throbber
Eur J Appl Physiol (2003) 89: 411–426
`DOI 10.1007/s00421-003-0818-2
`
`O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E
`
`H. A. M. Daanen
`Finger cold-induced vasodilation: a review
`
`Accepted: 10 February 2003 / Published online: 24 April 2003
`Ó Springer-Verlag 2003
`
`Abstract Cold-induced vasodilation (CIVD) in the finger
`tips generally occurs 5–10 min after the start of local cold
`exposure of the extremities. This phenomenon is believed
`to reduce the risk of local cold injuries. However, CIVD
`is almost absent during hypothermia, when survival of the
`organism takes precedence over the survival of peripheral
`tissue. Subjects that are often exposed to local cold (e.g.
`fish filleters) develop an enhanced CIVD response. Also,
`differences between ethnic groups are obvious, with black
`people having the weakest CIVD response. Many other
`factors affect CIVD, such as diet, alcohol consumption,
`altitude, age and stress. CIVD is probably caused by a
`sudden decrease in the release of neurotransmitters from
`the sympathetic nerves to the muscular coat of the arte-
`rio-venous anastomoses (AVAs) due to local cold. AVAs
`are specific thermoregulatory organs that regulate blood
`flow in the cold and heat. Their relatively large diameter
`enables large amounts of blood to pass and convey heat
`to the surrounding tissue. Unfortunately, information on
`the quantity of AVAs is lacking, which makes it difficult
`to estimate the full impact on peripheral blood flow. This
`review illustrates the thermospecificity of the AVAs and
`the close link to CIVD. CIVD is influenced by many
`parameters, but controlled experiments yield information
`on how CIVD protects the extremities against cold
`injuries.
`
`Keywords Arterio-venous anastomoses Æ Cold induced
`vasodilation Æ Finger blood flow
`
`Introduction
`
`Millions of people are daily exposed to cold and
`face
`the
`challenge
`to maintain their body core
`
`H. A. M. Daanen
`TNO Human Factors, PO Box 23, 3769 ZG Soesterberg,
`The Netherlands
`E-mail: Daanen@tm.tno.nl
`Fax: +31-346-353977
`
`temperature at about 37°C. Peripheral vasoconstric-
`tion is a powerful mechanism to reduce the heat
`loss, but results in strong cooling of the extremities.
`However, the extremities possess the ability to prevent
`the occurrence of local cold extremes. About 5–10 min
`after the initiation of cold exposure of
`the hand,
`blood vessels in the finger tips suddenly vasodilate,
`which increases the peripheral blood flow and subse-
`quently the temperature of the finger tips. This cold-
`induced vasodilation (CIVD) is followed by a new
`phase of vasoconstriction. This process repeats itself
`and is called ‘the hunting reaction’
`(Lewis 1930).
`Arterio-venous anastomoses (AVAs) are thought to
`play a major role in the mechanism of CIVD, but the
`exact mechanism is still subject to debate. The CIVD
`reaction and the related blood vessels are important
`issues for thermal physiologists, since CIVD is thought
`to reduce the risk of local cold injuries (Iida 1949).
`Wilson and Goldman (1970) found in their experi-
`ments that freezing did not take place when CIVD
`occurred. It is likely that CIVD also improves the
`manual dexterity and tactile sensitivity during work in
`the cold. Since the blood flow increases substantially
`in the fingers during CIVD, this increases the blood
`circulation in the large vessels of the forearm, with the
`consequence of
`increasing the temperature of
`the
`forearm muscles (Ducharme et al. 1991), and likely
`contributes
`to improved manual performance by
`improving muscle function. The increased skin tem-
`perature due to CIVD will increase the firing rate of
`the pressure transducers in the skin and thus increase
`tactile sensitivity.
`CIVD can occur at several locations in the human
`body. The focus in this review is placed on finger CIVD,
`since the finger is a common site for local cold injuries
`and most data are available for this body part.
`It is the purpose of this article to review the current
`knowledge on finger CIVD, to describe the effects of
`several parameters such as core temperature, diet and
`acclimatization on finger CIVD and to discuss the pos-
`sible mechanisms involved.
`
`APL1109
`Apple v. Valencell
`IPR2017-00317
`
`

`

`412
`
`Definitions and terminology
`
`CIVD and hunting reaction
`
`Cold-induced vasodilation can be defined as vasodila-
`tion of cold-exposed blood vessels,
`in particular the
`small arteries. The term hunting reaction or hunting
`response (Lewis 1930) is used to describe the alternating
`periods of vasodilation and vasoconstriction during cold
`exposure. Some authors use the term Lewis reaction
`instead of hunting reaction (Kramer and Schulze 1948;
`Werner 1977).
`Purkayastha et al. (1992) argue that the hunting
`reaction is only one out of four possible reactions of
`blood vessels to extreme local cold. The other responses
`observed in the fingers after immersion in cold water are:
`(1) a continuous state of vasoconstriction, (2) slow
`steady and continuous rewarming and (3) a proportional
`control form in which the blood vessel diameter remains
`constant after an initial phase of vasoconstriction.
`The majority of the vascular responses to immersion of
`the finger in cold water can be classified as the hunting
`reaction. Daanen (2001) observed that the hunting reac-
`tion was present in 210 out of 226 investigated male sub-
`jects (93%) who immersed their finger in ice water. The
`reactions of the remaining subjects were difficult to clas-
`sify.
`
`Arterio-venous anastomoses (AVAs)
`
`AVAs are thought to play a major role in CIVD. These
`blood vessels have a thick muscular wall and a lumen,
`measuring on average 10–30 lm (Gray 2000), 35 lm
`(Roddie 1983) or even 50 lm (Sherman 1963). Under the
`influence of the sympathetic nervous system, with its rich
`supply of non-myelinated fibres on the wall of the vessel,
`they are capable of complete closure. When the AVAs are
`open, large amounts of blood can pass. Anastomoses are
`not fixed structures, but may come and go on demand:
`they can develop when necessary and disappear when they
`are no longer needed. Hale and Burch (1960) observed
`that AVAs develop if blood requirement increases at the
`finger tip. Clark and Clark (1934) estimated that the for-
`mation of new AVAs requires 2–3 days.
`In their studies, Hale and Burch (1960) and Clara
`(1939) mentioned the following sites of AVAs: the skin of
`the inside of the hand and foot, the nail bed, the elbow,
`lips, cheeks, ears and the nose. There is some discussion
`about the presence of AVAs in the skin of the head.
`Grant and Bland (1931) found 501 AVAs per cm2
`surface area in the nail bed, 236 in the finger tip, 150 on
`the palmar side of the distal phalanx, 20 on the palmar
`side of the medial phalanx and 93 on the palmar side of
`the proximal phalanx. They found no AVAs on the
`dorsal side of the hand. However, the numbers were
`derived from only one index finger from only one sub-
`ject, although they claim that similar results were found
`
`in three other subjects. Masson (1937) only counted
`three to four AVA’s per cm2 at the top of the finger and
`about ten in the nail bed. Clara (1939) argued that Grant
`and Bland (1931) counted the same AVA several times.
`AVAs are tortuous and they did not account for that in
`their counting technique.
`The limited information on the number of AVAs in
`the fingers and the disagreement in the existing studies
`necessitates new research to address this topic. More-
`over, there is a strong need for more accurate data on
`the amount of AVAs in the human body, in particular to
`improve current computer models on blood flow and
`heat transfer.
`
`Methodology
`
`Several methods are available to quantify the amount of
`vasodilation in the finger skin. Direct measurement of the
`diameter of blood vessels is extremely difficult, if not
`impossible, and therefore indirect measures are used.
`When the blood vessel diameter increases, the blood flow
`increases (if the viscosity of the blood remains the same)
`and this can be measured by laser Doppler flowmetry and
`strain gauge plethysmography. Some authors determine
`the blood flow by the wash out of a marker added to the
`blood (e.g. Coffman 1972). The increased blood flow
`raises the tissue temperature and thus the temperature at
`the finger tip. The finger skin temperature is the most
`frequently used method to determine CIVD. The in-
`creased finger skin temperature leads to a higher heat
`transfer to the environment, which can be assessed by heat
`flux sensors and calorimetry. The most common methods
`to quantify CIVD are briefly discussed below.
`
`Strain gauge plethysmography
`
`The principle of this technique is that a cuff is placed and
`inflated proximal to the measuring site in such a way
`that blood can enter, but not
`leave the measured
`extremity. During the obstruction a linear increase is
`seen in circumference due to the accumulating blood.
`The increase in circumference is an estimator of blood
`flow (Elkington 1968). Since fingers are almost free of
`skeletal muscle tissue, their volume changes mainly
`represent alterations in the blood volume in the cuta-
`neous blood vessels (Okuda 1942).
`
`Laser Doppler flowmetry
`
`Laser Doppler flowmetry is a method that yields infor-
`mation on local skin blood flow. The emitted laser light
`from a small probe on the skin is backscattered from
`moving red blood cells or static skin structures. Light
`scattered from moving objects is shifted in frequency
`(Doppler shift) in proportion to the velocity of the
`moving target. A photodetector, located close to the
`laser beam end, measures the backscattered light.
`
`

`

`The penetration depth of the laser in the skin is
`determined by the wavelength of the laser Doppler sys-
`tem, the fibre separation (Hirata et al. 1988) and local
`properties of the skin (Tenland 1982). Nagasaka et al.
`(1988) argues that only a minimal part of the AVAs can
`be ‘‘seen’’ by laser Doppler flowmetry using He-Ne la-
`sers. Wollersheim (1988), however, shows that at least
`part of the shunt flow is included in the laser Doppler
`results. This issue is not resolved, but it can be expected
`that new laser Doppler systems will have the ability to
`set the penetration depth.
`Skin nutritional blood flow through the capillaries is
`generally related to total blood flow of an extremity. For
`instance, Johnson et al. (1984) found a good relationship
`between laser Doppler flow and forearm blood flow
`(r=0.94–0.98). However, there are also situations, such as
`reflex vasodilation, in which skin perfusion is regulated
`independently from total blood flow (Hirata et al. 1988).
`
`Finger skin temperature
`
`Finger skin temperature is the most commonly used
`measure for CIVD. A small thermocouple is generally
`attached to the palmar side of the distal phalanx with
`tape. The measured temperature is a mix of the finger
`skin temperature and the temperature of the surround-
`ing cooling medium. Careful attachment of a small
`thermocouple to the skin minimizes the influence of the
`cooling medium temperature.
`The nail bed is also often used as a measuring site,
`since it is known that AVAs are abundant there. How-
`ever, Yoshimura (1966) showed that the temperature
`reaction measured on the pad of the finger is more
`sensitive and reproducible than that on the nail bed.
`
`413
`
`The measured finger skin temperature is a slow
`indicator of what occurs in the tissue underneath.
`Daanen (1997) observed that CIVD onset measured
`using the finger skin temperature occurred 90 (48) s later
`than measured using laser Doppler flowmetry.
`The changes in finger skin temperature profile during
`cold exposure are quantified using the terminology
`shown in Fig. 1.
`– The minimum temperature (Tmin) is the lowest finger
`skin temperature just before CIVD starts.
`– The maximum temperature (Tmax) is the highest
`finger skin temperature during CIVD.
`– The onset time (Dtonset) is the time from immersion
`to Tmin.
`– The amplitude is the difference between Tmin and
`Tmax.
`– The peak time (Dtpeak) is the time interval between
`Tmin and Tmax.
`– The mean finger skin temperature (Tmean) denotes the
`finger skin temperature averaged over the immer-
`sion period. As a rule, the onset time is not included,
`since during this period the heat in the hand is re-
`moved. In practice, the first 5 min data are removed.
`– The frequency of the hunting reaction is expressed as
`the number of waves (vasodilation/vasoconstriction
`period) within a certain time frame.
`
`Yoshimura and Iida (1952) quantified the magnitude
`of the CIVD reaction using the Resistance Index for
`Frostbite (RIF) in which Dtonset, Tmin and Tmean were
`included. Short onset times and high minimal or mean
`finger skin temperatures were rated by 3 points and long
`onset times and low temperatures by 1 point, leading to
`summated RIFs from 3 to 9. This RIF index is used by
`many other authors, in particular from Japan.
`
`Fig. 1 Parameters derived from
`a temperature profile of a finger
`tip immersed in cold water. The
`onset time (Dtonset) is the time
`from immersion to the
`minimum temperature (Tmin).
`The amplitude is the difference
`between Tmin and Tmax. The
`peak time (Dtpeak) is the time
`interval between Tmin and Tmax.
`The mean finger skin
`temperature (Tmean) denotes the
`finger skin temperature
`averaged over the immersion
`period, excluding onset time
`
`

`

`414
`
`General reactions observed during CIVD
`
`Reflex vasoconstriction and reflex vasodilation
`
`When one body part is cooled, vasoconstriction also
`occurs in other parts of the body. This phenomenon is
`known as reflex vasoconstriction. Similarly, if heat is
`applied to another part of the body, such as a leg, the
`vessels open up and the hand gets warm (Gibbon and
`Landis 1932). This phenomenon is called reflex vasodi-
`lation. Sensors in the skin react to the external stimulus
`and transfer information to the vasomotor centre. This
`centre integrates the information and sends an adequate
`response to the effector organs. Pickering (1932) showed
`that blood temperature also plays an important role in
`this mechanism. He found no reflex vasodilation when
`the venous return of a heated hand was blocked.
`Reflex vasodilation and vasoconstriction are also
`noted during the hunting reaction. Immersion of the feet
`in cold water during the hunting reaction in fingers re-
`duced the magnitude of the hunting reaction (Keatinge
`1957). Lewis (1930) observed that cooling the forearm
`suppressed CIVD in the fingers. Page and Brown (1953)
`and Livingstone et al. (1978) observed that Eskimos had
`less reflex vasoconstriction in the fingers upon cold water
`immersion of a foot than control subjects. Thus, Eski-
`mos are able to maintain good dexterity when the feet
`are cold. Werner (1983) showed that reflex vasodilation
`or vasoconstriction not only depends on the skin and
`core temperatures but also on the rate of change of these
`temperatures.
`
`Pain
`
`Immersion in cold water is often a painful experience.
`LeBlanc (1975) and Heus and Daanen (1993) noted that
`the most painful period occurred during vasoconstric-
`tion, and that the vasodilation phase was often felt as a
`relief. The pain during strong vasoconstriction may be
`seen as a warning signal for exceptional cooling. Kreh
`et al. (1984) found a close relationship between pain
`intensity and degree of vasoconstriction. If the cooling
`continues, the tissue temperature may decrease below
`the threshold for nerve conduction (7–8°C, Vanggaard
`1975). If that threshold is reached no information from
`the periphery can reach the central nervous system and
`the extremity feels numb. Sawada et al. (2000) observed
`that pain diminished after repeated cold water immer-
`sions.
`
`Experimental factors affecting CIVD
`
`Ambient temperature and body temperature
`
`The regulation of blood flow to the extremities is, at
`low ambient temperatures, primarily determined by the
`
`thermal state of the body as a whole. Even at air tem-
`peratures below )30°C, the skin temperature of bare
`hands can be sustained above 21°C (Rapaport et al. 1949).
`Therefore, it is rather likely that the body core tempera-
`ture influences the hunting reaction. In Table 1 results of
`relevant articles in the literature are summarized.
`In most investigations the effects of body temperature
`were investigated by putting subjects in a relatively cold or
`warm room. Unfortunately, the resulting core and mean
`body skin temperatures were often not recorded. The
`general
`image emerging from Table 1 is that a high
`ambient or core temperature leads to higher mean finger
`skin temperatures during the hunting reaction. Also, the
`onset time of CIVD was observed to be shorter. Daanen et
`al. (1997) and Daanen and Ducharme (1999) found that
`onset time was mainly related to the mean skin tempera-
`ture of the body and that mean finger skin temperatures
`were mainly related to body core temperatures.
`Ambient temperature may change body core tem-
`perature, but the core temperature is also modified by
`exposure to ambient light and changes in melatonin
`variations during the day (Burgess et al. 2001), and
`during the year (seasonal) (Yoneyama et al. 1999). The
`effects of these changes in core temperature on CIVD, as
`reported in the literature, are:
`
`–
`
`– The hunting reaction is more pronounced in the
`afternoon, than in the morning or the night (Kramer
`and Schulze 1948).
`Schulze (Kramer and Schulze 1948) measured his
`CIVD each month at room temperature with his
`hand in a cold air box and found an average max-
`imal finger skin temperature of 28°C in the summer
`and 16°C in the winter during immersion, indicating
`a vasoconstrictive state during winter.
`– Tanaka (1971b) measured CIVD during middle
`finger immersion in 0°C water during summer and
`winter under identical ambient conditions and also
`observed that the CIVD reaction was more pro-
`nounced in summer.
`
`In summary, it appears that in the afternoon and in
`the summer, when the core temperature is relatively
`elevated, the hunting reaction is more pronounced.
`
`Cooling medium
`
`To evoke CIVD, two media are commonly used: water
`and air. Immersion in cold water is used most often. The
`thermal conductivity of water is about 25 times higher
`than that of air, so cooling is rather quick.
`Kramer and Schulze (1948) cooled fingers in a cold
`air box, and compared the results with those of other
`studies in which the fingers where cooled in water. The
`frequency of the hunting response in 0°C water showed
`most similarity with that in )18°C air. Kramer and
`Schulze (1948) observed that
`the frequency of
`the
`hunting reaction decreased when the air temperature in
`
`

`

`415
`
`lower(CIVD)than15°C
`watertemp.ishigheror
`ambienttemp.andwhen
`Bloodflowhigherathigh
`
`alsolower
`lowerinHamplitude
`Meanfingerskintemp.
`
`increasedwhenwarm
`Meanfingerskintemp.
`
`bath:13%ofmax.
`5%ofmax.Cold
`65%ofmax.Cold:
`
`Fingerheatloss.Hot:
`roomtemp.increases
`
`Moreheatlosswhen
`
`whenindirectlywarmed
`
`Morebloodflow
`
`warmroom
`
`Moreheatlossin
`
`coldroom
`andheattransferin
`Reducedbloodflow
`
`magnitude
`times,smallCIVD
`
`Coldbody:longonset
`
`huntingmagnitude
`frequencyandsmall
`
`Coldbody:lowhunting
`
`amplitude
`infrequencyand
`temp.),nodifferences
`7.8°Cat28°Croom
`chilled(3.8°Cversus
`Tminlowerwhen
`onambienttemp.
`waterorair)but
`onlocaltemp.(cold
`
`Bloodflownotdependent
`
`coolsubjects
`Dtonsetincreasedin
`Results
`
`plethysmography
`venousocclusion
`
`Bloodflowby
`
`waterof2to35°C
`Handsfor3hin
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`4°Cwater
`Rightmiddlefingerin
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`of)18to10°C
`Handsincoldair
`
`Calorimetry
`
`Indexfingerinice
`
`water
`
`Calorimetry
`
`plethysmography
`Venousocclusion
`
`Toesinwater0–6°C
`Handsinicewater
`
`Calorimetry
`
`calorimetry
`
`Plethysmography,
`
`(laserDoppler)
`skinperfusion
`
`Fingerskintemp,
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`waterof5°C
`Handfor30minin
`
`Fingerinicewater
`
`waterof5°C
`Handfor40minin
`
`waterof8°C
`Handfor30minin
`
`andplethysmography
`Fingernailbedtemp.
`
`15°Cair
`Handin0,5,10and
`
`andplethysmography
`
`Fingerskintemp.
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`Variables
`
`icewateror0°Cair
`Terminalphalanxin
`20minin0°C
`Indexfingerfor
`
`Measurement
`
`Induction
`
`Huntingreaction
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`Tre:36.29°C
`37.02°C;H:Tes:36.18°C;
`N:Tes:36.85°C;Tre:
`
`Tes,Tre
`
`24and32°Cfor3h
`Roomtemp.16,
`20°Cwater(H)
`air(N),60minin
`30minin27°C
`variations
`dailyandseasonal
`
`Spealman(1945)
`
`Leeetal.(1996)
`
`(1948)
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`Roomtemp,hotdrinks,
`
`KramerandSchulze
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`clothingandexercise
`and17–18°Cwith
`temp.5–6°C(1h)
`6°Cwaterbath;Room
`20.5°Cand22.5°C
`Roomtemp.14.5°C,
`andcooling
`Indirectwarming
`22°Cclothed
`unclothedand
`Roomtemp.18°C
`and9–17°C
`Roomtemp.‘‘neutral’’
`waterperfusedsuit
`immersion;hot
`
`Keatinge(1957)
`
`Greenfieldetal.(1951)
`
`Folkowetal.(1963)
`
`Elsneretal.(1960)
`
`(1960)
`
`EdwardsandBurton
`
`(2000)
`
`Tes,TreandTskTes36.1and38.0°C
`
`Wholebodycoldwater
`
`DaanenandDucharme
`
`Tre,TskandTearTear36.5and37.1°C
`
`43°Cbeverages
`Drinking0°Cand
`
`Daanenetal.(1997)
`
`roomtemp.:25°C
`33.1°C.Tskatother
`roomtemp.Tskat28°C:
`Trenotdifferentbetween
`
`Tre,Tsk
`
`15and12°Cfor2–3h
`Roomtemp.28,25,
`
`Blaisdell(1951)
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`Results
`
`Variables
`
`BaderandMead(1949)Roomtemp.13and
`
`AdamsandSmith(1962)Roomtemp.7and
`
`32°C
`22°Cfor1h
`
`minimumtemperatureoffingerskinduringimmersion(°C),DtonsettimefromimmersiontoTmininminutes]
`Table1Influenceofbodytemperaturesonthehuntingreaction.[TreRectaltemperature(°C),Tskmeanbodyskintemperature(°C),Tmeanmeanfingerskintemperature(°C),Tmin
`
`Measurement
`
`Induction
`
`Bodytemperatures
`
`Author(s)
`
`

`

`the cooling box around the hand increased: four to six
`hunting periods were observed in 2 h at an air temper-
`ature of )18°C and only one at 10°C air temperature.
`There is no agreement in the literature concerning the
`water temperatures at which CIVD occurs. The lowest
`temperature in the studies equals 0°C, the temperature
`of stirred water with ice. Lewis (1930) and Yoshimura
`(1960) performed their experiments at this temperature,
`since they wanted a maximal response. Lewis (1930) did
`not see a hunting reaction in water temperatures above
`18°C. Hirai et al. (1970) saw no response at 15°C and
`advised an optimal temperature for investigation of 5°C,
`since the differences between two investigated popula-
`tions were optimal then.
`Havenith et al. (1992) and Chen et al. (1994) observed
`CIVD when subjects were touching cold materials, but
`decided not to use and process the data for their cooling
`models. Chen et al. (1994) observed CIVD in 14% of the
`recordings. This low percentage is probably due to two
`factors. First, a part of the thermocouple was in direct
`contact with the aluminium bar, thus underestimating
`the finger tip temperature. Second, the cold stress was
`not severe enough: the aluminium surface was –7, 0 or
`7°C in a climatic chamber of 10 or 30°C.
`
`Surface area cooled
`
`Sendowski et al. (1997) investigated the differences in
`finger CIVD response between immersing a finger, hand
`or forearm in 5°C water. The finger CIVD was more
`pronounced (faster onset and higher amplitude) after
`finger immersion than after hand or forearm immersion.
`Two main explanations were given. The first explanation
`was that the arterial blood was cooler in the finger after
`hand/forearm immersion due to pre-cooling in the hand
`or forearm. This explanation was recently confirmed by
`Ducharme et al. (2001), who observed reduced CIVD
`when the forearm tissue was cooled. The second expla-
`nation was that sympathetic activity was higher during
`hand/forearm immersion, which was illustrated by higher
`scores for pain sensation. Moreover, cardiovascular
`changes were found during hand/forearm immersion and
`not during finger immersion. To investigate the sympa-
`thetic influence in more detail, Sendowski et al. (2000)
`co-immersed the left hand with the right middle finger.
`Increased plasma norepinephrine during co-immersion
`showed that sympathetic activity was elevated. The CIVD
`amplitude reduced during co-immersion, and this is likely
`to be attributed to increased sympathetic activity. On the
`other hand, blood cooling due to the left hand immersion
`cannot be completely ruled out.
`
`Altitude
`
`A significant reduction in the hunting reaction is found
`during exposure to high altitude, where cold co-exists
`with systemic hypoxia (Mathew et al. 1977). Takeoka
`
`roomtemp.
`decreaseswith
`roomtemp.Dtonset
`increaseswith
`roomtemp.Tmean
`Tminincreaseswith
`45°Cambienttemp.
`offluctuationsat
`temp.increaseNo.
`ambientandhand
`Tmeanhigherwhen
`TmeanandshorterDtonset
`temp:higherTminand
`Withincreasingroom
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`Fingerskintemp
`
`30minin0°C
`Middlefingerfor
`
`0and5°C
`0.5m°s-1at)5,
`Handsinairat
`
`30minin0°C
`Middlefingerfor
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`–
`
`Tre
`
`–
`
`4–36°C
`Roomtemp.
`
`(1950)
`
`YoshimuraandIida
`
`30and45°C
`(for0.5h),
`Roomtemp.15
`
`and35°C
`Roomtemp.)25,30
`
`Werner(1977)
`
`Tanaka(1971b)
`
`Results
`
`Variables
`
`Results
`
`Variables
`
`Measurement
`
`Induction
`
`Measurement
`
`Induction
`
`416
`
`Huntingreaction
`
`Bodytemperatures
`
`Author(s)
`
`Table1(Contd.)
`
`

`

`et al. (1993) showed that Tmean during the hunting
`reaction was lower in seven Japanese men at an altitude
`of 4860 m (ambient temperature 9°C) as compared to
`2260 m (ambient temperature 12°C). Recently, Daanen
`and Van Ruiten (2000) observed that CIVD was reduced
`in magnitude at an altitude of 5000 m, even when the
`body core was warmer than at sea level. Therefore, it
`seems that systemic hypoxia reduces the magnitude of
`the hunting reaction. During prolonged stay at altitude,
`the CIVD reaction gradually improves (Daanen and
`Van Ruiten 2000).
`
`Individual factors affecting CIVD
`
`Age
`
`In the elderly, the CIVD reaction occurs later and is less
`pronounced (Sawada 1996; Spurr et al. 1955). Also, Tan
`and Tregenza (2002) observed that the CIVD responses
`of a group of 18-year old students was more pronounced
`than that of their parents (40–50 years old). The reduced
`CIVD reaction is attributed to the diminished sympa-
`thetic vasoconstrictor responses with the advancing age
`(Khan et al. 2002) which is in line with the observations
`that the onset time and peak time are considerably de-
`layed by age and the finger skin temperatures are only
`slightly changed. Also, an age-related decrease in core
`temperature due to a decreased basal metabolic rate and
`reduced physical activity (Van Someren et al. 2002) will
`likely affect the CIVD response.
`For younger people, the results are less consistent.
`Yoshimura and Iida (1952) observed that children had
`lower RIF values than adults, but higher values than in
`puberty. In contrast, Miller and Irving (1962) found that
`the finger temperature of Eskimo children dropped more
`than that of adults during cold exposure. However, this
`may be attributed to their drop in body temperature,
`which was not controlled for.
`
`Gender
`
`417
`
`role here. Cooke et al. (1990) found no relation between
`female hand blood flow and levels of serum oestrogen or
`progesterone.
`Miller and Irving (1962) observed no differences in
`finger temperature response to cold air between three
`Eskimo women and eight Eskimo men and between
`eight Eskimo boys and four Eskimo girls. Also,
`Yoshimura and Iida (1952), Tanaka (1971b) and
`Yoshimura et al. (1958) found no sex differences in the
`hunting reaction. Reading et al. (1997) observed higher
`finger skin temperatures for males than for females
`during a 2-h exposure to 0°C air, during which hunting
`occurred.
`In summary, females tend to have lower hand blood
`flows when exposed to cold due to increased vascular
`reactivity, but the limited amount of available studies
`showed no indication that the temperature profiles of the
`hunting reaction differed between males and females. It
`was expected that the reduced vascular reactivity to cold
`of males as compared to females would be reflected in a
`delayed CIVD reaction, similar to the observations of
`CIVD with age. This, however, was not the case and
`further research is needed to elucidate this point.
`
`Physical fitness and fatigue
`
`Moriya and Nakagawa (1990) observed in 14 females
`that the RIF was not related to the maximum oxygen
`uptake divided by body weight. The latter variable is
`considered to be a good estimator of physical fitness.
`They found a significant correlation between absolute
`maximum oxygen uptake in l/min and RIF, but this was
`only in one subject and insufficient to draw conclusions.
`More research is needed to establish the effect of phys-
`ical fitness on CIVD.
`O’Brien et al. (1999) observed that the onset time was
`reduced in subjects after 5 days of repeated strenuous
`physical exertion with insufficient recovery. This effect
`was attributed to increased circulating norepinephrine
`levels that reduce the sensitivity to sympathetic stimu-
`lation.
`
`Hand and finger blood flow in thermoneutral conditions
`is higher for men than for women as measured by laser
`Doppler flowmetry and plethysmography, due to in-
`creased vasomotor tone in women (Cooke et al. 1990).
`By contrast, after total body warming of both genders,
`the blood flow was greater in females than in males.
`When warm hands (about 32°C) are exposed to cold,
`women show an enhanced vascular reactivity as com-
`pared to men, which causes a more pronounced decrease
`in peripheral blood flow and skin temperatures in fe-
`males (Bartelink et al. 1993; Pollock et al. 1993). The
`reactivity is most pronounced in women using oral
`contraceptives, followed by premenopausal women and
`postmenopausal women (Bartelink et al. 1993). These
`three different hormonal conditions thus have a strong
`impact on vascular reactivity, but age may also play a
`
`Mental stress
`
`Meehan (1957) showed that the hunting reaction was
`abolished in a stressed subject who had just completed
`an exam, in contrast to three previous experiments in
`which the hunting reaction had been clearly present.
`This finding was confirmed by Adams and Smith (1962).
`They showed that a strong emotional stress given during
`the vasodilation phase led to an immediate strong
`vasoconstriction. This demonstrates
`the
`functional
`integrity of efferent vasomotor nerves and receptors
`during CIVD. Mental stress increases the activity of the
`vasomotor centre which increases the intensity of vaso-
`constriction in the skin (Marriott et al. 1990). When the
`sympathetic vascular tone is high, however, mental tasks
`
`

`

`418
`
`may lead to paradoxical vasodilation (Cooke et al. 1990;
`Halperin et al. 1983). Also, humans are able to volun-
`tary vasodilate the blood vessels in the finger pad, and
`thus increase their finger temperature (Carter 1978).
`
`Acclimatization to cold
`
`Acclimatization refers to physiological or behavioural
`changes occurring within an organism that reduce the
`strain or enhance the endurance of strain caused by
`stressful changes, in particular climatic factors (IUPS
`Thermal Commission 2001). People working with their
`hands in a cold environment (e.g. fish-filleters) have in-
`creased blood flow through their hands in the cold as
`compared to unacclimatized subjects (Krog et al. 1960;
`LeBlanc et al. 1960). CIVD occurs at an earlier stage of
`cold exposure in the acclimatized subjects (Nelms and
`Soper 1962). Tanaka (1971a) found that ice chamber
`workers had a higher mean finger skin temperature, a
`higher minimal finger temperature and shorter onset
`time of CIVD than cool room workers. Purkayastha
`et al. (1992, 1993) showed that tropical residents deve-
`loped a more pronounced hunting reaction 7 weeks after
`an airlift to an arctic region and that the response was
`indistinguishable from the response of arctic residents.
`Table 2 summarizes the findings on the effects of local
`cold acclimatization on CIVD.
`
`Adaptation to cold
`
`The term genotypic adaptation refers to ‘‘a genetically
`fixed condition of a species or subspecies, or its evolu-
`tion, which favours survival in a particular total envi-
`ronment’’ (IUPS Thermal Commission 2001). This type
`of adaptation for cold is generally deduced from a
`comparison between populations who inhabit different
`locations. Studies on cold adaptation are included in
`Table 2.
`The results of the studies depend on the method that
`is used to measure the changes in finger circulation.
`The results were unambiguous when finger skin
`temperatures were measured. It was generally found that
`the cold adapted/acclimatized (CA) subjects had a
`shorter onset time and a higher minimal and mean
`temperature (Table 2). Only a few publications were not
`consistent with this statement. Takeoka et al. (1993)
`found that the cold-adapted Tibetans had a lower Tmean
`than the Japanese controls. In this case however, hy-
`poxia coexists with cold, which may alter the response.
`Bridgman (1991) found no differences between divers
`and non-divers, probably due to an insufficient stimulus
`for acclimatization. The other publications in which
`finger temperatures were measured were in general
`agreement. Meehan (1955) found that the mean skin
`temperature of the fingers during ice water immersion
`was highest for Alaskan natives, followed by Caucasians
`and Negroes respectively. Elsner et al. (1960) showed
`
`an earlier onset of vasodilation in arctic Indians as
`compared to a control group. Itoh et al. (1970) showed
`that natives of the colder Hokkaido island had a more
`pronounced hunting reaction (earlier onset, higher mean
`finger skin temperature and higher minima) than sub-
`jects born on the main island. Iampietro et al. (1959)
`showed that Negroes and Caucasi

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket