throbber
James F. Sumowski, PhD
`Maria A. Rocca, MD
`Victoria M. Leavitt, PhD
`Gianna Riccitelli, PhD
`Giancarlo Comi, MD
`John DeLuca, PhD
`Massimo Filippi, MD
`
`Correspondence to
`Dr. Sumowski:
`jsumowski@kesslerfoundation.org
`
`Brain reserve and cognitive reserve in
`multiple sclerosis
`What you’ve got and how you use it
`
`ABSTRACT
`Objective: We first tested the brain reserve (BR) hypothesis in multiple sclerosis (MS) by examining
`whether larger maximal lifetime brain volume (MLBV; determined by genetics) protects against
`disease-related cognitive impairment, and then investigated whether cognitive reserve (CR)
`gained through life experience (intellectually enriching leisure activities) protects against cogni-
`tive decline independently of MLBV (BR).
`Methods: Sixty-two patients with MS (41 relapsing-remitting MS, 21 secondary progressive MS)
`received MRIs to estimate BR (MLBV, estimated with intracranial volume [ICV]) and disease burden
`(T2 lesion load; atrophy of gray matter, white matter, thalamus, and hippocampus). Early-life cog-
`nitive leisure was measured as a source of CR. We assessed cognitive status with tasks of cognitive
`efficiency and memory. Hierarchical regressions were used to investigate whether higher BR (ICV)
`protects against cognitive impairment, and whether higher CR (leisure) independently protects
`against cognitive impairment over and above BR.
`Results: Cognitive status was positively associated with ICV (R2 5 0.066, p 5 0.017). An ICV 3
`disease burden interaction (R2 5 0.050, p 5 0.030) revealed that larger ICV attenuated the
`impact of disease burden on cognition. Controlling for BR, higher education (R2 5 0.047, p 5
`0.030) and leisure (R2 5 0.090, p 5 0.001) predicted better cognition. A leisure 3 disease
`burden interaction (R2 5 0.037, p 5 0.030) showed that leisure independently attenuated the
`impact of disease burden on cognition. Follow-up analyses revealed that BR protected against
`cognitive inefficiency, not memory deficits, whereas CR was more protective against memory
`deficits than cognitive inefficiency.
`Conclusion: We provide evidence of BR in MS, and show that CR independently protects against dis-
`ease-related cognitive decline over and above BR. Lifestyle choices protect against cognitive impair-
`ment independently of genetic factors outside of one’s control. Neurologyâ 2013;80:2186–2193
`
`GLOSSARY
`AD 5 Alzheimer disease; BR 5 brain reserve; CR 5 cognitive reserve; GM 5 gray matter; ICV 5 intracranial volume; MLBV 5
`maximal lifetime brain volume; MS 5 multiple sclerosis; WM 5 white matter.
`
`Many persons with multiple sclerosis (MS) have cognitive impairment, whereas others withstand
`considerable disease burden without cognitive decline.1,2 A similar cognitive-pathologic dissocia-
`tion in Alzheimer disease (AD)3 prompted theories of “brain reserve”4 and “cognitive reserve.”5
`The brain reserve hypothesis posits that larger maximal lifetime brain volume (MLBV) (estimated
`with head size or intracranial volume [ICV]) protects against cognitive decline.4 That is, cognitive
`impairment emerges when brain volume falls beneath a critical threshold; persons with larger
`MLBV withstand greater disease burden before reaching this threshold. Indeed, elders with
`larger MLBV have better cognition6–10 and lower risk of dementia.11,12 Herein, we investigate
`whether MLBV (brain reserve) protects patients with MS from cognitive impairment.
`Brain reserve (MLBV) is determined almost entirely by genetics.13,14 In contrast, the cognitive
`reserve hypothesis posits that enriching experiences (e.g., education, cognitive leisure) protect
`
`From Neuropsychology and Neuroscience (J.F.S., V.M.L., J.D.), Kessler Foundation Research Center, West Orange; Departments of Physical
`Medicine and Rehabilitation (J.F.S., V.M.L., J.D.), and Neurology and Neurosciences (J.D.), UMDNJ–New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ;
`and Neuroimaging Research Unit (M.A.R., G.R., M.F.) and Department of Neurology (M.A.R., G.C., M.F.), San Raffaele Scientific Institute,
`Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy.
`Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.
`
`2186
`
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 1 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`against dementia.5 Indeed, educational attain-
`ment attenuates the effect of AD neuropathol-
`ogy on cognition.15,16 We have extended the
`cognitive reserve hypothesis to MS,17–20 showing
`that lifetime intellectual enrichment attenuates
`the effect of disease burden on cognition.17,19
`Importantly, brain reserve and cognitive reserve
`have been investigated separately, so it remains
`unclear whether enriching life experiences pro-
`tect against cognitive decline independently of
`genetically determined MLBV. Given the mod-
`erate but
`robust correlation between brain
`reserve and cognitive reserve (brain size and
`intelligence21),
`it
`is unknown whether
`the
`protective effect of enriching experiences is
`explained through concomitantly higher brain
`reserve. Herein, we investigate whether early-life
`cognitive leisure (source of cognitive reserve)
`independently protects against cognitive impair-
`ment over and above MLBV (brain reserve) in
`patients with MS.
`
`METHODS Subject enrollment. Subjects were 62 patients
`with definite MS22 (30 women) without an exacerbation in the
`last 4 weeks, no current corticosteroid use, and no history of
`serious psychiatric illness, substance abuse, learning disability,
`or other neurologic condition. Mean age was 43.7 6 11.1 years
`with 13.1 6 3.4 years of education. Given that a) patients retro-
`spectively reported cognitive leisure from their early 20s, and b)
`we wanted formal education completed before participation, all
`patients were at least 25 years old. Mean disease duration was
`13.2 6 6.9 years, with a mean Expanded Disability Status Scale
`score of 3.2 6 2.1. MS phenotypes included relapsing-remitting
`(n 5 41) and secondary progressive (n 5 21). Current disease-
`modifying drug treatments included interferon b-1a (n 5 26) or
`interferon b-1b (n 5 4), glatiramer acetate (n 5 19), azathioprine
`(n 5 3), cyclophosphamide (n 5 2), natalizumab (n 5 2),
`mitoxantrone (n 5 1), or no treatment (n 5 5).
`
`Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient
`consents. Approval was received from the local ethical standards
`committee on human experimentation, and written informed
`consent was obtained from all subjects participating in the study.
`
`Cognitive functioning. Cognitive inefficiency and memory
`problems are the most prevalent cognitive deficits among patients
`with MS.1 Cognitive efficiency was measured with the Symbol
`Digit Modalities Test (oral version) and the Paced Auditory Serial
`Addition Task (3-second version). Norm-referenced z scores were
`calculated for both tasks,23 and the mean of these z scores com-
`prised our cognitive efficiency composite. Memory was assessed
`with the Selective Reminding Test and Spatial Recall Test.
`Norm-referenced z scores were calculated for the Selective Re-
`minding Test (Total Learning, Delayed Recall) and Spatial Recall
`Test (Total Learning, Delayed Recall),23 and the mean of these z
`scores comprised our memory composite. A norm-referenced
`overall cognitive status score was derived as the mean of cognitive
`efficiency and memory composites. Analyses first investigated the
`impact of brain reserve and cognitive reserve on overall cognitive
`status, and then separately for cognitive efficiency and memory.
`
`Lesion load and brain atrophy. Using a 3.0-tesla Philips Intera
`scanner (Philips Healthcare, Guildford, UK), the following brain
`sequences were acquired: a) dual-echo turbo spin echo (repetition
`time/echo time 5 3,500/24–120 milliseconds; fractional anisot-
`ropy 5 150°; field of view 5 240 mm2; matrix 5 256 3 256; echo
`train length 5 5; 44 contiguous, 3-mm-thick axial slices); and b)
`3-dimensional T1-weighted fast field echo (repetition time 5
`25 milliseconds; echo time 5 4.6 milliseconds; fractional anisot-
`ropy 5 30°; field of view 5 230 mm2; matrix 5 256 3 256;
`slice thickness 5 1 mm, 220 contiguous axial slices; in-plane res-
`olution 5 0.89 3 0.89 mm2). T2 lesion load was measured on
`dual-echo scans using a local thresholding segmentation technique
`(Jim 5.0, Xinapse System, www.xinapse.com). Brain atrophy
`was measured as normalized volumes of gray matter (GM) and
`white matter (WM) obtained using SIENAX (version 2.6, part
`of FSL 4.1), whereas normalized volumes of the thalamus and
`hippocampus were obtained using FIRST, then applying the
`same scaling factor calculated with SIENAX. To correct for the
`misclassification of WM lesions, all pixels classified as GM but
`lying neither in the cortical GM nor in the subcortical GM were
`reassigned to the WM before volume calculation. The scaling
`factor within SIENAX is derived from the transformation that
`matches the extracted brain and skull to standard-space brain and
`skull images (derived from the MNI152 standard image): values
`higher than one were obtained for heads with small ICV and values
`lower than one for ICVs larger than the MNI atlas. An advantage of
`this approach is that it does not require that CSF be robustly
`estimated, as it is difficult to distinguish between CSF and skull
`voxels in T images. Lesion load and brain atrophy were used as
`estimates of MS disease burden in subsequent analyses.
`
`Estimate of brain reserve: ICV. ICV is an estimate of MLBV,
`as brain growth corresponds to increased ICV during develop-
`ment,24 and ICV is strongly correlated with brain size in healthy
`persons (e.g., r 5 0.8625). ICV has been used as an estimate of
`brain reserve in previous research (e.g., references 6 and 9). The
`aforementioned scaling factor within SIENAX is a measurement
`of ICV; however, we reversed the direction of values such that
`larger values represent larger ICVs (for ease of presentation).
`Given that men have larger ICVs than women, as in our sample
`(t[60] 5 5.62, p , 0.001), we adjusted ICV measurements for
`sex. The brain reserve hypothesis states that persons with higher
`brain reserve withstand more severe disease burden before expe-
`riencing cognitive decline, not that higher brain reserve slows
`disease progression. As expected, therefore, there was no relation-
`ship between ICV and disease duration (r 5 20.02, p 5 0.88) or
`T2 lesion load (r 5 0.08, p 5 0.55), nor was there a difference
`between disease phenotypes (t[60] 5 0.81, p 5 0.41).
`
`Estimate of cognitive reserve: Cognitive leisure activity.
`As described previously,20 patients were surveyed to quantify par-
`ticipation in 7 cognitive leisure activities during their early 20s
`(table 1). Frequency of participation in each activity was endorsed
`as 1) once or less per year, 2) several times per year, 3) several
`times per month, 4) several times per week, or 5) daily. Total
`frequency across items was our estimate of early-life cognitive
`leisure (mean 5 18.8 6 5.7). This score was interpolated for
`patients missing 1 (n 5 3) or 2 (n 5 4) items. There was no
`difference in leisure frequency between our sample and a larger
`independent matched pilot sample of 124 patients with MS aged
`25 years or older (table 1), indicating that early-life cognitive
`leisure within our sample was representative of MS patients gen-
`erally. We have previously shown no difference between item
`endorsement between patients with MS and healthy persons,
`indicating that cognitive leisure was unaffected by preclinical
`
`Neurology 80 June 11, 2013
`2187
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 2 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`Table 1
`
`Means and SDs for the current sample and matched pilot sample on each of the 7 cognitive leisure
`activities, as well as the total cognitive leisure scorea
`
`Pilot sample (n 5 124),b
`mean 6 SD
`
`Sample (n 5 62),
`mean 6 SD
`
`Difference,
`p values
`
`Cognitive leisure activities
`
`Read books
`
`Read magazines or newspapers
`
`Produce art (e.g., painting, poetry,
`sculpture, song writing, ballet)
`
`Produce nonartistic writing (e.g., diary,
`newsletter, essay, blog)
`
`Play a musical instrument
`
`Play structured games (e.g., cards,
`board games, crossword puzzles)
`
`Participate in hobbies (e.g., gardening,
`model building, Web design)
`
`3.1 6 1.4
`
`3.9 6 1.2
`
`2.5 6 1.3
`
`2.3 6 1.3
`
`2.1 6 1.4
`
`2.8 6 1.2
`
`2.5 6 1.4
`
`3.2 6 1.5
`
`3.8 6 1.4
`
`2.2 6 1.3
`
`2.3 6 1.5
`
`2.0 6 1.5
`
`2.7 6 1.1
`
`2.6 6 1.3
`
`0.80
`
`0.62
`
`0.24
`
`0.80
`
`0.54
`
`0.67
`
`0.59
`
`0.71
`
`Total cognitive leisure activity
`
`19.0 6 4.8
`
`18.7 6 5.6
`
`a There were no differences between the current sample and the larger pilot sample on any items.
`b The pilot sample did not differ in age (42.0 6 10.3 years, p 5 0.29), disease duration (13.2 6 8.4 years, p 5 0.97),
`education (13.6 6 3.2 years, p 5 0.36), or Expanded Disability Status Scale score (3.1 6 1.9, p 5 0.70). There was a
`marginally higher proportion of women (60.5%, p 5 0.076) and patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis
`(78.2%, p 5 0.076) within the pilot sample.
`
`disease.20 The cognitive reserve hypothesis states that lifetime
`enrichment helps patients better withstand disease without cog-
`nitive impairment, not that enriching lifestyles slow disease pro-
`gression. As expected,
`therefore,
`there was no relationship
`between cognitive leisure and disease duration (r 5 0.14, p 5
`0.28) or T2 lesion load (r 5 20.06, p 5 0.67), nor was there a
`difference between disease phenotypes (t[60] 5 0.61, p 5 0.55).
`
`Statistical analyses. Brain reserve. We performed a hierarchical
`regression to investigate the protective effect of brain reserve on
`overall cognitive status. After controlling for age, sex, and pheno-
`type (block 1), estimates of disease burden (T2 lesion load, brain
`atrophy: normalized volumes of cerebral GM, cerebral WM, thal-
`amus, and hippocampus) were entered in a stepwise fashion (block
`2). ICV was entered within block 3 to test whether MLBV predicts
`cognitive status. (Stepwise entry of disease burden estimates within
`block 2 allowed us to assess the contribution of brain reserve over
`and above the estimate of disease burden most associated with cog-
`nitive status.) Finally, the interaction between ICV and disease bur-
`den (estimate retained within block 2) was evaluated in block 4. If
`brain reserve protects against cognitive decline, there should be an
`interaction between ICV and disease burden such that greater ICV
`moderates/attenuates the deleterious impact of disease burden on
`cognitive status. This hierarchical regression was repeated to predict
`cognitive efficiency and memory separately.
`
`Cognitive reserve. We then investigated whether cognitive
`reserve independently protects against disease-related cognitive
`decline, even after controlling for brain reserve. A hierarchical
`regression was again performed to predict overall cognitive status.
`After controlling for the previous brain reserve analysis (block 1),
`education (block 2) and early-life cognitive leisure (block 3) were
`entered, followed by the interaction between disease burden and
`cognitive leisure (block 4). If cognitive reserve independently pro-
`tects against disease-related cognitive decline, there will be an
`interaction whereby greater cognitive leisure moderates/attenu-
`ates the deleterious impact of disease burden on cognitive status.
`This hierarchical regression was repeated to predict cognitive effi-
`ciency and memory separately.
`
`RESULTS Brain reserve. The results for brain reserve
`analyses are presented in table 2.
`Overall cognitive status. After controlling for age, sex,
`and phenotype (block 1), T2 lesion load (the only
`estimate of disease burden retained) was negatively
`associated with cognitive status (block 2). There was
`a medium-sized positive relationship between ICV
`and cognitive status (block 3), such that patients with
`larger ICVs had better cognitive status (figure 1A).
`
`Table 2
`
`Results for the hierarchical regression analyses investigating the protective effect of brain reserve
`(ICV) on overall cognitive status, cognitive efficiency, and memory
`
`Overall cognitive status
`
`Cognitive efficiency
`
`Memory
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.236
`
`0.089
`
`0.066
`
`0.050
`
`p Value
`
`0.001
`
`0.008
`
`0.017
`
`0.030
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.203
`
`0.040
`
`0.100
`
`0.087
`
`p Value
`
`0.004
`
`0.090
`
`0.005
`
`0.005
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.180
`
`0.119
`
`0.012
`
`0.005
`
`p Value
`
`0.009
`
`0.003
`
`0.335
`
`0.528
`
`Age, sex, phenotype
`
`T2LL
`
`ICV
`
`T2LL 3 ICV
`
`Abbreviations: ICV 5 intracranial volume; T2LL 5 T2 lesion load.
`
`2188
`
`Neurology 80 June 11, 2013
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 3 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`Figure 1
`
`Brain reserve protects against disease-related cognitive decline
`
`Graphical depiction of (A) the positive correlation between intracranial volume (ICV) (brain reserve)
`and overall cognitive status, and (B) the interaction between ICV and T2 lesion load (T2LL)
`whereby larger ICV moderates the negative impact of T2LL on cognitive status.
`
`The interaction between ICV and disease burden (T2
`lesion load) was also significant (block 4), such that
`greater
`ICV moderated/attenuated the negative
`impact of disease burden (T2 lesion load) on cogni-
`tive status (figure 1B).
`
`Cognitive efficiency and memory. There was a large
`positive relationship between ICV and cognitive effi-
`ciency (block 3), such that patients with larger ICVs
`showed better cognitive efficiency. There was also an
`interaction whereby greater ICV moderated/attenu-
`ated the negative impact of T2 lesion load on cogni-
`tive efficiency. In contrast, there was no relationship
`between ICV and memory (block 3), nor was the
`interaction significant (block 4). Brain reserve pro-
`tected against disease-related cognitive inefficiency,
`not memory problems.
`
`Cognitive reserve. The results of cognitive reserve anal-
`yses are presented in table 3.
`Overall cognitive status. After accounting for the
`brain reserve analysis (block 1: age, sex, phenotype,
`T2 lesion load, ICV, ICV 3 T2 lesion load), there
`was a positive relationship between cognitive status
`and education (block 2). There was also a large inde-
`pendent positive relationship between cognitive lei-
`sure and cognitive status (block 3), such that patients
`who engaged in more early-life cognitive leisure had
`better cognitive status (figure 2A). The interaction
`between T2 lesion load and cognitive leisure was sig-
`nificant (block 4), with greater cognitive leisure mod-
`erating/attenuating the negative impact of T2 lesion
`load on cognitive status (figure 2B).
`Cognitive efficiency and memory. Cognitive efficiency
`was unrelated to education (block 2) but positively
`related to cognitive leisure (block 3). The interaction
`between T2 lesion load and cognitive leisure on cog-
`nitive efficiency was small and nonsignificant (block
`4). Memory was strongly and positively related to
`both education (block 2) and cognitive leisure (block
`3), and there was a significant small- to medium-sized
`interaction between T2 lesion load and cognitive lei-
`sure (block 4) such that greater cognitive leisure mod-
`erated/attenuated the negative impact of T2 lesion
`load on memory. In summary, cognitive leisure inde-
`pendently contributed to both cognitive efficiency
`and memory over and above brain reserve, but the
`interaction between cognitive leisure and disease bur-
`den was only significant for memory. The cognitive
`
`Table 3
`
`Results for the hierarchical regression analyses investigating the independent protective effect of
`cognitive reserve (leisure) on overall cognitive status, cognitive efficiency, and memory
`
`Overall cognitive status
`
`Cognitive efficiency
`
`Memory
`
`BR analysis
`
`Education
`
`Leisure
`
`T2LL 3 leisure
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.441
`
`0.047
`
`0.090
`
`0.037
`
`p Value
`
`,0.001
`
`0.030
`
`0.001
`
`0.030
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.368
`
`0.012
`
`0.061
`
`0.021
`
`p Value
`
`,0.001
`
`0.278
`
`0.014
`
`0.136
`
`ΔR2
`
`0.315
`
`0.086
`
`0.083
`
`0.040
`
`p Value
`
`0.001
`
`0.007
`
`0.005
`
`0.042
`
`Abbreviations: BR 5 brain reserve; T2LL 5 T2 lesion load.
`
`Neurology 80 June 11, 2013
`2189
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 4 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`Figure 2
`
`Cognitive reserve independently protects against disease-related
`cognitive decline over and above brain reserve
`
`Graphical depiction of (A) the positive correlation between early-life cognitive leisure (cogni-
`tive reserve) and overall cognitive status, and (B) the interaction between early-life cognitive
`leisure and T2 lesion load whereby greater engagement in cognitive leisure moderates the
`negative impact of T2 lesion load on cognitive status. These results demonstrate the inde-
`pendent protection afforded by cognitive reserve over and above brain reserve (intracranial
`volume).
`
`reserve hypothesis was upheld for memory, but less so
`for cognitive efficiency.
`
`Supplemental analyses. We entered brain reserve into
`regression models before cognitive reserve, as MLBV
`is established before education and leisure. Given a
`correlation between education and ICV (r 5 0.25,
`p 5 0.05), we examined whether the relationship
`between brain reserve (ICV) and cognitive efficiency
`is explained by the relationship between education
`and ICV. We reran the brain reserve regression pre-
`dicting cognitive efficacy, now controlling for educa-
`tion in block 1 (before ICV). The main effect of ICV
`(ΔR2 5 0.064, p 5 0.022) and the ICV 3 T2 lesion
`load interaction (ΔR2 5 0.075, p 5 0.009) remained,
`indicating that brain reserve provides independent
`protection from cognitive inefficiency over and above
`
`education. Although there was no link between ICV
`and leisure (r 5 0.03, p 5 0.84), to be thorough we
`reran the regression analysis controlling for education
`and leisure (block 1). There were relatively no
`changes to the effect of ICV (ΔR2 5 0.067, p 5
`0.014) or the ICV 3 T2 lesion load interaction
`(ΔR2 5 0.067, p 5 0.010). Similar to education,
`premorbid intelligence is a common proxy of cogni-
`tive reserve, and correlated with maximal
`lifetime
`brain size.21 Verbal intelligence (an estimate of pre-
`morbid intelligence) was only available for a subsam-
`ple of patients (n 5 36), but was strongly correlated
`with education (r 5 0.62, p , 0.001), indicating that
`they measure similar constructs. Note that verbal
`intelligence was only weakly related to cognitive lei-
`sure (r 5 0.16, p 5 0.350), so the protective effects of
`cognitive leisure reported herein are not explained by
`higher intelligence.
`Consistent with the MS population, half of our
`sample was diagnosed with MS before age 30. As such,
`for some patients, cognitive leisure was performed after
`disease onset. We investigated whether the protective
`effect of cognitive leisure differed based on age of diag-
`nosis. A cognitive leisure 3 disease burden (T2 lesion
`load) 3 age at diagnosis interaction term (controlling
`for 2-way interactions) was not significant for models
`predicting overall cognitive status (ΔR2 5 0.011, p 5
`0.217), cognitive efficiency (ΔR2 5 0.008, p 5 0.361),
`or memory (ΔR2 5 0.010, p 5 0.300). That is, the
`protective effect of cognitive leisure did not differ based
`on age of diagnosis.
`
`DISCUSSION Larger MLBV moderated/attenuated
`the negative impact of disease burden on cognitive sta-
`tus, thereby supporting the brain reserve hypothesis in
`MS. Given the moderate but robust correlation between
`estimates of cognitive reserve and brain reserve,21 the
`protective effect of higher cognitive reserve in previous
`research may be partially or fully explained by concom-
`itantly higher brain reserve. Our results demonstrate
`that early-life intellectual enrichment (cognitive reserve)
`protects patients from disease-related cognitive impair-
`ment independently of MLBV (brain reserve), thereby
`supporting the independent role of enriching experien-
`ces in protecting against cognitive decline.
`Brain reserve protected against cognitive ineffi-
`ciency, not memory decline. This may seem inconsis-
`tent with the aging/AD literature linking larger head
`size or ICV to better cognition in elders6–10 and lower
`risk of dementia11,12; however, closer examination of
`these aging/AD studies confirms that larger head size
`or ICV predicts cognitive efficiency, not memory.6,8,9
`Furthermore,
`longitudinal studies link age-related
`brain atrophy to declines in cognitive efficiency, not
`memory.26,27 Other aging/AD studies link larger ICV
`or head size to better Mini-Mental State Examination
`
`2190
`
`Neurology 80 June 11, 2013
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 5 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`scores,7,10 but the Mini-Mental State Examination
`makes minimal memory demands. Finally, some
`studies show that larger head size protects against
`dementia,11,12 but other studies do not.28,29 Although
`memory impairment is the hallmark of dementia, all
`elders with dementia also have a decline in nonme-
`mory cognition. It is conceivable that higher brain
`reserve protects against nonmemory cognitive decline
`associated with conversion from amnestic mild cog-
`nitive impairment to dementia. Indeed, cognitive
`inefficiency is among the best predictors of conver-
`sion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia.30
`In summary, the aging/AD literature appears to be
`largely consistent with our finding that brain reserve
`is protective against declines in cognitive efficiency,
`not memory.
`The specific link between brain reserve and cogni-
`tive efficiency is consistent with the strong heritability
`of both MLBV13,14 and cognitive efficiency (much
`more than memory).31,32 Strong heritability may con-
`traindicate rehabilitation efforts
`to bolster brain
`reserve and cognitive efficiency. However, rather than
`building brain reserve, persons may be able to pre-
`serve their remaining brain reserve (and protect cog-
`nitive efficiency) through effective disease-modifying
`therapies (which may slow brain volume loss) and by
`maintaining a “brain healthy” lifestyle (e.g., aerobic
`exercise). Indeed, cardiorespiratory fitness is posi-
`tively correlated with brain volume and cognitive effi-
`ciency in healthy persons33 and patients with MS.34 In
`contrast to brain reserve, cognitive reserve is devel-
`oped through enriching life experiences. The stronger
`protective impact of life experience on memory rela-
`tive to cognitive efficiency in the current study is
`consistent with lower heritability of memory relative
`to cognitive efficiency,31,32 which is further aligned
`with lower heritability of hippocampal volume (esti-
`mated genetic variance 5 0.40) relative to ICV
`(0.81).35 That is, 60% of the variance in hippocampal
`volume seems to be attributable to environmental
`factors (relative to 19% for ICV). Indeed, enriching
`cognitive experiences may have a positive impact on
`hippocampal volume in humans.36,37
`Cognitive reserve may protect against cognitive
`decline through superior/optimal neurocognitive pro-
`cessing.5 Consistent with this notion, functional MRI
`research has revealed differences in cerebral processing
`among patients with MS who have greater lifetime intel-
`lectual enrichment,
`including greater activation (or
`lesser deactivation) within the brain’s default network.18
`The default network consists largely of limbic structures,
`including the hippocampus,38 and has been implicated
`in memory.39 We have subsequently demonstrated that
`default network activity during functional MRI predicts
`performance on neuropsychological tasks of memory
`(but not cognitive efficiency) on a separate day.40 These
`
`links among cognitive reserve, default network activity,
`and memory are consistent with our current finding that
`cognitive reserve is specifically protective against mem-
`ory decline; however,
`future research should more
`directly investigate whether differences in default net-
`work activity mediate the relationship between intellec-
`tual enrichment and memory. Although the current
`study provides less support for the role of intellectual
`enrichment in protection against cognitive inefficiency,
`there was a positive correlation between cognitive leisure
`and cognitive efficiency. We have previously shown that
`higher cognitive reserve protects against cognitive inef-
`ficiency in MS,17 although we did not control for brain
`reserve in that study. Taken together, the protective
`impact of cognitive reserve appears to be more pro-
`nounced for memory than for cognitive efficiency, at
`least for patients with MS.
`Given that larger MLBV (estimated with ICV)
`protects against disease-related cognitive inefficiency
`in MS, clinical consideration of patient ICV may
`improve identification of patients at risk for cognitive
`impairment, and efforts to maintain cardiorespiratory
`fitness may help preserve brain reserve and cognitive
`efficiency. As discussed, the specific link between
`brain reserve and cognitive efficiency (not memory)
`in this study is consistent with results from aging
`studies, and should be further explored in aging,
`AD, and other neurologic populations. The current
`study also demonstrates that a cognitively enriching
`lifestyle (a source of cognitive reserve) independently
`protects against cognitive impairment
`(especially
`memory decline) over and above brain reserve. This
`is critical, because estimates of cognitive reserve and
`brain reserve are correlated, and the protective effects
`of higher cognitive reserve in previous research may
`have been at least partially attributable to concomi-
`tantly higher brain reserve. Our finding that early-life
`cognitive leisure protects against memory decline
`more than cognitive inefficiency is consistent with
`lower heritability of memory and hippocampal vol-
`ume relative to cognitive efficiency and ICV. Cogni-
`tive rehabilitation efforts targeting memory in MS
`stand to be most beneficial as the hippocampus is
`more affected by experience than other brain regions.
`Future prospective
`and/or
`experimental
`studies
`should investigate whether intellectual enrichment is
`associated with larger/increased hippocampal volume
`(or lesser/reduced hippocampal atrophy) in patients
`with MS. Finally, the positive link between intellec-
`tual enrichment and cognition in the current and pre-
`vious studies is observational, and cognitive leisure
`activity is almost always sampled from a period before
`disease onset. Longitudinal research is needed to
`investigate whether
`cognitive
`leisure moderates
`decline within MS patients as disease progresses,
`and randomized controlled trials of
`intellectual
`
`Neurology 80 June 11, 2013
`2191
`© 2013 American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
`
`Page 6 of 8
`
`YEDA EXHIBIT NO. 2026
`MYLAN PHARM. v YEDA
`IPR2017-00195
`
`

`

`link
`enrichment are required to establish a causal
`between enrichment and protection from disease-
`related cognitive decline in patients already diagnosed
`with MS. Such evidence is needed to support a pre-
`scription of intellectual enrichment as a therapeutic
`intervention to minimize or prevent disease-related
`cognitive decline.
`
`AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
`James F. Sumowski, PhD, drafted the manuscript for content, contrib-
`uted to the study concept and design and analysis/interpretation of the
`data, and performed statistical analyses. Maria A. Rocca, MD, assisted
`in drafting the manuscript for content and analysis/interpretation of data,
`as well as acquisition of data and study supervision/coordination. Victoria
`M. Leavitt, PhD, assisted in drafting the manuscript for content and con-
`tributed to the interpretation of the data. Gianna Riccitelli, PhD, assisted
`in the analysis of data and acquisition of data. Giancarlo Comi, MD,
`assisted with interpretation of the data. John DeLuca, PhD, assisted in
`drafting the manuscript for content. Massimo Filippi, MD, assisted in
`drafting the manuscript for content and interpretation of data, as well
`as acquisition of data, study supervision, and obtaining funding.
`
`STUDY FUNDING
`This project was funded in part by the NIH (R00HD060765 to J.F.S.).
`
`DISCLOSURE
`J.F. Sumowski reports no disclosures. M.A. Rocca received speakers’
`honoraria from Biogen Idec and Serono Symposia International Founda-
`tion and receives research support from the Italian Ministry of Health
`and Fondazione Italiana Sclerosi Multipla. V.M. Leavitt and G. Riccitelli
`report no disclosures. G. Comi has received personal compensation for
`activities with Teva Neuroscience, Merck Serono, Bayer Schering, No-
`vartis, Sanofi-Aventis Pharmaceuticals, and Biogen-Dompé as a consul-
`tant, speaker, or scientific advisory board member. J. DeLuca received
`salary support through comp

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket