throbber
THIRD EDITION
`
`EDITED BY
`
`ROBERT E. HALES, M.D., M.B.A.
`
`Professor and Chair, Department of Psychiatry,
`University of California, Davis School of Medicine;
`
`Director, Behavioral Health Center, UC Davis Health System;
`Medical Director, Sacramento County Mental Health Services,
`
`Sacramento, California
`
`STUART C. YUDOFSKY, M.D.
`
`D. C. and Irene Ellwood Professor and Chairman,
`
`Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
`
`Baylor College of Medicine;
`
`Chief, Psychiatry Service,
`
`The Methodist Hospital,
`
`Houston, Texas
`
`JOHN A. TALBOTT, M.D.
`
`Professor and Chairman,
`
`.
`
`Department of Psychiatry,
`
`University of Maryland School of Medicine,
`
`Baltimore, Maryland
`
`American _
`sychlatrlc
`
`Press, Inc.
`
`Washington, DC
`London, England
`
`10f89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`THE AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS
`
`TEXTBOOK OF PSYCHIATRY
`
`
`
`,::.\«m.-sv..~)a.=-v
`
`1 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
` m
`
`J1
`
`|
`,
`
`1
`
`‘
`
`I
`
`1
`‘-
`
`V.
`I
`
`.
`
`Note: The authors have worked to ensure that all information in this book concerning drug dosages, schedules, and routes
`of administration is accurate as of the time of publication and consistent with standards set by the U.S. Food and Drug Ad-
`ministration and the general medical community. As medical research and practice advance, however, therapeutic standards
`may change. For this reason and because human and mechanical errors sometimes occur, we recommend that readers fol-
`low the advice of a physician who is directly involved in their care or in the care of a member of their family.
`Books published by the American Psychiatric Press, Inc., represent the views and opinions of the individual authors and do
`not necessarily represent the policies and opinions of the Press or the American Psychiatric Association.
`
`Diagnostic criteria included in this textbook are reprinted, with permission, from the Diagnortic and Statirtical Manual of
`Mental Dirorderr, 4th Edition. Copyright 1994, American Psychiatric Association.
`
`Copyright © 1999 American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
`
`ALL RIGHTS RESERVED
`
`Manufactured in the United States of America on acid-free paper
`
`Third Edition
`
`02
`
`01
`
`00
`
`99
`
`4
`
`3
`
`2
`
`1
`
`American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
`1400 K Street, N.W., Washington, DC 20005
`www.appi.org
`
`I
`
`1
`
`Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
`The American Psychiatric Press textbook of psychiatry / edited by
`Robert E. Hales, Stuart C. Yudofsky, John A. Talbott. — 3rd ed.
`p.
`cm.
`
`Includes bibliographical references and index.
`ISBN 0-88048-819-0 (alk. paper)
`II. Yudofsky, Stuart C.
`1. Psychiatry.
`1. Hales, Robert E.
`III. Talbott, John A.
`IV. Title: Textbook of psychiatry.
`[DNLM:
`1. Mental Disorders.
`2. Psychiatry. WM 100 A5112 1999]
`RC454.A197
`1999
`
`616.89—dc21
`DNLM/DLC
`for Library of Congress
`9
`British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
`A CIP record is available from the British Library.
`
`_
`
`98-43411
`CIP
`
`_T.,-,.
`
`2 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064 ‘
`
`2 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`MOOD DISORDERS
`
`
`CHAPTER 13
`
`STEVEN L. DUBOVSKY, MD.
`RANDALL BUZAN, MD.
`
`One know: not whether there can be human compassionfor anemia ofthe xoul. I/Vhen thepitch oflife 2': dropped and
`the xpirit ix :0put over and reversed that only 1': horrihle which before wassweet and worldly and ofthe day, the hu-
`man relation dixappearx.
`
`—Oliver Onions
`
` ood
`
`relatively '
`be
`can
`disorders
`.. straightforward, or they can assume
`complex forms that can be difficult to treat. In this chap-
`ter, we review the epidemiology, diagnosis, cornorbidity,
`and treatment of the wide variety of affective syndromes
`that are encountered in psychiatric practice.
`
`EPIDEMIOLOGY
`
`Estimates of the incidence and prevalence of mood disor-
`ders vary. In the United States, the lifetime risk of a major
`depressive episode is said to be around 6 % , and the lifetime
`risk of any mood disorder is said to be around 8% (Cassem
`1995; Kashani and Nair 1995). The prevalence of major
`
`depression ranges from 2.6% to 5.5% in men and from
`6.0% to 11.8% in women (Fava and Davidson 1996). The
`prevalence of dysthymia is 3%—4% (Keller et al. 1996).
`Some reports suggest that as much as 48% of the United
`States population has had one or more lifetime mood epi-
`sodes (Cassem 1995). Most studies have found unipolar
`depression in general to be twice as common in women as
`in men (Reynolds et al. 1990). The meaning of the gender
`difference remains to be clarified. Gender does not appear
`to affect the prevalence of bipolar disorder (Reynolds et al.
`1990). The incidence of major depression is higher in sep-
`arated or divorced people than in married individuals, es-
`pecially men, and in medically ill patients (Lehtinen and
`Joukamaa 1994; Reiger et al. 1988), and depression is asso-
`ciated with greater use of general health services
`
`479
`
`30f89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`3 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`THE AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS TEXTBOOK OF PSYCHIATRY, THIRD EDITION
`480
`
`(VVeissman et al. 1988b). The prevalence ofmaj or depres-
`sion in primary care practice is 4.8%—9.2 %, and the preva-
`lence of all depressive disorders is 9%-—20°/o, which makes
`mood disorders the most common psychiatric problems in
`primary care (McDaniel et al. 1995).
`The effects of culture and stress on the prevalence of
`depression were illustrated by the Cross—National Collab-
`orative Group study of 10 countries, which used the Diag-
`nostic Interview Schedule to make DSM—III (American
`Psychiatric Association 1980) diagnoses (VVeissman et al.
`1996). In this study, the lifetime rate for major depression
`varied from a low of 1.5 cases/ 100 adults in Taiwan to as
`
`many as 19.0/100 in Beirut, and the annual rate of depres-
`sion was as low as 0.8 cases/ 100 in Taiwan and as high as
`5.8/100 in New Zealand.
`
`The prevalence of bipolar disorder is generally re-
`ported as being between 1% and 2.5% (Akiskal 1995b;
`Angst 1995; Bebbington 1995; Kashani and Nair 1995);
`however, some studies suggest rates for bipolar mood dis-
`orders of 3 %—6.5% (Akiskal 1995b; Angst 1995)‘L..The fre-
`quency with which bipolar disorder isdiagnosed probably
`depends on how it is defined; broader definitions produce
`significantly higher rates (Akiskal 1995b; Angst 1995).
`Most prevalence studies require the presence ofmania for a
`bipolar diagnosis to be recorded, but the bipolar II variant,
`which is characterized by episodes of hypomania but not
`mania, is more common than the bipolar I variant (Cassano
`et al. 1989; Simpson et al. 1993). Ifbipolar spectrum disor-
`ders (Akiskal 1995b), or subsyndromal and complex forms
`ofbipolar disorder (discussed later in this chapter), are also
`considered, the incidence of bipolar mood disorder is sub-
`stantially higher. Roughly 10%—-15% of patients with a di-
`agnosis of unipolar depression will eventually receive a re-
`vised diagnosis of bipolar disorder (Olie et al. 1992).
`When conservative criteria are used, between 5% and
`15% of cases of adult depression are found to be bipolar
`(Bebbington 1995; Geller et al. 1996). Akiskal’s group
`(Cassano et al. 1989) found that one-third of patients with
`primary depression met their criteria for bipolar spectrum
`disorders. The risk of bipolarity is higher in juvenile major
`depression—at least 20% in adolescents and 32% in chil-
`dren ages less than 11 years (Geller et al. 1996). The life-
`time rate of bipolar disorder is relatively consistent across
`cultures, ranging from 0.3/100 in Taiwan to 1.5/100 in
`New Zealand (VVeissman et al. 1996).
`In all industrialized countries in the world, the inci-
`dence of depression, mania, suicide, and psychotic mood
`disorders has been increasing in every generation born af-
`ter 1910 (Cross-National Collaborative Group 1992;
`Klerman 1988; Klerman et al. 1985). For unknown rea-
`sons, there was an abrupt jump in the rate of increase for
`
`people born after 1940—a true increase in the incidence of
`mood disorders (cohort effect) and not a function of better
`recognition (Cross-National Collaborative Group 1992;
`Klerman 1988; Klerman et al. 1985). Not only are mood
`disorders becoming more common, but they are appearing
`at an earlier age (especially bipolar mood disorders) (Lasch
`and Weissman 1990).
`Suicide is an obvious public health problem that com-
`plicates mood disorders more frequently than other condi-
`tions. The lifetime risk of suicide in mood disorders is
`
`10%—1 5 % (Barklage 1991; Guze and Robbins 1970;
`Mueller and Leon 1996), and the risk of attempted suicide
`was increased 41-fold in depressed patients compared with
`those with other diagnoses in the Epidemiologic Catch-
`ment Area survey (Petronis et al. 1990). It is well known
`that women attempt suicide more frequently than men, but
`men are more likely to succeed. In one study, however, the
`excess risk of completed suicide in men was entirely ac-
`counted for by a higher prevalence of substance abuse in
`men and a greater likelihood that women have primary re-
`sponsibility for children under age 18 (Young et al. 1994).
`The risk of sujcide=is high in mania as well as in depression.
`Patients with mixed bipolar states characterized by a com-
`bination of depression, rage, and grandiosity may be more
`likely to involve others in a suicide attempt—for example,
`through gunfights with the police. As many as 4% ofpeople
`who commit suicide murder someone else first.
`
`Although many clinicians agree on factors that in-
`crease the risk ofsuicide, formal attempts to predict suicide
`have been disappointing (Oxley and Van Meter 1996). This
`is not surprising; suicide is such a rare event (in the United
`States, the rate is about 11/100,000) that a prohibitively
`large number of patients would have to be followed pro-
`spectively to demonstrate that a constellation of features
`predicted an increased risk. In addition, no consensus exists
`about how long to follow a depressed patient before a con-
`clusion can be made that suicide will not occur. There may
`be a statistically significant association between suicide and
`traditional risk factors such as older age, recent loss, male
`sex, bipolar depression, psychosis, comorbid substance
`abuse, history of a suicide attempt (especially if it was dan-
`gerous), and family history ofsuicide, but this association is
`not necessarily helpful in predicting suicide in an individ-
`ual patient.
`Despite the demonstrated inability of mental health
`professionals to predict (or prevent) suicide in any system-
`atic manner (H. L. Miller et al. 1984), patients, families,
`and courts expect them to be able to do so. In an evaluation
`of immediate suicide risk, factors summarized in Table
`13-1 can be considered (Oxley and Van Meter 1996;
`Pokorny 1993; Young et al. 1994). However, these factors
`
`4of89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064 .,
`
`4 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`III
`
`MOOD DISORDERS
`481
`
`TABLE I3-1. Factors suggesting an increased risk of
`suicide
`
`Demographic factors
`Male sex
`
`Recent loss
`
`Never married
`
`Older age
`
`Symptoms
`
`Severe depression
`
`Anxiety
`
`Hopelessness
`
`Psychosis, especially with command hallucinations
`
`History
`
`History of suicide attempts, especially if multiple or severe
`attempts
`
`Family history of suicide
`Active substance abuse
`
`Suicidal thinking
`
`Presence of a specific plan
`
`Means available to carry out the plan
`
`Absence of factors that would keep the patient from
`completing the plan
`Rehearsal of the plan
`
`at best suggest increased immediate risk. In addition, it is
`not known whether one risk factor is more important than
`another or how risk factors may interact with each other
`(Oxley and Van Meter 1996). Given the current state of
`knowledge, it is probably impossible for anyone to predict
`with any accuracythe long—term risk ofcompleted suicide.
`
`MOOD DISORDERS IN SPECIAL POPULATIONS
`
`Postpartum depression occurs in about 10% of mothers;
`risk factors include a history ofa mood disorder, unwanted
`pregnancy, unemployment of the mother,
`lack of
`breast—feeding, and the mother as head of the household
`(I. Hopkins et al. 1984; Warner et al. 1996). Postpartum
`depression increases the chance of alcohol and illicit drug
`use in teenage mothers (Barnet et al. 1995). There is some
`evidence that depression in a mother adversely affects tem-
`perament (C. T. Beck 1996) and cognitive development
`(Hay and Kumar 1995) in the infant. D epressed mothers of
`preschoolers have more negative perceptions ofand inter-
`actions with their children (Lang et al. 1996).
`Estimates of the prevalence of major depression in el-
`derly people range from 2 %—4% in community samples to
`12 % of medically hospitalized patients to 16% of geriatric
`patients in long—term care (Blazer and Koenig 1996). Geri-
`
`atric depression is associated with an increased likelihood
`of cerebrovascular disease and enlarged ventricles and may
`be more likelythan depression in younger patients to be ac-
`companied by prominent cognitive complaints (Soares and
`Mann 1997).
`
`Major depressive disorder (MDD) is said to occur in as
`many as 18 % ofpreadolescents, with no gender differences
`(Kashani and Nair 1995). However, mood disorders are of-
`ten underdiagnosed in this population because many clini-
`cians still do not believe that depression occurs in children
`and because depression may be more difficult to recognize
`in children than in older patients. Among adolescents, the
`prevalence ofMDD has been reported to be 4.7% in 14- to
`16—year—olds (Kashani and Nair 1995). By this age, depres-
`sion is more common in girls than in boys (Kashani and
`Nair 1995). In nonclinical samples, up to one—third of ado-
`lescents reported some depressive symptoms (Kashani and
`Nair 1995). Major depression in adolescents is associated
`with substance abuse and antisocial behavior, both ofwhich
`sometimes obscure the affective diagnosis (Kashani and
`Nair 1995). The lifetime prevalence of bipolar disorder
`was 0.6% in 150 adolescents who were not psychiatrically
`referred (Kashani and Nair 1995). As discussed later in this
`chapter, many cases ofbipolar disorder in younger patients
`may be overlooked because many depressed children and
`adolescents have not yet had time to exhibit mania and be-
`cause manic symptoms, when present, may be confused
`with behavior disorders and attention—def1cit disorder.
`
`ECONOMICS OF MOOD DISORDERS
`
`Depression produces more impairment of physical func-
`tioning, role functioning, social functioning, and per-
`ceived current health, is associated with more bodily pain,
`and causes patients to spend more days in bed due to poor
`health than hypertension, diabetes, arthritis, and chronic
`pulmonary disease (VVells et al. 1989). In a study ofgeneral
`medical patients in a health maintenance organization, pa-
`tients with depressed mood or anhedonia of2 weeks’ dura-
`tion but with an insufficient number of additional symp-
`toms to meet full criteria for MDD still had 7.7 times as
`
`much impairment of social, family, and work functioning
`as did patients without any depressive symptoms (Olfson
`1996). The total cost of depressive disorders in the United
`States is generally estimated at $44 billion (Hall and VI/"ise
`1995). This is equivalent to the total cost of coronary heart
`disease, a condition that is no more prevalent and less
`readily treatable than depression. The direct costs of treat-
`ing depression are about $12 billion, only $890 million of
`which is accounted for by the price of antidepressants
`(Hall and Wise 1995). Yet, tremendous effort is being
`
`50f89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064 I
`
`5 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`THE AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS TEXTBOOK OE PSYCHIATRY, THIRD EDITION
`482
`
`expended by third—party reviewers to get physicians to pre-
`scribe cheaper antidepressants. The morbidity cost of de-
`pressive disorders in the United States is around $24 bil-
`lion, and the mortality costs are $8 billion; these costs can
`be attributed in part to increased accident rates, substance
`abuse, development ofsomatic illness, and increased use of
`medical hospitalization and outpatient treatment (Hall
`and Wise 1995).
`
`for psychosis in use by most clinicians. Alternating depres-
`sion and elation was called qyclot/aymia, which was classified
`with the personality disorders on the grounds that it was
`chronic and was not caused by a specific circumstance. In
`‘ subsequent editions of DSM (discussed later in this chap-
`ter), mood disorder diagnoses are based on symptom clus-
`ters rather than the presence or absence of an identifiable
`precipitant, since the presence ofa precipitant does not de-
`monstrably affect the course or treatment response of
`mood disorders.
`
`DIAGNOSIS
`
`Attempts to classify depression date back to at least the
`fourth century B.C., when Hippocrates coined the terms
`melanc/aolia (black bile), and mania (to be mad). The inde-
`pendent descriptions in 1854- by two French physicians,
`Falret and Baillarger, offolie circulaire and la folie a double
`fiorme were the first formal diagnoses of alternating epi-
`sodes of mania and depression as a single disorder (Sedler
`1983). At the beginning of the current century, Emil
`Kraepelin differentiated schizophrenia (dementia prac-
`cox) from “manic—depressive insanity” on the basis ofa de-
`teriorating course of the former and an episodic course of
`the latter (Akiskal 1996). Kraepelin (1921) believed that
`manic—depressive insanity was a single illness that included
`“periodic and circular insanity,” mania, and melancholia.
`Many of Kraepelin’s observations of the symptoms and
`course of mood disorders remain accurate, but manic-
`depressive (bipolar) disorder is now known to be a complex
`group of disorders that share features such as a high rate of
`recurrence and alternations of mood states but differ in
`
`other important respects.
`In the United States, the first edition ofDSM, Diagnos-
`tic and Statistical Manual: Mental Disorders (American Psy-
`chiatric Association 1952), reflected the influence of
`Adolph Meyer. Meyer believed that psychiatric disorders
`were reactions to conflict or stress that were more specific
`to the individual than to the illness. Psychotic mood disor-
`ders (e.g., psychotic depressive reaction) were diagnosed
`on the basis not of hallucinations and delusions but of se-
`
`verity and lack ofa precipitant (American Psychiatric Asso-
`ciation 1952). In DSM-II (American Psychiatric Associa-
`tion l968), involutional melanc/oolia and manic—depressive
`psychosis were added. The concept of a depressive reaction
`was maintained as depressive neurosis, which was considered
`a neurotic reaction to an internal conflict or external event.
`
`In the absence of a precipitant, a diagnosis ofpsychotic de-
`pressive reaction was made for a single episode and
`manic—depressive psychosis for recurrent depressive epi-
`sodes, whether or not the patient met traditional criteria
`
`ENDOGENOUS AND REACTIVE DEPRESSION
`
`The differentiation of depression according to whether a
`precipitant is present is derived from an early distinction
`between endogenous (vital or melancholic) and reactive
`depression. In its original use by German descriptive psy-
`chiatrists, the term reactive referred to a depressed patient’s
`ability to react positively to interactions and events and
`thus implied the presence of milder symptomatology. As
`the term was translated into English, however, it came to
`mean depression that developed in reaction to some exter-
`nal stress, thus implying an association between mild de-
`pression and depression in response to stress. In DSM-II,
`this concept was conserved as neurotic depressive reaction. In
`later informal diagnostic schemes, milder forms ofdepres-
`sion that are more responsive to the environment evolved
`into the concept of/aysteroid dysp/aoria, which is a type ofde-
`pression with atypical symptoms that occurs in a patient
`with interpersonal sensitivity and a characterological ten-
`dency to dramatize (Shea and Hirschfeld 1996). In
`DSM—III-R (American Psychiatric Association 198 7) and
`DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994a), the
`term atypical depression (a modifier ofa major depressive ep-
`isode) is more or less equivalent to /aysteroid dysp/aoria and
`the modern derivative of neurotic depression.
`Atypical depression is distinguished by mood reactiv-
`ity (i.e., the capacity to be cheered up temporarily by posi-
`tive interactions or events) as well as by severe fatigue
`(leaden paralysis), sensitivity to rejection, self—pity, a re-
`verse diurnal mood swing (depression is worse later in the
`day), and reverse vegetative symptoms (e.g., increased in-
`stead of decreased appetite and sleep) (M. T Tsuang and
`Faraone 1996). About 15% of depressive episodes have
`atypical features. Atypical symptoms are more common in
`bipolar depression. As is discussed later, atypical depres-
`sion appears to respond better to monoamine oxidase
`inhibitor (MAOI) antidepressants than to other antide-
`pressants.
`In contrast to reactive depression, the term endogenous
`depression referred in the German literature to depression
`
`60f89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`6 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`MOOD DISORDERS
`483
`
`that was unresponsive to the environment and in the Amer-
`ican literature to depression with greater severity, more
`considerable guilt and loss of interest, typical vegetative
`symptoms such as decreased appetite and sleep, and other
`physical symptoms such as difficulty concentrating, early
`morning awakening, and a diurnal mood swing (depression
`is worse in the morning) (M. T. Tsuang and Faraone
`1996). In DSM-IV, the melancbolicfizatures specifier retains
`most of the features of endogenous depression; recent re-
`search suggests that “lack of reactivity” and “distinct qual-
`ity of depressed mood” predict the full syndrome most con-
`’ sistently (K. S. Kendler 1997). However, melancholic
`depression can appear in response to an obvious precipi-
`tant. Endogenous depression has a better response to
`tricyclic antidepressants than does reactive depression and
`has a lower rate of response to psychotherapy and placebo
`(M. T. Tsuang and Faraone 1996).
`Recent work has confirmed that the melancholic sub-
`
`type of major depression is a more severe form ofmajor de-
`pression that is associated with more depressive episodes,
`more symptoms, more impairment, more help-seeking,
`and more comorbidity with anxiety disorders and nicotine
`dependence but that is not qualitatively different from
`nonmelancholic major depression (K. S. Kendler 1997). In
`twins, the presence of MDD with melancholic features in
`one twin increased the risk ofmajor depression but notnec-
`essarily melancholia in the other twin (K. S. Kendler
`1997). Twin studies do not suggest an environmental influ-
`ence on liability to melancholia in depressed patients (K. S.
`Kendler 1997). It is also now appreciated that melancholic
`and atypical depression are not necessarily mutually
`exclusive.
`
`DIAGNOSIS AND DSM-IV
`
`The term afifect usually refers to the outward and change-
`able manifestation of a person’s emotional tone, whereas
`mood is a more enduring emotional orientation that colors
`the person’s psychology (American Psychiatric Associa-
`tion 1984a). However, the change from aflective disorders in
`DSM-III to mood disorders in DSM-IV does not imply a
`reconceptualization of what these disorders primarily in-
`volve (i.e., dysregulation of mood or dysregulation of af-
`fect); the two terms are used interchangeablyin DSM-IV.
`DSM-IV distinguishes between mood episodes and
`mood disorders (Fava and Davidson 1996; First et al.
`1996). An episode is a period lasting at least 2 weeks during
`which there are enough symptoms for full criteria to be met
`for the disorder. The criteria for a major depressive episode
`are summarized in Table 13-2. Patients with or without a
`
`history of mania may have a major depressive episode if
`
`TABLE 13-2. DSM-IV criteria for a major
`depressive episode
`
`A.
`
`Five (or more) of the following symptoms have been
`present during the same 2-week period and represent a
`change from previous functioning; at least one of the
`symptoms is either (1) depressed mood or (2) loss of
`interest or pleasure.
`
`Note: Do not include symptoms that are clearly due to a gen-
`eral medical condition, or mood-incongruent delusions or hal-
`lucinations.
`
`(1) depressed mood most of the day, nearly every day, as
`indicated by either subjective report (e.g., feels sad or
`empty) or observation made by others (e.g., appears
`tearful). Note: In children and adolescents, can be
`irritable mood.
`
`(3)
`
`(2) markedly diminished interest or pleasure in all, or
`almost all, activities most of the day, nearly every day
`(as indicated by either subjective account or
`observation made by others)
`significant weight loss when not dieting or weight
`gain (e.g., a change of more than 5% of body weight
`in a month), or decrease or increase in appetite
`nearly every day. Note: In children, consider failure
`to make expected weight gains.
`insomnia or hypersomnia nearly every day
`(4)
`(5) psychomotor agitation or retardation nearly
`every day (observable by others, not merely
`subjective feelings of restlessness or being slowed
`down)
`
`(6)
`(7)
`
`fatigue or loss of energy nearly every day
`feelings of worthlessness or excessive or
`inappropriate guilt (which may be delusional)
`nearly every day (not merely self-reproach or
`guilt about being sick)
`(8) diminished ability to think or concentrate, or
`indecisiveness, nearly every day (either subjective
`account or as observed by others)
`recurrent thoughts of death (not just fear of dying),
`recurrent suicidal ideation without a specific plan, or
`a suicide attempt or a specific plan for committing
`suicide
`
`(9)
`
`The symptoms do not meet criteria for a mixed
`episode.
`The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or
`impairment in social, occupational, or other important
`areas of functioning.
`The symptoms are not due to the direct physiological
`effects of a substance (e.g., a drug of abuse, a
`medication) or a general medical condition (e.g.,
`hypothyroidism).
`The symptoms are not better accounted for by Bereave-
`ment, i.e., after the loss of a loved one, the symptoms
`persist for longer than 2 months or are characterized by
`marked functional impairment, morbid preoccupation
`with worthlessness, suicidal ideation, psychotic
`symptoms, or psychomotor retardation.
`
`70f89
`
`A_|kermes, Ex . ‘I064
`
`7 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`484
`
`THE AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC PRESS TEXTBOOK OF PSYCHIATRY, THIRD EDITION
`
`they fulfill these criteria, but major depressive disorder
`(MDD) refers to one or more episodes of major depression
`in the absence of mania or hypomania (i.e., unipolar de-
`pression). A major depressive episode may be modified by
`additional specifiers for melancholic features (Table 13-3)
`and/or atypical features (Table 13-4).
`The interpretation of studies of mood disorders is fa-
`cilitated by familiarity with several common terms (Fava
`and Davidson 1996; First et al. 1996). In most treatment
`studies, response is defined as at least 50% improvement,
`whereas partial response is 25%—S 0% improvement and
`nonresponse is < 25% improvement. According to this ter-
`minology, patients who are still half as symptomatic as at
`the beginning of treatment will be considered responders
`at the end of a treatment study. This is not a trivial point,
`given that most studies consider improvement rather than
`remission as the end point. Remission is defined as the state
`of having few or no symptoms of a mood disorder for at
`least 8 weeks. Recovery, the period after remission, is pres-
`ent if no symptoms have been present for more than 8
`weeks, and the term implies that the disorder is quiescent.
`A relapse is a return of symptoms during the period of re-
`mission, and the term implies continuation of the original
`episode; whereas recurrence is a later return of symptoms
`(during recovery), and this term implies development of a
`
`TABKE 13%. DSM-IV melancholic features
`
`specifier
` ::._.?
`
`With melancholic features (can be applied to the current or
`most recent major depressive episode in major depressive dis-
`order and to a major depressive episode in bipolarl or bipolar
`II disorder only if it is the most recent type of mood episode)
`A. Either of the following, occurring during the most
`severe period of the current episode:
`(1)
`loss of pleasure in all, or almost all, activities
`(2)
`lack of reactivity to usually pleasurable stimuli (does
`not feel much better, even temporarily, when
`something good happens)
`
`B. Three (or more) of the following:
`(1) distinct quality of depressed mood (i.e., the
`depressed mood is experienced as distinctly different
`from the kind of feeling experienced after the death
`of a loved one)
`
`(2) depression regularly worse in the morning
`(3)
`early morning awakening (at least 2 hours before
`usual time of awakening)
`
`(4) marked psychomotor retardation or agitation
`
`(5)
`
`(6)
`
`significant anorexia or weight loss
`
`excessive or inappropriate guilt
`
`_:__
`
`TABLE 13-4. DSM-IV atypical features specifier
`__é.
`
`With atypical features (can be applied when these features
`predominate during the most recent 2 weeks of a major de—
`pressive episode in major depressive disorder or in bipolar I or
`bipolar II disorder when the major depressive episode is the
`most recent type of mood episode, or when these features pre-
`dominate during the most recent 2 years of dysthymic disor-
`der)
`
`A. Mood reactivity (i.e., mood brightens in response to
`actual or potential positive events)
`
`B. Two (or more) of the following features:
`(1)
`significant weight gain or increase in appetite
`
`(4)
`
`(2) hypersomnia
`(3)
`leaden paralysis (i.e., heavy, leaden feelings in arms
`or legs)
`long-standing pattern of interpersonal rejection
`sensitivity (not limited to episodes of mood
`disturbance) that results in significant social or
`occupational impairment
`C. Criteria are not met for with melancholic features or
`with catatonic features during the same episode.
`
`
`new episode. These distinctions can be difficult to make in
`clinical practice. For example, mild residual symptoms of
`an initial episode may be overlooked or may be attributed
`to character pathology after improvement ofthe more dra-
`matic manifestations of an episode; this may lead to the
`conclusion that a return of more severe symptoms repre-
`sents a new episode rather than an exacerbation ofthe orig-
`inal episode.
`
`UNIPOLAR AND BIPOLAR I\/IOOD DISORDERS
`
`One ofthe most important distinctions between mood dis-
`orders is the distinction between unipolar and bipolar cat-
`egories (Leonhard 1987a, 1987b). Unipolar mood disor-
`ders are characterized by depressive symptoms in the
`absence of a history of a pathologically elevated mood. In
`bipolar mood disorders, depression alternates or is mixed
`with mania or hypomania. Patients who have only had re-
`current mania (“unipolar mania”) are given the diagnosis
`of bipolar mood disorder on the assumption that they will
`eventually develop an episode of depression (M. T. Tsuang
`and Faraone 1996). DSM-IV criteria for a manic episode
`are summarized in Table 13-5 . Hypomania, a milder form
`of pathologically elevated mood that can be present for a
`shorter period before it is diagnosed, is described in Table
`13-6. Although most people think of elation as a defining
`characteristic of mania and hypomania, many patients ex-
`perience only irritability, anxiety, or a dysphoric sense of
`
`80f89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`8 of 89
`
`Alkermes, Ex. 1064
`
`

`
`MOOD DISORDERS
`485
`
`TABLE 135. DSM-IV criteria for a manic episode
`
`A. A distinct period of abnormally and persistently elevated,
`expansive, or irritable mood, lasting at least 1 week (or
`any duration if hospitalization is necessary).
`
`B. During the period of mood disturbance, three (or more)
`of the following symptoms have persisted (four if the
`mood is only irritable) and have been present to a
`significant degree:
`
`(1)
`
`inflated self—esteem or grandiosity
`
`(2) decreased need for sleep (e.g., feels rested after only
`3 hours of sleep)
`
`(3) more talkative than usual or pressure to keep talking
`
`(4)
`
`flight of ideas or subjective experience that thoughts
`are racing
`
`(5) distractibility (i.e., attention too easily drawn to
`unimportant or irrelevant external stimuli)
`
`(6)
`
`(7)
`
`increase in goal—directed activity (either socially, at
`work or school, or sexually) or psychomotor
`agitation
`
`excessive involvement in pleasurable activities that
`have a high potential for painful consequences (e.g.,
`engaging in unrestrained buying sprees, sexual in-
`discretions, or foolish business investments)
`
`C. The symptoms do not meet criteria for a mixed episode.
`F3 The mood disturbance is sufficiently severe to cause
`marked impairment in occupational functioning or in
`usual social activities or relationships with others, or to
`necessitate hospitalization to prevent harm to self or
`others, or there are psychotic features.
`
`E. The symptoms are not due to the direct physiological
`effects of a substance (e.g., a drug of abuse, a medication,
`or other treatment) or a general medical condition (e.g.,
`hyperthyroidism).
`
`increased energy, as if they were

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket