throbber

`
`TCL 1023, Page 1
`TCL 1023, Page 1
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`

`

`
`
`
`|318127995 || 00474
`
`
`
`is a free public library, maintained by the
`City of Pittsburgh and the County of Alle~
`gheny, with
`supplemental
`appropriations
`from the State of Pennsylvania
`
`
`
`
` iiiIBRA
`
`Carnegie Library of Pittsburgh
`
`]
`
`
`
`495
`~k8?
`10855
`
`AL Pr PT
`ert /
`[aye ease
`
`(1)
`
`
`
`FINES: Five cents a day on juvenile and adult cards for
`
`each book kept past due date. A borrower is respon-
`sible for books damaged while charged on his card,
`TCL 1023, Page 2
`TCL 1023, Page 2
`Books may not be renewed.
`DEMco
`
`
`Carnegie Library of Pittsburgh
`4400 Forbes Ave.
`Tel. 622-3131
`
`
`
`
` IF
`
`
`
`P
`
`

`

`HAZEL ROSSOTTI
`COLOUR
`
`Awow ko
`HAS
`
`Princeton University Press
`Princeton, NewJersey
`
`
`
`TCL 1023, Page 3
`
`TCL 1023, Page 3
`
`

`

`Published by Princeton University Press,
`41 William Street, Princeton, New Jersey 08540
`
`Copyright © 1983 by Hazel Rossotti
`All rights reserved
`First Pelican originaledition, 1983
`First Princeton Paperback printing, with corrections, 1985
`
`
`
`
`LOC 84-1145]
`ISBN 0-691 -08369-X
`ISBN 0-691 -02386 —7 (pbk.)
`
`Reprinted by arrangement with Penguin BooksLtd.
`Made andprinted in GreatBritain
`by Richard Clay (The ChaucerPress) Ltd,
`Bungay, Suffolk
`Set in VIP Times
`
`Clothboundeditions ofPrinceton University Press books are printed
`on acid-free paper, and binding materials are chosen for strength and durability.
`Paperbacks, while satisfactory for personal collections, are not usually suitable
`for library rebinding.
`
`CONTENTS
`
`List of Text Figures
`Foreword
`Preface to the Princeton Edition
`Introduction
`
`Part One: Light and Dark
`
`Ne
`
`. Light Particles
`. White Light on Clear Glass
`
`Part Two: Lights and Colours
`
`AunWw
`
`owes.
`
`. Steady Colours
`. Shimmering Colours
`. Special Effects
`. Lights
`
`Part Three: The Natural World
`
`. Air and Water
`. Earth and Fire
`. Vegetable Colours
`. The Colours of Animals
`
`Part Four: Sensations of Colour
`
`11
`. Light and the Eye
`12:
`Anomalous Colour Vision
`15,
`Colour Vision in Animals
`14.
`The Eye and the Brain
`15
`. Sorting and Recording
`
`Part Five: Technology
`
`16.
`17;
`
`Colour Reproduction
`Added Colour
`
`it
`12
`
`19
`26
`
`37
`
`48
`55
`
`65
`77
`84
`91
`
`109
`122
`126
`130
`143
`
`169
`185
`
`TCL 1023, Page 4
`TCL 1023, Page 4
`
`
`
`

`

`
`18. Imparting Information
`19. Communicating Feelings
`20. Colour, Music and Movement
`21. Words and Colours
`
`Index
`
`Acknowledgements
`
`
`
`
`
`
`
`203
`209
`220
`222
`
`231
`
`239
`
`LIST OF TEXT FIGURES
`
`COIAMRYN
`
`Light waves
`Prismatic colours
`A home-madeprism
`From a rose window
`How light travels through glass
`How light bounces off glass
`The dark eyes of houses
`A single atom of matter
`Glass
`A metal
`. Interference of light
`. Printer’s blue and blood red
`. Oil patches and soap bubbles
`. Diffraction by twoslits
`. Diffraction by a grating
`. Polarization of light
`. Three combs
`A highly ordered fluid
`. Light sources
`. Laserlight
`. Colours from white light
`. White light and tiny particles
`. Distant blue
`. White light and droplets
`- Reflected colour in clouds
`. The Blue Grotto
`. Sunset
`. Colours from a dewdrop
`. A primary rainbow
`. A double rainbow
`
`. The Brocken spectre
`. How a glory occurs
`. Shiny graphite
`. Fine blue, damp pink
`
`TCL 1023, Page 5
`
`TCL 1023, Page 5
`
`

`

` . Leaf green
`
`175
`178
`206
`208
`223
`225
`226
`
`TCL 1023, Page 6
`TCL 1023, Page 6
`
`List of Text Figures
`
`77. Phosphor dots
`78. Colour negatives and prints
`79. Code for resistors
`80. Coloured signals
`81. Evolution of colour words
`82. Heraldic tinctures
`83. Names for colours
`
`
`
`
`List of Text Figures
`
`. Carrot orange
`. Delphinium blue
`. The colourful juice of pickled red cabbage
`. Butterfly wings
`. Insect iridescence
`. Blue birds
`. How animals change colour
`. The human eye
`. A light twist
`. Changesin the retina
`. Brightness by night
`. Brightness by day
`. Cones and colour: a possible mechanism
`. Blue-green ambiguity
`. A bee’s-eye view?
`. Colours which recede or advance
`. The corner of the eye
`. Connections for contrast
`. Stereo
`. Brain waves
`. Coloured tops
`. Sidney Harry’s top
`. Colours in space
`. Ostwald’s coloursolid
`. Munsell’s tree
`. Comparison of saturation
`. Mixing lights
`. Labelling with filters
`. Tops for colour measurement
`. A good match?
`. Maxwell’s triangle
`. ‘Negative’ colours
`. CIE tongue diagram
`. Purple and white
`. Dominant wavelength
`. Colours and complementaries
`. Brightness
`. Some commoncolours
`. Colour and temperature
`. The flight of colours
`. Colour television
`
`

`

`
`
`142
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`him from the donkey at that particular momentthan to the average colour
`which the donkey would have if viewed by a layman in diffuse light.
`It is not surprising that the ability to look analytically can be cultivated by
`willpower and training; nor that those without training often prefer a
`naturalistic picture which represents colours as they remember them to
`those in whichtheartist tries to reproduce the actuallight coming from the
`object. In much the same way, an observerwill often select, as the most
`‘natural’ colour photograph, one in which the colours are actually brighter
`than those in the original scene. Colours selected to match memories from
`dreams, however, are often paler than those of similar objects we observe
`when awake.
`Colour sensations may often be even more dramatically altered by, or
`even produced by, factors other than light. Some forms of hysteria and
`hallucinogenic drugs enhance appreciation of colour: hallucinogens often
`provide experience of brightly coloured patterns which bearlittle relation
`to the real world, and those aboutto suffer attacks of epilepsy or migraine
`may see coloured rays and geometrical shapes before an attack. The blind,
`particularly if they are old, may have similar sensations, akin to seeing
`‘golden rain’ and coloured patterns. In childhood, a pressure on a closed
`eye was enough to produce patterns as exotic as Catherine wheels or
`peacocktails. In adult life, a firmer touch, preferably on the upperpart of
`the eye, is needed to produce an inferior but nonethe less impressive result.
`Electrical and mechanical stimulation of the optic nerve andthe visual areas
`of the brain can makeus see colours; so can some acuteillnesses, and evena
`strong magnetic field. And so, of course, can memory and dreams. But
`although we canexperience an immense variety of colour sensations pro-
`ducedin these different, non-visual, ways, we haveas yetonly a negligible
`understanding of any of the mechanismsinvolved.
`
`15
`
`SORTING AND RECORDING
`
`To what extent can we record a colour? Can we impose any order on our
`rich variety of colour sensations? If we can, would our schemebe entirely
`personal, or could we use it to communicate information about a colour?
`How can webesttell someonethe exact colour we should like the new door
`to be?
`As weshall see in Chapter 21, there are manydifficulties in trying to
`describe colours with words. A request to paint the door turquoise would be
`likely to produce a fairly bright door in the greenish-blue (or might it be
`bluish-green?) range, neither very pastel nor very murky. Perhaps we could
`get nearer to the colour we have in mind by a request to paint the wall the
`same colour as the curtains. But, though the two may seem a good match in
`one light, they may clash horribly in another (see page 152). And as they
`will have different textures, they will have different highlights; so although
`the colours of the two may ‘go’ very satisfactorily, they will certainly not
`matchall over, even if the light falling on them happensto be identical.It is
`safer to choose from samplesof the actual paint, on the manufacturer’s own
`colour card. But maybe the exact colour requiredis not available. ‘Please
`mix me...’ How does one continue? ‘Something between these two’;
`‘Something like this, only lighter’; ‘A more subtle shade of that’; ‘A bluer
`versionofthis one.’It is difficult to know howto specify, or how to produce,
`the colour required. And it may even be difficult to envisage precisely any
`colours which are not on the colour card.
`Perhapsit would help if we could arrange colours in somerational order
`and label them appropriately. We could then refer to them in much the
`same way as we can pinpoint a place by a map reference. Many child-hours
`must be passed in just this way, rearranging crayons, pastels or embroidery
`threads. It is easy enough to makea lineof the rainbow colours, and join the
`ends, through purple, to give a circle. But problems soon arise. Do black,
`grey and white count as colours? If they are to be included, where should
`they go? What should be donewith pale colours: primrose, duck-egg blue,
`salmon? And what about the browns? One can imagine a cross-roads at
`yellow, with primrose leading to white on one side, and ochre leading to
`khaki, brown and black on the other. Perhaps, however, black and white
`should be joined through the greys, to give anothercircle, perpendicular to
`
`TCL 1023, Page 7
`TCL 1023, Page 7
`
`

`

`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`145
`
`
`
`Yellow
`
`
`
`Pe
`
` 144
`
`brightness (or value). The so-called ‘natural’ or ‘achromatic’ colours, black,
`the first. It seems we cannot arrange our coloured objects onaflat surface,
`but need some three-dimensional scheme.
`grey and white, are of zero saturation, and differ from each other only in
`Maybeit would be better to seek a more‘scientific’ classification than
`brightness. A series obtained by adding onehue,say blue, to white differs
`any subjective arrangement of coloured materials? We might try to specify
`only in saturation, as does a series obtained by adding a blue pigmentto a
`the colour of a sample byirradiating it with the light of a large numberof
`grey one. If the same pigment were added to a white one in the same
`very narrow bands of wavelength and measuring the percentage of each
`proportions, the dusky, pale blue would differ from the clear pale blue only
`which the sample reflects. These measurements can be made extremely
`in brightness.
`easily, given the appropriate equipment. But the light which enters the eye
`There are many three-dimensional arrangements of colours, the best
`dependsonthe lighting as well as on the sample, so we would also need to
`knownbeing those devised by Munsell and by Ostwald. Both are based on
`know the composition of the illumination. Even this does nottell us the
`the colour circle formed by joining the two endsof the spectrum through
`colourof the sample unless we know howtheeye reacts to lightofdifferent
`purple (see Figure 58). So the hue changes around the circumference of the
`wavelengths. Two materials may match exactly underone type ofillumina-
`circle, much as the hours progress aroundthe face of a clock. Through the
`tion even if they sendlight oftotally different composition to the eye; we
`centre of the clock face, and perpendiculartoit, like the axle of a wheel,
`knowthat manyyellows can be matched by mixturesof red and greenlight.
`runsthe line representing the neutral colours, usually with white at the top,
`We might, however, combine, for each narrow band of wavelength,
`changing, through deepeninggreys, to black at the bottom. Radially, like
`measurementsofthe reflecting powers of the sample, and the composition
`spokes on a wheel, the saturation increases towardsthe rim, to give a space
`of the light source with our knowledge of the response of the retina.
`which can be filled in by different colours, according to which system is
`Although this procedurestill needs laboratory equipment,it relates the
`being used.
`scientific measurements of the light reaching us to our perceptions of
`colour, assuming that the observer has normalcolour vision, adapted for
`daylight viewing and uninfluenced by the effects (such as after-images,
`contrast of near-by colours, memory, expectation) which we discussed in
`the previous chapter. So we are attemptingto chart,not just the stimulusof
`the light entering the eye, but a normal observer’s response toit. The idea of
`attempting to measure colour in this way soundsattractive, if somewhat
`Vivid colour
`complex. But do its advantages always outweigh those of map references
`eco+»Saturation (chroma)
`with an ordered arrangement,albeit subjectively chosen and represented in
`three dimensions? Since both systems are usedin practice, we shall look at
`each in more detail.
`If we are to arrange a numberofcolours in any systematic order, we must
`decide whatqualities we shall use to sort them.Let usfirst recall the ways in
`which light can vary. The sensation of colour depends primarily on the
`composition of the light, and partly on the intensity; and the composition
`maybe usually described as a mixture, in a certain proportion,of white light
`with a ‘coloured’ light of a particular dominant wavelength within the
`visible spectrum. (For purplelight, the ‘coloured’ componentis itself a
`mixture.) We can describe the primary sensationof colourin terms ofhue,
`which refers to the greenness, blueness and so forth, and varies with any
`changein the dominant wavelength. The extent to which this wavelengthin
`fact dominatesthe light is knownas saturation (or chroma). As the domin-
`ant wavelength is diluted with white light, the saturation decreases. An
`increase in intensity in light of a particular composition increases the
`
`Green
`
`Blue
`
`
`
`
`Black
`
`Figure 58. Colours in space The skeleton ofa colour solid. The ‘achromatic’ colours
`formthe vertical backbonefrom which the different hues radiate: red in one direction,
`green in the opposite one, and the others in between, For any one hue, the colour
`becomes more vivid the fartherit is from the centre.
`
`Ostwald arranged his colours in a double cone, based on twenty-four
`different hues, arranged around the circumference (see Figure 59), Each
`hue is combined, in a number of fixed proportions, with each of eight
`
`TCL 1023, Page 8
`TCL 1023, Page 8
`
`
`
`

`

`146
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`147
`
`
`
`(a)
`
`11
`
`Full saturation at edge
`
`Planes of constant dominant
`wavelength (hue)
`
`numbered on a grid system, so that any colour contained by the solid can be
`specified by a map-reference.
`In Munsell’s arrangement, saturationis increased byaseries of visually
`equal steps rather than by addinga fixed proportion of pigment; and there
`are nine neutral colours, rather than eight. As the numberof equal steps of
`saturation at a particular hue and brightness dependson the hue, the arms
`are of different length in different parts of the solid. Munsell’s solid is
`therefore much less regular than Ostwald’s, and on account ofits untidy
`appearanceis knownas a colour‘tree’. A typical vertical section throughit
`is shown in Figure 60. Each position in the tree, as in Ostwald’s cone, is
`encoded, thereby allowing colours to be specified. And the tree has one
`great advantage over Ostwald’s solid: whenever some new,dazzling pig-
`ment is made, it may be incorporated by extending an existing branch.
`
`
`
`Hue (1)
`Hue(2)
`Full
`sina
`colour/(complementary to (1))
`
`(b)
`
` Full
`
`Grey axis
`
`Figure 59. Ostwald's colour solid (a) Exterior view, (b) Vertical section.
`(Adapted, with permission, from G.J. Chamberlin and D. G. Chamberlin, Colour: Its
`Measurement, Computation and Application, Heyden, London, 1 980.)
`equally spaced neutral colours from white to black. The resulting colours
`are arranged so that brightness decreases vertically towards the bottom of
`the diagram, while saturation decreases towardsthe centre. Thus Ostwald’s
`coloursolid consists of twenty-four triangles (one for each hue), arranged
`radially so that a vertical section through it gives two such triangles, for
`complementary hues, fused at the centre, Each position within the solid is
`
` Hue (1)
`
`Black
`
`Hue (2)
`(complementary to (1))
`
`Figure 60. Munsell’s tree Vertical section (cf. Figures 59(b), page 146, and 83, page
`226),
`
`The two systems resemble each otherin that the circumference is divided
`arbitrarily into hues, and the vertical axis is graduated into visually equal
`steps, which are obtained by asking large numbers of observers to estimate
`equal differences in brightness. But the two solids are based on different
`waysof varying saturation. While Ostwald usedthe ratio of neutral pigment
`to saturated pigment, Munsell again invoked visual assessment by the
`average observer.
`When Ostwald devised his colour solid, he was an old man whose sensi-
`tivity to blue was doubtless declining, which accounts for the slight com-
`pression in the blue region of the circumference. Swedish workers have
`attempted to correct this defect in Ostwald’s system by placing each of
`the four psychological primaries at right angles to one another on the
`
`TCL 1023, Page 9 —
`
`TCL 1023, Page 9
`
`

`

`149
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`{b)
`
`
`
`
`
`Transparentplunger——
`
`|easetSeasesEEE
`,
`;
`.
`th
`fa
`cstancaael
`vil
`eviigine’
`Figure 61. Comparison of saturation A fixed depth ofthe sample is viewed with one
`eye and a variable length ofstandard with the other. The geometry ofthe instrumentis
`adjusted until the same depth of colour is observed by each eye.
`(a) Sample.
`(b) Variation of depth of standard by means oftransparent plunger.
`(c) Variation of depth of standard by use of wedge.
`(d) Split field, one halffrom each eyepiece.
`In the top two diagrams, the same depth used for sample and standard gives a darker
`field on the right. In the lower diagram,the length(1) ofstandard has beenadjusted so
`that the two fields are indistinguishable. In this example, d= 21, indicating that the
`Saturation of the sample is half that of the standard.
`
`
`
`Horizontalacan
`
`
`
`MovingeyepieceM
`
`\,
`
`
`
`
`
`FixedeyepieceF| Fixeddepth,d,ofsampleE
`
`{c)
`
`(d)
`
`(a)
`
`|
`
`circumference and dividing the segments between them intovisually equal
`steps, constituting a more ‘natural’ colour circle for use as the basis of a
`three-dimensional coloursolid.
`Althougha colour solid is a useful concept, and may even be constructed
`as a display objectof great visual and intellectual appeal, either swatches or
`books are more convenient for everyday use. Colour atlases, such as the
`Munsell Book ofColour, often represent vertical sections of a coloursolid,
`cut through each hue represented. For more specialist use, a restricted
`range of colours, varying by smaller gradations, may be reproducedas in
`collections for those who wish to specify the precise colour of a rose petal or
`a sample of humanskin or tooth.
`Such visual matching of colour is a stepwise process, a placing of the
`sample of unknown specifications between two standard colours whose
`specifications are known. But how can wetry to measure the colour specifi-
`cations of a material if we have no standard colour which matches it? What
`can we do to try to measure ‘colour’, to produce specifications of hue,
`saturation and brightness?
`Since colour is a sensation, thereis a lot to be said for the measurements
`being made by the eye. The humaneyeis, in fact, an excellent detector of
`differences of hue, and many people can assess the percentage of red, blue
`and yellow in a pigment with surprising precision. But humanestimates of
`saturation, and of brightness, are muchless reliable.
`The mostprecise visual methodsof attemptingto specify colours,like the
`use of a colour atlas, involve matching. The simplest are those devised
`merely to measuresaturation, as in the determination of the concentration
`of a single coloured componentin a liquid (see Figure 61).
`If the two solutions appear to be the same colour, the ratio of their
`concentrations is simply related to the ratio of the length of the two
`solutions through which the light has passed; and this may easily be found
`using a simple comparator involving either a plunger or a wedge.
`
`TCL 1023, Page 10
`
`TCL 1023, Page 10
`
`

`

`
`
`Sorting and Recording
`151
`magenta, one yellow and one blue-green (see Figure 63). Sets of suchfilters
`are available commercially for use in an instrument equipped with a stan-
`dard light source and knownasthe Lovibond Tintometer. Thefull range of
`250filters of different depth for each of the three hues allows nearly nine
`million different colours to be obtained,
`including the full range of
`achromatic colours from white to black. The colourof the sampleis readily
`specified in terms of the three filters used to match it.
`MBG Y
`
`150
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`For many purposes, however, we need to know the hue, as well as the
`saturation. We again compare our sample with a colour which we can
`specify. But how can wevary this colour whilststill being able to specify it?
`One way is to mix coloured lights of known wavelength in known pro-
`portions. A simple arrangementis shownin Figure 62. The amountofred,
`blue and green light can be varied by horizontaland vertical movement of
`the filter assembly over the source of light, and the three lights are then
`mixed by diffusion and multiple reflections. More sophisticated devices,
`used mainly for research on colour,
`involve six lights of varying
`wavelengths. In each case the mixture of coloured lights, shone on to a
`white background,is matched with the unknown sample, illuminated from
`a standard source of white light.
`Alternatively, the sample may be matched by a patch of colouredlight
`which has been obtained by passing white light through threefilters, one
`
`
`
`Ae2773N
`
`
`
` Red filter
`
`
`N
`
`Greenfilter
`
`Figure 62. Mixing lights The required mixture of red, blue and green light may be
`— by adjusting that area ofeach filter which lies overtheaperture to the mixing
`OX.
`
`Standard
`
`
`white lightNN 1—
`
`Sample
`Figure 63. Labelling with filters The colour ofthe sample can be matched with that of
`light which has passed through three Lovibondfilters, magenta (M), blue-green (BG)
`and yellow (Y) ofspecified strength. A purplish blue sample, for example, would need
`a deep magenta (to absorb mostofthe green), a medium blue-green (to absorb some,
`but notall, ofthe red), and yellow ofappropriate depth to reduce the intensity ofthe
`colour to that of the sample.
`Instead of matchingthe light which reaches us from a sample with that
`from an unknown,we can exploit the phenomenonof persistence ofvision
`and match only the sensations.Split discs, coloured in saturated blue, green
`and red, are placed on a revolving platform in such a way that the pro-
`portions of the three colours can be varied (see Figure 64). When the
`platform is spun at high speed, the sensations merge, just as if three
`coloured lights were superimposed. The areas of the three colours are
`adjusted until the colour of the spinning disc exactly matches that of the
`sample, which can then be expressed in terms of the proportionsof primary
`colours used.
`Nowadays, there is an increasing tendency to use photoelectric instru-
`ments for colour matching, instead of the eye of one or twoindividual
`observers. Such instruments monitor narrow bandsover the whole visible
`spectrum,first detecting how muchlight in each band reaches them from
`TCL 1023, Page 11
`TCL 1023, Page 11
`
`

`

`
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`153
`
`152
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`the sample, and then converting this information into the size of the
`stimulus which bombardseach of the three cone systemsin a normal human
`eye. Finally, the responses to these three stimuli are combinedto give the
`colour experienced by the ‘standard observer’. Inescapably, these instru-
`ments give results based on sensations experienced as a consequence of
`humanvision. But the vision is that, not of a few individuals, but of the
`“standard observer’ built up from observations made bya large numberof
`individuals selected for their normality of vision. But even when the match-
`ing is done, rather approximately, by eye, it is seldom left for a single
`individual: more often two, or even three, observers are used.
`
`(a)
`
`(b) oa
`
`
`
`Figure 64. Tops for colour measurement(a) Split circular disc ofpaper ofstandard
`colour. (b) Standard green, blue and red interlocked, exposing known areas ofeach for
`colour mixing when the disc is rotated.
`
`Matching, whether by eye or machine, often gives different results with
`different sourcesof light; and as matching implies identity only ofresponse,
`this is no surprise. Two extreme examples ofidentical colours produced by
`light of very different composition were given in Table 3 (page 119).
`Imagine a pair of yellow pigments,one reflecting light of only 580 nm and
`one reflecting only light of 540 nm and 630 nm, eachat half the intensity of
`the first pigment. If both were illuminated with light containing equal
`intensities of the three wavelengths, the two pigments would match exactly.
`But if they were illuminated with light containing a slightly higher pro-
`portion of longer wavelengths, the second pigment would look redder than
`the first. Such ‘metameric’ pigments, which match only under onelight
`source, are the bane of those who try to match clothes and accessories, or
`paint and fabrics (see Figure 65). Metamerism accounts for much of the
`popularity of ‘colour coordinated’ goods sold by the same manufacturer
`
`whouses dyessuch that any changein illumination causes almost the same
`change in colour for the different materials.
`Colour solids and atlases can give us no information about the compost-
`tion of the light which causesa particular colour. For charts relating colour
` 016
`
`Relative
`
`O12
`
`reflectance
`
`400
`
`500
`
`600
`
`700
`
`Figure 65. A good match? Thereflectance spectra oftwo fabrics which match perfectly
`in daylight. (Reproduced, with permission, from W. D. Wright, The Measurementof
`Colour, 4th edition, Adam Hilger, London, 1969.)
`Green
`
`Wavelength (nm)
`
`
` Unsaturated greenish-yellow
` Blue-green
`
`Blue-violet
`Magenta
`Orange-red
`Figure 66. Maxwell's triangle Shows how many, but notall, colours can be represented
`as a mixture of three primarycoloured lights. The nearer a point ts to an apex of the
`triangle the higheris the proportionoflight ofthe colour represented by that apex. The
`point X (50 per cent green, 35 per cent orange-red and 15 per cent blue-violet)
`represents an unsaturated greenish yellow.
`
`TCL 1023, Page 12
`TCL 1023, Page 12
`
`

`

`154
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`155
`
`+25
`
`+20
`
`+15
`
`
`
`Green (546)
`
`
`
`These ‘positive’ colours
`when mixed together
`
`to composition, we turn to the second, more‘scientific’ approach. Just as
`
` Blue (436)
`manycolour solids are based onaring ofspectral colours, joined through
`purple, a triangle usually forms the basis of attempts to chart the colours
`Red (700)
`produced by the mixing of lights. As early as 1855, Maxwell found that a
`great numberof colours could be produced by mixing lights of only the
`three ‘primary’ colours: orange-red, green and blue-violet. The colour
`resulting from a particular mixture can be represented by a point on a
`triangular grid (see Figure 66). Many colours can be specified in this way:
`but not all. Whichever three primary sources we choose, there are always
`somecolours (including many pure spectral ones) which cannot be rep-
`resented by a point in, or on, the triangle; which confirms that we cannot
`always match one colour by a mixture of three others unless we allow
`ourselves the option of mixing one of the primary colours with the sample
`and then matching the result with a mixture of the two otherlights. Figure
`67 gives the recipe for obtaining a matchfor every visible wavelength with
`three primaries. Thus vivid yellow (570 nm) (cf. page 119) can never be
`exactly matched by red (700 nm) and green (546 nm); but if a little blue
`(436 nm) is added to the yellow, a perfect match can be made. We can
`express this algebraically by stating that vivid yellow can be matchedbyred,
`green and a small negative amount of blue. But since there is no scope for
`plotting negative contributions on a Maxwell triangle, colours such asvivid
`yellow cannot be represented onit.
`It is too bad that we cannot choose any three wavelengths which, when
`themselves mixed together, will produce all visible colours. But there is
`nothing to stop us imagining that such ideal primary colours might exist;
`‘and if they did, they could be mixed in such a way as to produce three
`convenient teal primaries, such as Maxwell used. So we could draw a
`mathematical modification of Maxwell's triangle, with our three imaginary
`primaries at the corners. Points for the real ones, and for all other colours,
`would then be within it. Three such imaginary primaries have indeed been
`devised, such that any real colours can be represented as a mixture of the
`appropriate amounts of the three of them, and plotted as an idealized
`version of the Maxwell triangle. But not everyone is used to triangular
`graphs and the ruled paper may not be easy to obtain. Could we not use
`squared paper instead? How manyvariables do we need? Since we have
`decreed that a real colour R can be matched by a mixture of our three
`Imaginary Primary Lights—say, I units ofone, P units of the second and L of
`the third —it might look as though we need thethreevariables, I, P, and L, to
`specify R. But we could also say that the brightness ofthelight is the sum S
`of the three primaries (so S=1+P+L), mixed in theratio of 1/S:P/S:L/S.
`And if we know I/S and P/S we already know L/S, because
`1/S+P/S+L/S=1. Since we are often more interested in colour than in
`
`
`
`+1-0
`
`+05
`
`Pos
`0 a
`
`MATCH
`
`-05
`
`mixture of pure patch and
`these ‘negative’ colours
`
`500
`
`400
`
`700
`600
`Wavelength of pure patchoflight (nm)
`Figure 67. ‘Negative’ colours Many pure spectral colours can be exactly matched with
`three primaries only by use of‘negative’ colours. When oneprimary is mixed with the
`‘pure’ patch oflight, this mixture can be matched by some mixture 0fithe other two. For
`theprimary lights used here, the only colour which can be matched directly by the three
`primaries is a greenish yellow in the region of550.nm. (Reproduced, with permission,
`from F. W.Billmeyer and M.Saltzman, Principles of Color Technology,Interscience,
`New York, 1966, p. 33.)
`brightness, we could concentrate on the two quantities I/S and P/S which
`specify the colour. We could then use ordinary squared graph paper. If we
`ever needed to know L/S,it would be easy enoughto calculateit. And if we
`decided that, after all, we wanted to specify the brightness, we could
`representit on an axisrising vertically, out of the paper. The final diagram
`would be muchlike a plan, with the two specifications of colour running
`north-south and east-west; or,if brightness is added,like a map which also
`showsheights, or isobars (or any third variable) superimposed onthe plan
`as a series of contours.
`ae
`So all that we need in order to specify the colour of an object is a
`knowledge of the composition of the lightfalling on it, of the modification
`
`TCL 1023, Page 13
`
`TCL 1023, Page 13
`
`

`

`
`
`156
`
`Sensations of Colour
`
`Sorting and Recording
`
`157
`
`of the light by it, of the response of the normal humaneye, andof the
`quantities of three imaginary primary lights which would produce the same
`response; and a piece of ordinary graph paper. The Commission Inter-
`nationale de l’Eclairage (CIE), in 1931, defined the standard observer and
`three possible standard sources; and they produced tables showing the
`relationship between the observers’ response and the quantities of the
`imaginary primary lights which wouldin theory be needed to producethem.
`
`?
`
`The tongue-shaped curve(see Figure 68) in the graph showsthe specifica- 100% primary P
`
`‘ideal green’
`
`p
`
`08
`
`0-6
`
`0-4
`
`02
`
`0
`100%primaryL
`—_
`:
`Figure 68. CIE tongue diagram The tongue enclosesall visible colours, with the pure
`spectral ones lying alongits curved edge. The inner triangle encloses those colours
`obtainable by mixing real primaries 436 nm, 546nm, 700 nm (i.e. those enclosed in
`Maxwell's triangle of Figure 66, page 153).
`
`0-8
`100%primary | Siete?
`
`0
`
`:
`
`:
`
`08
`
`Figure 69, Purple and white The point Efor p =0-33, i =0-33 (and sol =4=0-33)is
`‘equal energy’ white. Purples lie along the baseline (see text).
`
`tions of the pure spectral colours as their values of i=I/S and p=P/S.
`(Modified definitions of the standard observer and the standard sources,
`introduced by the CIE in 1967, change only-details on the graph.) The
`diagram, known as a CIE chromaticity curve, has been used to depict
`colours, and the relationship between them, in a wide variety ofsituations.
`ee
`s usefulness of the CIE diagrams arises from the fact that
`The enormoususefulness ¢
`8
`a
`mi
`:
`i
`i
`e
`line joining
`the
`ce
`mecan representa mixture of twolights as a point on cation :
`points which specify them. So pu rples, formed by HAINTed and
`Clue, Ne On
`the base-line of the ‘tongue’ (see Figure 69). A mixture of two parts red
`
`|
`
`TCL 1023, Page 14
`TCL 1023, Page 14
`
`

`

`
`
`
`
`0-4
`
`o2
`
`0
`
`02
`
`04
`
`06
`
`08
`
`Pre
`en
`dominant wavelength D with white light of composition represented by E.
`Tofind which is the dominant wavelength, we rememberthat X mustlie on
`a line joining E and D.So we drawaline from E to X and continueit untilit
`meets the curve, at the dominant

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket