throbber
24-29 April1993
`
`INTIRCHI’
`
`A One-handed Keyboard Facilitating
`Half-QWERTY:
`Skill Transfer From QWERTY
`
`Edgar Matiasf,
`
`I. Scott Mackenzie, William Buxton$
`
`tl%e Matias
`Corporation
`Boulevard
`178 Thistledown
`Canada
`M9V
`Rexdale,
`Ontario,
`(416)
`749-3124
`ematias@dgp.toronto.edu
`
`lK1
`
`3Dept.
`
`Guelph,
`824-4120
`
`(519)
`
`and Information
`of Computing
`of Guelph
`University
`2W1
`Canada
`NIG
`Ontario,
`mac@snowhite.cis.uoguelph.ca
`
`Science
`
`Xerox
`of Toronto&
`*university
`Research
`Systems
`c/o Computer
`of Toronto
`University
`Canada
`M5S
`Ontario,
`Toronto,
`(416)
`978-1961
`buxton@dgp.toronto.
`
`PARC
`Institute
`
`1A4
`
`edu
`
`ABSTRACT
`technique,
`typing
`is a new one-handed
`Half-QWERTY
`typing skill
`designed to facilitate
`the transfer of
`two-handed
`to the one-handed condition.
`It
`is performed
`on a standard
`keyboard, or a special half keyboard (with full-sized
`keys).
`In an experiment
`using touch typists, hunt-and-peck
`typing
`speeds were surpassed after 3-4 hours of practice. Subjects
`reached 50% of their
`two-handed
`typing speed after about 8
`hours. After 10 hours, all subjects typed between 41 % and
`73% of
`their
`two-handed
`speed, ranging from 23.8 to 42.8
`wpm.
`These results are important
`in providing
`access to
`disabled users, and for
`the design of compact
`computers.
`They also bring into question p;evious re;earch
`cl~ming
`finger
`actions if
`one ‘hand map to the other
`via spatial
`congruence rather
`than mirror
`image.
`
`tasks,
`input
`devices,
`Input
`K E Y WO R D S:
`keyboard, QWERTY,
`performance,
`one-handed
`computers, disabled users, skill
`transfer.
`
`human
`portable
`
`INTRODUCTION
`The idea of a one-handed keyboard is not new. As early as
`1968, Engelbart
`and English
`[2] used a one-handed
`chord
`keybomd
`in conjunction
`with
`a newly
`developed
`input
`device — the mouse. The user entered text with one hand,
`while using the mouse to enter spatial
`information
`with the
`other.
`However,
`unlike
`the mouse, acceptance
`of one-
`handed
`keyboards
`has been limited
`to very
`specific
`
`is
`this material
`of
`fee all or part
`without
`to oopy
`Permission
`for
`the copies
`are not made or distributed
`granted
`provided
`that
`direct
`commercial
`advantage,
`the ACM copyright
`notice
`and tha
`title
`the publication
`and its date
`appear,
`and notice
`is given
`of
`that
`copying
`is by permission
`of
`the Association
`for Computing
`Machinery.
`To copy
`otherwise,
`or
`to republish,
`requires
`a fee
`and/or
`specific
`permission.
`
`~ 1993
`
`ACM 0-89791
`
`-575-51931000410088
`
`. ..S 1.50
`
`88
`
`There are
`the disabled.
`such as keyboards for
`applications,
`several
`reasons for
`this, but chief among them is the need
`to learn a new typing
`technique.
`For most people,
`the
`benefit of
`touch typing with one hand is not worth the cost
`of
`learning to do it.
`
`text
`to one-handed
`This paper describes a new approach
`entry which exploits
`the skills already developed
`in two-
`handed typing.
`It
`is called,
`“Half-QWERTY~
`because it
`uses only half of
`the QWERTY
`keyboard.
`The technique
`can be used on an unmodified
`standard QWERTY keyboard
`(using only half of
`the available keys, Figure
`1), or with a
`special half keyboard (Figures 2 & 3). The former provides
`wide access to the technique,
`The latter provides a compact
`keyboard with full-sized
`keys supporting
`touch typing on
`portable computers,
`for example.
`
`the degree to which
`study examines
`The present
`transfers from QWERTY to Half-QWERTY
`keyboards.
`
`skill
`
`CONCEPT1
`THE HALF-QWERTY
`Most one-handed
`keyboards
`are chord keyboards.
`QWERTY
`is not. The design builds on two principles:
`
`Half-
`
`1.
`
`2.
`
`A user’s abflity to touch type on a standard
`QWERTY keyboard.
`The fact
`that the human hands are symmetrical —
`one hand is a mirror
`image of the other.
`
`the keys
`of all
`is comprised
`keyboard
`A Half-QWERTY
`typed by one hand, with the keys of
`the other hand unused
`
`#
`International Application
`pending.
`lPatents
`PCT/CA90/00274 published March 21, 1991, under
`International Publication
`# W091/03782.
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`lNTkRcHr93
`
`24-29 April1993
`
`!
`
`1(J
`
`@
`29
`
`#
`38
`
`$
`47
`
`77
`56
`
`Tab
`
`Q
`
`w
`
`E
`
`R
`
`T
`
`Delete
`
`p
`
`o
`
`I
`
`u
`
`65
`
`Y
`
`74
`
`T
`
`83
`
`92
`
`01
`
`:
`
`=
`
`u
`
`I
`
`~
`
`~ Ow
`
`p
`
`;
`
`]
`
`;
`
`Shift
`
`/
`
`“
`
`*
`
`M
`
`N
`
`~ J
`
`KD
`
`L~
`
`Return
`
`N
`
`;
`
`B
`
`>
`<
`v $C .
`
`x /z
`
`Shift
`
`layouts on a standard QWERTY keyboard.
`
`Figure
`
`1. Left- and right-hand Half-QWERTY
`
`the missing
`or absent. When the space bar is depressed,
`characters are mapped onto the remaining
`keys in a mirror
`image
`(Figure
`1), such that
`the typing
`hand makes
`movements
`homologous
`to those previously
`performed
`by
`the other hand, Thus, using the space bar as a modifier,
`a
`typist can generate the characters of either side of a full-
`sized keyboard using only one hand.
`
`a timeout
`the space bar within
`and releasing
`Depressing
`reduces
`the
`a space character.
`The timeout
`generates
`number of erroneous
`spaces generated as a side-effect
`of
`using the space bar as a modifier
`key.
`It
`is often the case
`that a typist will depress the space bar with the intention
`of
`
`then change their
`key but
`the state of another
`mirroring
`such actions would
`mind and release. Without
`the timeout,
`result
`in an unwanted space character. For this experiment,
`the timeout was 16/60 seconds, or 267 ms.
`
`keys (such as shift and control) are supported via a
`Modifier
`“latch” mechanism,
`commonly
`known
`as “Sticky
`Keys.”
`Depressing and releasing a modifier
`key once activates it for
`the next key pressed. Depressing
`it
`twice locks that key
`until
`it
`is unlocked
`by depressing
`it again.
`Sticky Keys
`allows
`one finger
`to do the work
`of
`several,
`when
`performing
`key sequences that would otherwise require the
`simultaneous
`depression of
`two or more keys.
`
`#w
`\
`
`Tab
`
`Return
`
`Shift
`
`!Q @
`) 2(
`— 1
`o
`
`Delete
`
`P
`
`A
`
`II
`I
`
`i
`
`.
`
`#
`3*
`
`$
`4&
`
`?40
`5A
`
`+
`=}
`
`E*
`
`R’
`
`T6
`
`o
`
`I
`
`u
`
`Y
`
`s
`
`D
`
`F
`
`G
`
`Delete
`
`Option
`
`1
`
`I
`\{
`
`[
`Control
`
`z’
`
`XL
`
`CK
`
`VJ
`
`BH
`
`Alt
`
`?
`I
`
`>
`.
`
`<
`,
`
`M
`
`N
`
`portable keyboard (actual size). When a key is depressed,
`2. Left-hand
`Figure
`of the key is entered. When preceded by holding down the spacebar,
`the character
`
`the character
`in the lower
`
`left
`in the upper
`right
`is entered.
`
`89
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`24-29 April1993
`
`lNTiiRctlr9
`
`IyTl”J’
`I
`
`,
`
`}-
`
`IOption
`IControl
`IAlt
`
`m
`
`..-
`
`,..
`
`-
`
`I R
`
`v
`
`m
`
`m
`
`. . ,
`
`m
`
`Which Hand to Use?
`Given the keyboard described above, we must now decide
`which hand is ‘best’
`for one-handed typing.,
`In general, we
`believe this is the non-dominant
`hand. This would free the
`more dexterous
`dominant
`hand to use a mouse (or other
`device)
`to enter spatial
`information.
`This arrangement
`would work e~ecially
`well on a palmtop
`computer.
`For
`example,
`the computer
`could
`open horizontally,
`like
`a
`wallet
`(Figure 4),
`thus keeping the keyboard comfortably
`to
`the side (where the hand is) and the screen in the centre
`(where the eyes are).
`If equipped with a touch screen,
`concurrent entry of
`text and graphics is possible. Note also
`that the left-hand and right-hand
`versions of Half-QWERTY
`are physically
`identical
`(Figures 2 & 3), differing
`only in
`their key cap markings
`and the mappings.
`So, a left-hand
`typist can easily adapt a right-hand
`keyboard for
`left hand
`use, and vice versa.
`
`using
`can be performed
`typing
`two-handed
`Furthermore,
`together.
`This has the added
`two of
`these half keyboards
`benefit
`of allowing
`the user
`to adjust
`each keypad
`indeWndently
`to whichever position is most comfortable.
`
`Computers
`Wearable
`than camied, has significant
`rather
`that
`is worn,
`A computer
`advantages for data collection
`“in the field.”
`By eliminating
`infrequently
`used keys (e.g.,
`the number keys) and reducing
`the size of
`the space bar, a Half-QWERTY
`keyboard can be
`made small enough to wear on the wrist of
`the dominant
`hand. With
`an LCD screen worn on the other wrist,
`the
`resulting
`typing posture allows
`the user to type and view
`the screen, simultaneously.
`Note that
`this arrangement
`is
`consistent
`with
`the convention
`of wearing
`one’s wrist
`watch on the non-dominant
`arm.
`
`allowing
`portable,
`be extremely
`Such a computer would
`or other
`the need of a table
`fast data entry without
`supporting
`surface required by most computers today. Data
`could even be entered while standing or walking.
`
`vs. Spatial Congruence
`Hand Symmetry
`the human
`is based on the principle
`that
`Half-QWERTY
`typing movements
`according
`to the finger
`brain controls
`used, rather
`than the spatial position
`of
`the key. Thus,
`the
`finger
`used to hit a key is the critical
`invariant — the
`critical
`similarity
`that
`is maintained
`across the training
`and
`transfer
`tasks — in the transfer of skill
`from QWERTY
`to
`Half-QWERTY.
`Lintem [81 writex
`
`keyboard. When a
`portable
`3. Right-hand
`Figure
`in the upper left of the
`key is depressed,
`the character
`key is entered. When preceded by holding down the
`space bar,
`the character
`in the lower
`right
`is entered.
`
`Implementation
`and
`Application
`a
`objective
`of
`this design was to establish
`The original
`keyboard
`for palmtop
`computerx
`one that was small yet
`permitted
`touch typists
`to use their existing
`skills.
`Prior
`efforts
`tended toward reducing
`the size and spacing of
`the
`keys of standard QWERTY
`[18].
`Such attempts
`are
`problematic
`since they lead to keyboards
`that are too small
`to accommodate
`two hands. We have side-stepped this by
`requiring
`only one hand for
`typing.
`However,
`the idea is
`versatile, and has more applications.
`
`in one hand, and a
`keyboard
`a Half-QWERTY
`Using
`in the other
`recaptures
`device,
`such as a mouse,
`pointing
`and English’s
`system
`the two-handed
`flavour
`of Engelbart
`[2]. Text can be entered with one hand, and items selected
`and manipulated
`with the other.
`Since both hands aw in
`“home position”
`for
`their
`respective task, no time is lost
`in
`moving
`between devices.
`Furthermore,
`by implementing
`the Half-QWERTY
`keyboard on a standard keybomd,
`one
`can easily switch between this type of input and two-handed
`typing.
`Finally,
`since each side of
`the keybotcud is mapped
`onto the other side when the space bar is depressed,
`the user
`can choose which hand to use for one-handed
`typing.
`In
`effect,
`the user has a choice of
`three keyboards
`in on~ a
`two-handed QWERTY
`keyboard,
`and two Half-QWERTY
`keyboards, one for each hand. All of this we have achieved
`entirely
`in software.
`This
`is especially
`beneficial
`to
`disabled
`computer
`users, since it obviates
`the need for
`specialized hardware.
`
`m““’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’’”;’
`n )“’’’’’’’’’:
`
`left- and right-hand
`for
`screen placements
`4. Various
`Figure
`palmtop computers equipped with Half-QWERTY
`keyboards.
`
`90
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`lNlfRCH1’93
`
`24-29 April1993
`
`those that pose a
`(specifically,
`invariants
`If critical
`remain unchanged,
`learning challenge)
`meaningful
`[skill]
`transfer will be high even when many other
`features of
`the environment,
`context,
`or
`task are
`changed ... If an operator’s perceptual
`sensitivity
`to
`critical
`invariants
`can be improved,
`that enhanced
`sensitivity will serve to facilitate
`transfer.
`(p. 262)
`
`above)
`scheme (described
`encoding
`image
`The mirror
`encoding
`scheme is that of
`A rival
`follows
`from this.
`which maintains
`that
`the spatial
`spatial
`congruence,
`position
`of
`the key is the critical
`invariant.
`There is
`disagreement
`in the literature as to which of
`these schemes
`is ‘better.’
`In the context of
`this experiment,
`we believe
`mirror
`image mapping is preferred.
`
`in his analysis of error patterns in transcription
`Grudin [7],
`typing,
`found
`that homologous
`substitution
`errors
`are
`among the most common
`errors.
`These occur when the
`character corresponding
`to the mirror
`image position on the
`keyboard,
`is substituted for
`the one required.
`For example,
`substituting
`D for K (middle
`finger
`of either hand)
`is a
`homologous
`error. These findings, which were confined
`by Munhall
`and Ostry [10], suggest a predisposition
`among
`QWERTY
`typists to mirror
`image mapping.
`
`chord keyboard,
`of a one-handed
`the evaluation
`During
`and Sh~
`[17]
`trained one student
`Rochester, Bequaert,
`using the right hand only.
`The subject was later
`retrained
`to type with the left hand only. The subject
`“reached close
`to his right-hand
`typing
`speed in less than one third the
`time he spent
`learning right-handed
`typing”
`(p. 62). Their
`left-hand
`keyboard was a mirror
`image of
`the right-hand
`version.
`
`trained subjects on a two-
`[5]
`Gopher, Karis, and Koenig
`handed chord keyboard
`and then investigated whether
`the
`skill
`thus acquired transferred
`to the other hand by mirror
`image or spatial
`congruence.
`Their
`conclusions
`suggest
`that spatial
`congruence
`is the dominant mapping.
`They
`also tested a third condition,
`a combination
`of
`the two,
`using keyboards mounted vertically
`rather than horizontally.
`Hand-to-hand mapping was best in this condition.
`
`than
`congruence was stronger
`This suggests that spatial
`mirror
`image mapping, which would
`seem to contradict
`what we have argued above. However,
`closer
`inspection
`reveals
`that
`the combined
`scheme was actually
`the
`equivalent of
`the mirror
`image keyboard, but with a vertical
`rather
`than flat posture (i.e., with the hands positioned
`as
`though playing a saxophone, as opposed to a piano).
`
`to keep
`[5]
`despite the efforts of Gopher et al.
`Furthermore,
`error
`rates low,
`the errors
`that did slip
`through were
`primarily
`homologous
`errors made by subjects using the
`This
`suggests
`a
`spatial
`congruence
`keyboard.
`predisposition
`among chord keybomd
`typists
`to mirror
`image mapping.
`
`section, we describe
`In the following
`the degree to which
`skill
`intended
`to test
`QWERTY
`to Half-QWERTY
`keyboardy
`touch typists.
`
`an experiment
`transfers
`from
`among skilled
`
`METHOD
`
`Subjects
`Ten right-handed,
`a local university
`their non-dominant
`The Edinburgh
`handedness.
`
`literate, QWERTY typists from
`computer
`served as paid volunteers.
`Subjects used
`(left) hand when typing with one hand.
`Inventory
`[13] was used to determine
`
`Equipment
`11 and IIci
`on Apple Macintosh
`Tasks were performed
`A cardboard
`computers using a standard Apple keybomd.
`shield was placed between the keyboard
`and the subjects’
`eyes in order to pwent
`them from looking at the keyboard.
`
`Procedure
`10 sessions, with no more than one
`Each subject performed
`session per day.
`Each session contained
`a two-handed
`pretest, multiple
`blocks of one-handed
`typing,
`and a two-
`handed post-test. The fwst session included a few specially
`prepared one-handed blocks, designed to ease subjects into
`understandin~
`the oDeration
`of
`the keyboard.
`All
`one-
`handed typin~was
`p;tionned
`with the le~ hand.
`
`Measure
`
`Hands
`
`1
`
`2
`
`3
`
`4
`
`Session
`6
`
`5
`
`7
`
`8
`
`9
`
`10
`
`Speed
`(wpm)
`
`Errors
`(%)
`
`;
`
`1
`2
`
`13.2
`58.5
`
`18.3
`59.8
`
`21.1
`62.3
`
`24.4
`61.6
`
`27.1
`63.7
`
`15.96 12.13
`3.40
`3.25
`
`9.93
`2.45
`
`9.70
`3.05
`
`9.21
`3.40
`
`29.0
`63.3
`
`8.98
`3.55
`
`30.7
`64.0
`
`7.55
`3.55
`
`31.6
`64.6
`
`8.23
`3.55
`
`33.6
`66.2
`
`7.54
`3.30
`
`34.7
`64.9
`
`7.44
`4.20
`
`scores for speed and accuracy
`Figure 5. Mean performance
`on one-handed and two-handed typing over 10 sessions.
`
`91
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`24-29 April1993
`
`lNTfRtH1’9
`
`50-
`
`45-
`
`40-
`
`35-
`
`30-
`
`25-
`
`20-
`
`15-
`
`10-
`
`Subject
`
`--1
`+-2
`43
`+4
`-9-5
`46
`*7
`-A-8
`+9
`+
`
`10
`
`5-
`
`0!
`
`-1
`
`I
`01234567
`
`1
`
`I
`
`I
`
`I
`
`I
`
`I
`
`i
`8910
`
`I
`
`(
`
`Session
`
`Figure
`
`6. One-handed typing speed by subject and session.
`
`IV1 with
`to that of Typing Tutor
`The interface was similar
`text. The
`the subject’s
`typing displayed
`beneath the input
`delete key was disabled so subjects could not correct errors.
`A beep was heard for every error made.
`Subjects were
`instructed
`to type as quickly
`and accurately
`as possible,
`while remaining
`in sync with the input
`text.
`They could
`rest as desired between blocks.
`
`for all
`The text
`Japanese-American
`lower
`case letters,
`period),
`
`about
`typing was taken from a novel
`relations.
`It contained only upper and
`and simple
`punctuation
`(comma
`and
`
`Design
`the learning
`of
`is an investigation
`This
`experiment
`Each 50 minute
`potential
`of
`the Half-QWERTY
`keyboard.
`session consisted
`of a series a text blbcks
`typed by the
`subject.
`The block
`length was set
`to 60 characters by 4
`lines in the first session (using Courier
`14 point
`type), and
`was increased to 6 lines when subjects managed to type 30
`one-handed
`blocks in one session. Subjects completed
`as
`many blocks as were possible in a session,
`ranging from 7
`to 35 blocks, depending on speed and the amount of rest.
`
`rate.
`The dependent measures were typing speed and error
`Typing speeds are given in words per minute (wPm), with a
`word defined as 5 characters (4 letters plus a space). Error
`
`1Kriya Systems, Inc. Published by Simon & Schuster
`Software, Gulf+Western Building, One Gutf+Western Plaza,
`New York, NY 10023, USA.
`
`rates are given as a percentage of total keystrokes (the lower
`the better). Subjects had to type the correct character
`in the
`correct position.
`Thus,
`they had to type in sync with the
`If
`text on the screen.
`they
`fell
`out of sync,
`it was
`considered an error
`(as consistent with Typing Tutor
`IV).
`
`level data were also collected which
`keystroke
`Complete
`for a detailed
`examination
`of
`interkey
`timings
`allowed
`across states (space-up, space-down) and fingers, and of error
`patterns across letters and state sequences. Due to space
`limitations,
`these analyses are not provided
`in the present
`paper.
`
`RESULTS
`Subjects were able to adapt
`typing very
`to Half-QWERTY
`quickly.
`As shown in Figure
`5, session 1 resulted in an
`average speed of 13.2 wpm, with over 84% accuracy. This
`performance
`is impressive, especially considering
`how little
`training was given. For instance, subjects were not required
`to memorize
`the layout
`before
`starting
`the one-handed
`typing
`task, and therefore
`had to rely entirely
`on skill
`transfer
`from two-handed typing.
`
`the ten
`over
`significantly
`speed improved
`One-handed
`sessions (F9,sI = 80.7, p < .0001)
`to reach a tenth session
`average of 34.7 wpm.
`Improvement
`in one-handed
`error
`(F9,81 = 14.6, p <
`rate was also statistically
`significant
`.0001) dropping
`to an average of 7.44% errors in the tenth
`session.
`This is approximately
`double the rate of errors
`made in two-handed typing.
`
`92
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`lNlfRcH1’93
`
`24-29 April1993
`
`Subject
`
`*1
`+2
`43
`+4
`+5
`-o-6
`+7
`+8
`+9
`+
`
`10
`
`I
`01234567
`
`I
`
`I
`
`1
`
`I
`
`I
`
`I
`
`I
`
`f
`
`1
`8910
`
`Session
`
`Figure
`
`7. One-handed typing error
`
`rates by subject and session.
`
`varied a
`As Figures 6 and 7 show, one-handed performances
`great deal among subjects. For example, subject 6 averaged
`19.5 wpm in session
`1.
`Subject
`7 did
`not
`reach
`a
`comparable
`speed until
`session 6. Many
`factors
`likely
`contribute
`to this disparity
`among subject performances:
`two-handed speed and accumcy,
`regularity of practice, etc.
`
`the subjects had peaked by session 10,
`Note that none of
`even though three of
`them were typing in the low 40 wpm
`range. Subjects 5, 8, and 9 have agreed to undergo further
`long-term testing to determine what possible peak speeds
`can be achieved. These tests, which are on-going,
`indicate a
`potential
`for touch typists to achieve 88% (or more) of
`their
`twdwmded
`speed.
`
`speeds
`typing
`two-handed
`of note is that
`Also worthy
`the ten S&X$iOt’M (F9,8I = 4,43,
`improved
`significantly
`over
`p < .0001). This
`is likely
`due to subjects
`getting
`accustomed
`to the software and the feel of
`the keyboard.
`One-handed typing may also have had an effect. There was
`no significant
`reduction
`in two-handed
`error
`rates over
`the
`ten sessions (F9,81 = 1,12, p > .05).
`
`were typing in this range in less than two hours of practice,
`and exceeded 50% of
`their two-handed speed after about 8-9
`hours of use. This is comparable
`to Wiklund
`et al’s
`[18]
`measure of average handwriting
`speed (33 wpm).
`By the
`tenth session, subjects were typing between 41% and 73%
`of
`their
`two-handed
`speed. These speeds ranged from 23.8
`to 42.8 wpm.
`This is strong evidence that skill
`transfers
`hand-to-hand by mirror
`image and not spatial congruence.
`
`to the learning
`these results
`to compme
`is instructive
`It
`Gopher artd Raij
`[6]
`tested
`curves of chord keyboards.
`subjects’
`rate of skill acquisition
`on both one-handed
`and
`two-handed chord keyboards, as well as standard QWERTY.
`at
`After
`10 hours,
`the one-handed
`group was typing
`approximately
`21 wpm and the two-handed
`group at 22
`wpm.
`l%is compares to the Half-QWERTY
`subjects’
`tenth
`session average of 34.7 wpm,
`Gopher
`et al.’s
`[6] one-
`handed and two-handed
`subjects did not reach comparable
`rates until
`the sessions 29 and 26, respectively.
`Therefore,
`horn an economic
`standpoint,
`it is more cost effective for a
`QWERTY
`typist
`to adopt
`the Half-QWERTY
`technique
`than to learn to type on a one-handed chord keyboard.
`
`DISCUSSION
`On average, subjects were able to exceed hunt-and-peck
`typing speeds after about 3-4 hours. Wiklund
`et al.
`[181
`determined the average speed for one-handed hunt-and-peck
`typing on a standard keyboard to be approximately
`23 wpm.
`Performances on the different compact keyboards tested were
`They
`ranged
`from 15-21 wpm,
`considerably
`worse.
`depending
`on key type, size, and spacing, Our subjects
`
`[6] also found that until about session 25,
`Gopher et al.
`two-handed
`performance was only slightly better
`than one-
`handed performance
`on their chord keyboard.
`This begs an
`intenxting
`question: What percentage of
`two-handed
`speed
`can be achieved with one hand? This, of course,
`is not yet
`known, but we feel
`it maybe
`as high as 88%. The answer
`is liely
`different
`for chord and Half-QWERTY
`keyboards.
`More study is required.
`
`93
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips
`
`

`
`24-29 April1993
`
`lNTfRtH1’9
`
`(pp. 95-120).
`
`a
`
`aspects of skilled typewriting
`Cognitive
`New York Springer-Verlag.
`The
`(1985).
`Gopher, D., Karis, D., & Koenig, W.
`in long-term
`representation
`of movement
`schemas
`Lessons
`from
`the
`acquisition
`of
`memory:
`transcription
`skill. Ada Psychological,
`60, 105-134.
`Gopher, D., & Raij, D.
`(1988).
`Typing with a two-
`handed chord keyboard Will
`the QWERTY
`become
`obsolete?
`IEEE Transactions
`on Systems, Man, and
`Cybernetics,
`18,601-609.
`and
`in novice
`Error patterns
`Grudin,
`J. T.
`(1983).
`(Ed.),
`typing.
`In W. E. Cooper
`skilled
`transcription
`(pp. 121-143).
`Cognitive
`aspects of skilled typewriting
`New York Springer-Verlag.
`on
`perspective
`Lintem, G.
`(1991).
`An informational
`skill
`transfer
`in human-machine
`systems.
`Human
`Factors, 33,251-266.
`—
`8). OWERTYUIOP
`Januarv
`Litterick.
`I.
`(1981.
`dinosaur
`‘in a computer
`age.’ N~w S;ientist,
`pp. 66-68.
`Munhall, K. G., & Ostry, D. J. (1983). Mirror-image
`In W. E. Cooper
`(Ed.),
`movements
`in typing.
`Cognitive
`aspects of skilled typewriting @p. 247-257).
`New York Springer-Verlag.
`Noyes,
`J.
`(1983).
`Chord
`Ergonomics,
`14,55-59.
`keyboard
`Noyes, J. (1983).
`The QWERTY
`[nternalional
`Journal
`of Man-Machine
`265-281.
`The assessment and analysis of
`Olfield, R. C. (1971).
`inventory.
`Edinburgh
`The
`handedness:
`Neuropsychologica,
`9,97-113.
`times in
`interkey
`of
`Ostry, D. J. (1983). Determinants
`typing.
`In W. E. Cooper
`(’Ed.), Cognitive
`aspects of
`skilled
`typewriting
`(pp.
`225-246).
`New York:
`Springer-Verlag.
`In M.
`Keys and keybomds.
`(1988).
`Potosnak, K. M.
`Helander
`(Ed.), Handbook
`of
`human-computer
`interaction
`op. 475-494).
`Amsterdam: Elsevier.
`(1968).
`Provins,
`K.
`A.,
`& Glencross,
`D.
`J.
`Handwriting,
`typewriting
`and handedness. Quarterly
`Journal of Experimental
`Psychology,
`20,282-320.
`(1978,
`Rochester, N., Bequaert,
`F. C., Sharp, E. M.
`December).
`The chord keyboard. Computer, 57-63.
`Wiklund,
`M. E,, Dumas,
`J. S., & Hoffman,
`L. R.
`(1987). Optimizing
`a portable terminal
`keyboard
`for
`combined one-handed and two-handed use. Proceedings
`of
`the Human Factors Society — 31st Annual Meeting
`— 1987 (pp. 585-589).
`Santa Monica, CA: Human
`Factors Society.
`
`keyboards.
`
`Applied
`
`A review.
`Studies, 18,
`
`CONCLUSIONS
`We have shown that
`typists to
`is possible for QWERTY
`it
`achieve
`high one-handed
`typing
`rates (40+ wpm)
`in a
`relatively
`short period
`of
`time (< 10 hr) using the Half-
`QWERTY
`technique.
`These speeds are 2-3 times the rates
`achievable
`using
`compact
`keyboards,
`and
`exceed
`handwriting
`speeds. These high learning rates are due to the
`transfer of
`two-handed
`skill
`via Half-QWERTY’s
`mirror
`image hand-to-hand mapping scheme.
`
`for human-computer
`These results lead to new possibilities
`interfaces.
`By exploiting
`standard two-handed
`typing skill
`and the Half-QWERTY
`concept described above, we have
`demonstrated the potential
`to build a keyboard with full-size
`keys, but no larger
`than a paperback
`book.
`Furthermore,
`since the design can be implemented
`in software, wide and
`convenient
`access to one-handed typing is also possible on
`a standard
`keyboard.
`These findings
`are important
`for
`designers of compact
`computing
`systems and systems for
`disabled users.
`
`ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
`of
`the contributions
`We would
`like
`to acknowledge
`Jonathan Cheng who wrote
`the software
`and the input
`Research Group at the University
`of Toronto who provided
`the forum for
`the design and execution
`of
`this project.
`In
`addition,
`we gratefully
`acknowledge
`the support
`of
`the
`Natural
`Sciences and Engineering
`Research Council
`of
`Canada, Digital Equipment Corporation,
`Xerox Palo Alto
`Research Centre
`(PARC),
`Apple
`Computer’s
`Human
`Interface Group,
`IBM Canada’s Toronto Laboratory Centre
`for Advanced
`Studies,
`and the Amott Design Group
`of
`Toronto.
`
`2,
`
`REFERENCES
`1. Buxton, W. (1990). Chord keyboads.
`The pragmatic
`of haptic input, Tutorial
`26 Notes of CHI’90,
`6.1-6.9.
`Engelbart, D., & English, W. K.
`(1968).
`A research
`center for augmenting
`human intellect.
`Proceedings
`of
`the Fall
`Joint Computer
`Conference
`(pp. 395-410).
`Washington, DC: Thompson Book Co.
`S., Norman,
`3. Gentner, D. R., Grudin,
`J. T., Larochelle,
`A glossary
`of
`D. A., & Rumelhart,
`D. E.
`(1983).
`terms including
`classification
`of
`typing errors.
`In W.
`E. Cooper
`(Ed.),
`Cognitive
`aspects
`of
`skilled
`typewriting
`(pp. 39-43). New York: Springer-Verlag.
`in
`4. Gentner,
`D. R.
`(1983).
`Keystroke
`timing
`In W. E. Cooper
`(Ed.),
`transcription
`typing.
`
`5.
`
`6.
`
`7.
`
`8.
`
`9.
`
`10.
`
`11.
`
`12.
`
`13.
`
`14.
`
`150
`
`16,
`
`174
`
`18,
`
`94
`
`GOOGLE EX. 1013
`Google v. Philips

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket