acute
`tolerance development to these effects.
`
`Discussion
`
`The purpose of this report was to describe the rela-
`tionship between plasma concentrations and subjective
`and physiologic effects of d-amphetamine in six
`healthy, normal men. The results of the study indicate
`that the time course of plasma levels and drug effects of
`d-amphetamine are dissociable. For example, although
`plasma levels peaked 4 hours after drug administration
`and were still detectable at 24 hours, subjective effects
`peaked at approximately 2 hours and declined within 6
`hours (Fig. 1). Systolic blood pressure rose quickly, and
`the increase was sustained. The heart rate response
`
`was apparently dampened initially and then exhibited a
`delayed nse. These results suggest tolerance to the sub-
`jective effects of d-amphetamine but not to the pressor
`effects. At similar plasma drug concentrations, subjec-
`tive responses were greater on the ascending compared
`with the descending limb of the concentration-effect
`curve. This is evident in the phase plots as marked
`clockwise hysteresis (Fig. 2). In the absence of evi-
`dence for the production of active antagonist metabo-
`lites' of d-amphetamine, the most likely explanation of
`this finding is the development of acute tolerance. Co-
`caine exhibits a similar phenomenon (e.g., see ref. 7).
`An interesting finding in this study was the time course
`of the heart rate effects of d-amphetamine. Heart rate re-
`mained at baseline levels until 5 hours into the session
`and began to rise steadily thereafter, at a time when
`plasma levels were beginning to decline (see Fig. 1). A
`similar pattern of physiologic response to d-ampheta-
`mine has been reported in a study by Martin and col-
`leagues.19 They found that although blood pressure rose
`steadily within the 5 hours after drug administration,
`heart rate remained low during this time and did not be-
`gin to increase until blood pressure was declining. Mar-
`tin and colleagues19 attributed this relationship to reflex-
`ive slowing of the heart rate in response to increased
`blood pressure. Our findings also suggest an early physi-
`ologic reflex response and a later adjustment to the pres-
`sor rise. These cardiovascular responses are consistent
`with the mechanism of action of d-amphetamine (i.e., re-
`lease of norepinephrine at sympathetic nerve synapses).
`Norpinephrine infusion causes a pressor response with
`reflec cardiac slowing. The actual heart rate depends on
`a balAnce of chronotropic modifying factors.23
`Although the results of this study are suggestive of the
`development of acute tolerance to some of the effects of
`d-amphetamine, the absence of a placebo control con-
`dition makes it difficult to rule out other possible inter-
`pretations of the data. For example, the time course of
`the cardiovascular effects of d-amphetamine could be
`related to nonpharmacologic factors such as uncon-
`trolled variations related to circadian rhythms or to en-
`vironmental events. The time course of the subjective ef-
`fects could be the result of adaptation to repeated
`administration of the questionnaires. These issues can
`only be addressed by subsequent studies including a
`placebo control condition. Nevertheless, the results are
`consistent with the development of acute tolerance and
`are in agreement with the findings of studies with an-
`other psychomotor stimulant, cocaine (see above).
`The findings of this study have implications for un-
`derstanding the toxicity sometimes associated with
`stimulant abuse. It is commonly assumed that individu-
`als self-administer doses based on the subjective effects
`experienced.2 Thus, as subjective effects begin to wane,
`
`Page 6
`
`

`

`76
`
`J CLIN PSYCHOPHARMACOL, VOL 16/NO 1, FEBRUARY 1996
`
`Brauer and Associates
`
`individuals may take additional doses of the drug. The
`consequences of such a pattern may be dangerous be-
`cause subjective effects appear to dissipate at times
`when plasma levels and physiologic responses are still
`maximal or rising. Thus, individuals who repeatedly
`self-administer the drug in an attempt to maintain a cer-
`tain level of euphoria may be at increased risk for car-
`diovascular toxicity. These results also suggest that
`acute tolerance to the subjective effects of d-ampheta-
`mine may develop in drug-naive, normal volunteers in
`much the same manner as acute tolerance to cocaine's
`effects develops in cocaine abusers. Additional studies
`with d-amphetamine should be conducted to character-
`ize more fully the relationship between plasma levels
`and drug effects.
`
`Acknowledgment
`
`This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute
`on Drug Abuse (DA02812) and a Grant-in-aid of Research from Sigma
`Xi, The Scientific Research Society. The authors acknowledge the sup-
`port and expert assistance of the Clinical Research Center (USPHS
`grant MO1 RR00055). The authors are also extremely grateful to Dr.
`Daniel Luchins for his suggestions and support.
`
`References
`1. Dingemanse J, Danhof M, Breimer DD. Pharmacokinetic-phar-
`macodynamic modeling of CNS drug effects: an overview. Phar-
`macol Ther 1988;38:1-52.
`2. Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Assessment of abuse liability of stim-
`ulant drugs in humans: a methodological survey. Drug Alcohol
`Depend 1991;28:3-48.
`3. Farre M, Cami J. Pharmacokinetic considerations in abuse liabil-
`ity evaluation. Br J Addict 1991;86:1601-6.
`4. de Wit H, Bodker B, Ambre J. Rate of increase of plasma drug
`level influences subjective response in humans. Psychopharma-
`cology 1992;107:352-8.
`5. Javaid JI, Fischman MW, Schuster CR, Dekirmenjian H, Davis JM.
`Cocaine plasma concentration: relation to physiological and sub-
`jective effects in humans. Science 1978;292:227-8.
`6. Fischman MW, Schuster CR, Javaid JI, Hatano Y, Davis JM. Acute
`tolerance development to the cardiovascular and subjective ef-
`fects of cocaine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1985;235:667-82.
`7. Ambre JJ, Belknap SM, Nelson J, Ruo TI, Shin S, Atkinson Al.
`Acute tolerance to cocaine in humans. Clin Pharmacol Ther
`1988;44:1-8.
`
`8. Foltin RW, Fischman MW, Pedroso JJ, Pearlson GD. Repeated in-
`tranasal cocaine administration: lack of tolerance to pressor efr
`fects. Drug Alcohol Depend 1988;22:169-77.
`9. Foltin RW, Fischman MW. The relationship between cocaine ve-
`nous blood levels and the cardiovascular and subjective effects of
`smoked and intravenous cocaine. In Harris LS, ed. Problems of
`drug dependence 1990: proceedings of the 52nd Annual Scientific
`Meeting, Committee on Problems of Drug • Dependence, Inc.
`Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office, 1990:440-1.
`10. Ambre JJ. Acute tolerance to pressor effects of cocaine in hu-
`mans. Ther Drug Monit 1993;15:537-40.
`11. Brown WA, Corriveau DP, Ebert MH. Acute psychologic and neu-
`roendocrine effects of dextroamphetamine and methylphenidate:
`Psychopharmacology 1978;58:189-95.
`12. Dommisse CS, Schulz SC, Narasimhachari N, Blackard WG, Hamer
`RM. The neuroendocrine and behavioral response to dextroam-
`phetamine in normal individuals. Biol Psychiatry 1984;19:1305-15.
`13. Angrist B, Corwin J, Bartlik B, Cooper T. Early pharmacokinetics
`and clinical effects of oral d-amphetamine in normal subjects.
`Biol Psychiatry 1987;22:1357-68.
`14. Yonkers KA, Kando JC, Cole JO, Blumenthal SB. Gender differ-
`ences in pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of psy-
`chotropic medication. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149:587-95.
`15. Brauer LH, de Wit H. Subjective responses to d-amphetamine
`alone and after pimozide pretreatment in normal, healthy volun-
`teers. Biol Psychiatry (in press).
`16. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Profile of mood states (man-
`ual). San Francisco: Educational and Industrial Testing Service,
`1971.
`17. Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth E. Drug preference and mood in hu-
`mans: d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 1980;71:275-9.
`18. Haertzen CA, Hill HE, Belleville RE. Development of the Addic-
`tion Research Center Inventory (ARC1): selection of items that
`are sensitive to the effects of various drugs. Psychopharmacology
`1963;4:155-6.
`19. Martin WR, Sloan JW, Sapira JD, Jasinski DR. Physiologic, sub-
`jective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine; methampheta-
`mine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine, and methylphenidate in man.
`Clin Pharmacol Ther 1971;12:245-57.
`20. Fischman MW, Foltin RW. Utility of subjective-effects measure-
`ments in assessing abuse liability of drugs in humans. Br J Addict
`1991;86:1563-70.
`21. Wilkins D, Crouch D, Rollins D. Analysis of phenylpropanolamine
`by positive chemical ionization mass spectrometry [abstract
`K17]. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Academy
`of Forensic Science. Las Vegas, NV, 1989.
`22. Elkins K, Gibb J, Hanson G, Wilkins D, Johnson M. Effects of ni-
`modipine on the amphetamine- and methamphetamine-induced
`decrease in tryptophan hydroxylase activity. Eur J Pharmacol
`1993;250:395-402.
`23. Hoffman BB, Lefkowitz RJ. Catecholamines and sympath-
`omimetic drugs. In Gilman AG, Rall TW, Nies AS, Taylor P, eds.
`The pharmacological basis of therapeutics. New York: Pergamon
`Press, 1990:187-220.
`
`Page 7
`
`

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket