throbber
Journal of Psychopharmacology
`
`http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
`
`1Histamine H
`
`-receptor blockade in humans affects psychomotor performance but not memory
`P. van Ruitenbeek, A. Vermeeren and WJ Riedel
`
` 2008; 22; 663 originally published online Jan 21, 2008; J Psychopharmacol
`DOI: 10.1177/0269881107081526
`
`
`
`
`The online version of this article can be found at:
`http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/6/663
`
`Published by:
`
`
`
`http://www.sagepublications.com
`
`On behalf of:
`
`
`
`
`British Association for Psychopharmacology
`
`
`
`Additional services and information for can be found at:Journal of Psychopharmacology
`
`
`
`Email Alerts:
`
`
`
`http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
`
`Subscriptions:
`
`
`
`http://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptions
`
`
`
`http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
`
`
`
`http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
`
`Citations
` (this article cites 59 articles hosted on the
`SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms):
`
` http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/22/6/663
`
`Downloaded from
`http://jop.sagepub.com
` at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008
`© 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`AVENTIS EXHIBIT 2213
`Mylan v. Aventis, IPR2016-00712
`
`

`
`Original papers
`
`Histamine H1-receptor blockade in
`humans affects psychomotor
`performance but not memory
`
`Journal of Psychopharmacology
`22(6) (2008) 663–672
`© 2008 British Association
`for Psychopharmacology
`ISSN 0269-8811
`SAGE Publications
`Los Angeles, London,
`New Delhi and Singapore
`10.1177/0269881107081526
`
`P van Ruitenbeek Experimental Psychopharmacology Unit, Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Brain and Behaviour Institute,
`Faculty of Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
`A Vermeeren Experimental Psychopharmacology Unit, Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Brain and Behaviour Institute, Faculty of
`Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
`WJ Riedel Experimental Psychopharmacology Unit, Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Brain and Behaviour Institute, Faculty of
`
`Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
`Abstract
`
`Results from recent animal studies suggest an important role for histamine in
`memory functioning. Histaminergic drugs might prove beneficial for people
`suffering from memory impairment. To determine if histamine is involved in
`memory functioning this study evaluates the effects of histaminergic dysfunc-
`tion on memory performance by administrating a H1-antagonist to humans.
`The study was conducted according to a 4-way, double-blind, crossover design
`in 20 healthy female volunteers, aged 18–45 years. On each test day subjects
`completed three test sessions: before and around 2 and 4 h after administra-
`tion of single oral doses of dexchlorpheniramine 2 mg or 4 mg, scopolamine
`1 mg or placebo. Drug effects were assessed using tests of memory, psychomo-
`tor and attention performance, and subjective alertness. Results showed that
`
`dexchlorpheniramine impaired performance in tests of spatial learning, reac-
`tion time, tracking and divided attention but showed no effects on working
`memory, visual memory, word learning or memory scanning. Scopolamine
`induced a similar pattern of effects. In addition, both drugs decreased subjec-
`tive alertness. In conclusion results show that dexchlorpheniramine and scopo-
`lamine clearly impaired performance on psychomotor and attention tasks but
`do not suggest a specific role of the histaminergic system in learning and
`memory in humans.
`
`Keywords
`cognition, memory, psychomotor performance, histamine, acetylcholine, H1-
`antagonist, muscarinic antagonist
`
`Introduction
`
`Recently interest in the role that histamine might play in cognition
`has increased (Bacciottini et al., 2001; Blandina et al., 2004).
`Especially the cognitive domain of learning and memory is of inter-
`est. Animal studies have shown that increases in histaminergic
`functioning improve memory performance and decreases impair it
`[c.f.(Giovannini et al., 1999; Hancock and Fox, 2004; Komater
`et al., 2005; Meguro et al., 1995; Orsetti et al., 2001; Witkin and
`Nelson, 2004)].
`Currently, evidence supporting a role of histamine in learning
`and memory is mainly on the basis of studies in animals. So far
`there are hardly any studies specifically addressing this subject in
`humans. Although there are many studies assessing the behavioural
`effects of histaminergic blockade by centrally acting
`H1-antagonists in humans, they provide little support for the
`hypothesis that decreased histaminergic functioning is associated
`
`with impaired memory functions. This is largely because of the fact
`that most of these studies simply did not include tests for memory
`functioning. Most of them have been conducted in the context of
`behavioural safety of anti-histamines with an emphasis on car-
`driving [for reviews see: (Hindmarch and Shamsi, 1999; O’Hanlon
`and Ramaekers, 1995; Shamsi and Hindmarch, 2000; White and
`Rumbold, 1988)]. As driving performance is more strongly
`dependent on perceptual-motor and attentional functions than on
`memory, only a few studies included memory tests.
`The studies that did assess the effects of H1 blockade on mem-
`ory, show inconsistent results. The majority found no significant
`effects on memory (Acons et al., 2006; Bower et al., 2003; Curran
`et al., 1998; De Brabander, 1990; Kerr et al., 1994; Lee et al., 1988;
`Turner et al., 2006), whereas others did (Hindmarch et al., 2001;
`Katz et al., 1998; Sands et al., 1997; Vuurman et al., 1994). Two
`studies (Katz et al., 1998; Sands et al., 1997) found significant
`effects of diphenhydramine 50 mg on memory performance in
`
`Corresponding author: P. van Ruitenbeek, Experimental Psychopharmacology Unit, Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Brain & Behaviour Institute, Faculty of
`Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands PO Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands. Email: p.vanruitenbeek@psychology.unimaas.nl
`
`Downloaded from at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008 http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
` © 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`

`
`664 Histamine H1-receptor blockade in humans
`
`healthy elderly subjects. Vuurman et al., (1994; 1996) found signif-
`icant learning impairment in children and adolescents after the use
`of diphenhydramine, and Hindmarch and Shamsi, (2001) found a
`significant effect of triprolidine 10 mg on performance of healthy
`young volunteers in a memory scanning task.
`The contradictory results may be partly explained by the
`methods used. The use of insensitive memory tests and assessing
`memory functions before or after the time of peak behavioural
`impairments might explain the absence of effects. Alternatively, the
`significant effects found in some studies might be because of the
`anti-muscarinic effects of the anti-histamines used.
`The aim of the present study was therefore, to assess the effects
`of the relatively selective H1-antagonist dexchlorpheniramine
`(2 and 4 mg) on performance of healthy female volunteers. The
`decision to select only female subjects was based on results from
`a number of previous studies suggesting that females are more
`sensitive to effects of H1-antagonists than males (Ramaekers and
`O’Hanlon, 1994; Robbe, 1990; Vermeeren et al., 2002; Vuurman
`et al., 1994). The effects were to be compared with those of the
`muscarinic antagonist scopolamine that was included as a verum.
`To detect potential effects of the dexchlorpheniramine on mem-
`ory, a number of tests were selected that had previously been shown
`sensitive to detect drug induced impairments and together covered
`a range of important aspects of memory function, such as short-
`term or working memory, long-term memory, memory for verbal
`and visual material and implicit and explicit (declarative) memory
`(Ramaekers et al., 1992; Riedel et al., 1990; Rubinsztein et al.,
`2001; Vermeeren et al., 1995; Vuurman et al., 1994). A word learn-
`ing test and a pattern recognition test were included to assess
`effects on short- and long-term memory for verbal and visual mate-
`rial respectively; a memory scanning task and a syntactic reasoning
`task were included to assess integrity of working memory func-
`tions; and a spatial paired associate learning test was included to
`assess effects on implicit memory and learning. A simple reaction
`time test and a choice reaction test were added to the battery for the
`purpose of measuring effects on response speed without the cogni-
`tive components of the memory tests. To assess time of peak
`impairment of dexchlorpheniramine two tasks were included that
`have repeatedly been shown to be among the most sensitive tests to
`assess the impairing effects of H1-antagonists; that is a critical
`tracking test, and a divided attention test (Burns and Moskowitz,
`1980; Hindmarch and Shamsi, 1999; Meltzer, 1991; Theunissen
`et al., 2004; Vermeeren and O’Hanlon, 1998; Verster et al., 2003).
`Dexchlorpheniramine was selected as a tool drug, because it is
`a first generation H1-antagonist having a moderately high-binding
`affinity for the H1-receptors, but relatively low affinities for mus-
`carinic, alpha-1, alpha-2 and beta-receptors. The affinity for the
`muscarinic receptor is especially low (Kd ⫽ 3300 for chlorpheni-
`ramine) as compared with scopolamine (Kd ⫽ 0.1) (Wiech and
`Martin, 1982). The half-life of chlorpheniramine is approximately
`28 h (range 19–43 h) and maximum plasma concentrations are
`reached at 2.8 h (range 2–4 h) after oral doses (Huang et al., 1982;
`Paton and Webster, 1985). Two studies have shown that single oral
`doses of 4 mg chlorpheniramine produced significant performance
`impairment, which was most pronounced shortly after tmax (Kamei
`et al., 2003; Witek et al., 1995). Yet, in a third study performance
`
`was significantly impaired only at 1.5 h after drug administration
`(Clarke and Nicholson, 1978). On the basis of these results it was
`decided to measure the effects of dexchlorpheniramine at both
`times reported for peak impairment that is between 1.5 and 2.5 h
`and between 3.5 and 4.5 h after administration.
`Scopolamine 1 mg in oral doses (p.o.) was included as an active
`control because the drug is well known for its impairing effects on
`learning and memory (Bartus et al., 1985). Oral doses of 1 mg or
`above have been reported to impair cognitive functioning (Kennedy
`et al., 1990; Rammsayer et al., 2000). Peak plasma concentrations
`are reached at approximately 0.8 h after oral intake and scopo-
`lamine has an average elimination half-life of 4.3 h (Golding et al.,
`1991).
`
`Methods
`
`Subjects
`
`Twenty healthy female volunteers aged between 18 and 45 years
`were recruited as subjects for the study by means of advertisements
`in local newspapers and paid for their participation. Subjects were
`screened using a medical history questionnaire and a physical
`examination, including a 12-lead electrocardiogram, blood chem-
`istry and haematology and urinary tests for pregnancy and drugs of
`abuse (opiates, benzodiazepines, cocaine, tricyclic anti-depressants
`and cannabis). Exclusion criteria were pregnancy or lactation, a
`history or presence of any mental or physical disorder; gastroin-
`testinal, hepatic, renal, cardiovascular or neurological. Also, drug
`abuse, a body mass index (BMI) value outside the limits of 18 and
`28 kg/m2, blood pressure outside the limits of 100 and 150 Hg sys-
`tolic and 60 and 90 Hg diastolic and drinking more than 20 standard
`alcoholic units per week or more than five beverages containing
`caffeine per day, were regarded as exclusion criteria. No drugs or
`medication except oral contraceptives, aspirin and acetaminophen,
`were allowed to be taken from a week before the first test-day until
`the end of the study. Smoking and use of caffeine was prohibited on
`test-days and the use of alcohol from 24 h before and during each
`test-day.
`Three volunteers did not complete the study for reasons unrelat-
`ed to treatment. Two of them withdrew before the second treatment
`session and the third subject was excluded for smoking on the first
`test-day as determined by measurement of carbon monoxide in
`expired air, using a Smokerlyzer Micro® (Bedfont Scientific Ltd).
`These subjects were replaced. Mean ⫾ SD age of the 20 subjects
`who completed the study was 23.7 ⫾ 7.3 years. Their mean ⫾ SD
`BMI was 21.6 ⫾ 3.0 kg/m2. Five subjects were smokers, who on
`average (⫾SD) smoked 4.6 (⫾2.1) cigarettes and no more that 10
`per day. Eighty percent of the subjects were in college or had a sim-
`ilar level of education. The remaining 20% had all at least finished
`high school at an average level of education.
`All subjects received written information about the study proce-
`dures and were able to ask questions. They signed an informed con-
`sent form prior to enrolment. The study was approved by the Ethics
`Committee of Maastricht University and University Hospital
`Maastricht and carried out in accordance with the World Medical
`Association Declaration of Helsinki (Edinburgh, 2000).
`
`Downloaded from at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008 http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
` © 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`

`
`Histamine H1-receptor blockade in humans
`
`665
`
`Study design and treatments
`
`The study was conducted according to a double-blind, placebo-
`controlled, four-way crossover design. Treatments were single oral
`doses of dexchlorpheniramine 2 and 4 mg, scopolamine 1 mg (all
`immediate release formulations) and placebo. Treatments were
`spaced apart by a washout period of at least 7 days.
`
`Procedure
`
`Subjects were individually trained to perform all psychomotor and
`memory tests within 2 weeks prior to their first treatment day.
`On treatment days they arrived at the University around 9:00 h.
`Between 9:45 and 10:45 h, a battery of psychometric tests was
`completed to obtain baseline performance scores. At 11:00 h, sub-
`jects ingested the study medication. Thereafter the test battery was
`repeated at 12:30 and 14:30 h that is between 1.5 and 2.5 h, and 3.5
`and 4.5 h after ingestion, henceforth indicated as t2 and t4.
`The duration of the test battery was approximately 1 h and
`consisted of the tasks and assessments in the following sequence:
`critical tracking, divided attention, subjective alertness, syntactic
`reasoning, immediate pattern recognition, memory scanning, sim-
`ple reaction time, choice reaction time, spatial paired associate
`learning, delayed pattern recognition and critical tracking. The t4
`test battery included a 30-words learning task, that was completed
`only once per test day to prevent interference of the previously
`learned words that same day. The immediate recall part was done
`following the syntactic reasoning test and delayed recall part was
`done following the spatial paired associate learning test. At 0.5 h
`after drug intake an additional critical tracking task was performed,
`with performance on this test then assessed at hourly intervals after
`drug administration. This allowed better monitoring of the time of
`peak impairment induced by both drugs.
`
`Assessments
`
`Memory performance 30-Words learning task – The 30-words
`learning test (Klaassen et al., 2002; Rey, 1964; Riedel et al., 1999)
`assesses short- and long-term verbal memory. Thirty Dutch mono-
`syllabic meaningful nouns and adjectives are presented for 1000 ms
`at a rate of 1 per 2 s and subjects are required to read them aloud.
`When the presentation ends, subjects are required to verbally recall
`as many words as possible (immediate recall). This procedure is
`repeated three times, with the same words presented in the same
`sequence. After a 45 min delay subjects are requested again to
`recall as many words as possible (delayed recall). Finally, subjects
`are presented a series of 30 words on a computer screen that
`include 15 words from the original list and 15 comparable but new
`words. Subjects are asked to indicate as quickly as possible whether
`the presented words are from the original list or not by pressing one
`of two buttons (delayed recognition). Dependent variables were the
`total number of words correctly recalled over the three immediate
`recall trials, the number of correctly recalled words after the delay
`and median reaction time (ms) of correct answers during recogni-
`tion. Since the distribution of reaction times is generally skewed to
`the right, the median RT was selected as dependent variable in all
`
`tasks except when indicated otherwise. The median is less sensitive
`to the presence of one sided outliers as compared with the mean.
`Pattern recognition task – The pattern recognition task assesses
`short- and long-term memory for visual information. In this test
`subjects are presented a series of 15 randomly generated black and
`white block patterns of a 6 ⫻ 4 grid, at a rate of 1 pattern per 3 sec.
`Subjects are asked to memorize the patterns. The same series of
`patterns is presented three times in the same order. Immediately
`thereafter a series of 30 patterns is presented, including 15 patterns
`from the original set and 15 new patterns. Subjects are asked to
`indicate as quickly as possible, whether the presented patterns are
`from the original list or not by pressing one of two buttons. After
`approximately 30 min this recognition procedure is repeated. The
`dependent measures are the median reaction time and the number
`of patterns correctly recognized in the immediate and delayed
`recognition tests.
`Memory scanning task – The memory scanning task (Sternberg,
`1969) measures the time it takes to scan items held in memory as
`part of working memory integrity, separating it from other process-
`es required to respond. When subjects judge whether a test symbol
`is contained in a short memorized sequence of symbols, their mean
`reaction time increases linearly with the length of the sequence.
`The linearity and slope of the function imply the existence of an
`internal serial comparison process whose average rate is between
`20 and 30 items per second. In this test the subjects are presented
`with a set of 1, 2 or 4 consonants, which they are asked to memo-
`rize. Hereafter, a series of 48 consonants is presented on a comput-
`er screen of which 24 are targets and 24 are nontargets. The
`subjects’ task is to indicate as fast as possible whether or not the
`presented letter was one from the memory set by pressing one of
`two buttons. The task consists of six blocks of 48 stimuli with
`different memory sets. The order of the blocks is 1, 2, 4, 4, 2 and 1
`letters, respectively. The median reaction time for correct respons-
`es is recorded and used to calculate individual linear regression
`lines between reaction time and memory set size. The slope of this
`line is a measure of speed of scanning short-term memory, where-
`as the intercept is a measure of psychomotor speed. Both slope
`(ms/letter) and intercept (ms) are outcome measures.
`Spatial paired associate learning – Spatial memory and learn-
`ing is assessed by the spatial paired associate learning task. In this
`task the subject is presented with two highly discriminative pictures
`flanking a central crosshair on a computer screen for 1000 ms.
`Hereafter, one of the two original stimuli (target) or a third (new
`picture) is presented in the centre of the screen and subjects were
`asked to indicate the original location (left or right) of the stimulus
`by pressing a corresponding button as fast as possible. Targets
`appear either left or right with a 50% probability. In total there are
`32 different targets of which 16 targets are presented only once and
`16 targets are presented eight times at the same location with ran-
`dom intervals over the test. Reaction time following repeated pres-
`entation of a target is hypothesized to diminish during the test as a
`result of implicit learning of the location associated with each tar-
`get picture. Therefore, the median reaction time following the
`repeated items should be lower than that following the nonrepeated
`items. The dependent variables are the median reaction times (ms)
`for repeated and nonrepeated targets.
`
`Downloaded from at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008 http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
` © 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`

`
`666 Histamine H1-receptor blockade in humans
`
`Syntactic reasoning task – The syntactic reasoning task
`(Baddeley, 1968) assesses speed and accuracy of logical reasoning
`processes in working memory (Repovs and Baddeley, 2006). The
`task consists of 32 short sentences each describing the order of the
`letters ‘A’ and ‘B’ and belonged to one of four categories: active
`positive, active negative, passive positive and passive negative (e.g.,
`A follows B, A does not follow B, A is followed by B, A is not fol-
`lowed by B, respectively). The sentence is immediately followed by
`a letter-pair (‘A–B’ or ‘B–A’) in the same or opposite order as in the
`sentence. The required response is to indicate as quickly as possi-
`ble whether or not the letters are in the same order as in the sen-
`tence. Dependent variables are the number of correct responses and
`the mean reaction time (ms). The mean reaction time was chosen,
`because the categories differed to a large extend in terms of diffi-
`culty and the median is very sensitive to fluctuations in perform-
`ance on the different categories.
`
`Psychomotor performance and attention Simple and choice reac-
`tion time tasks – The simple and choice reaction time tasks assess
`the speed of perceptual-motor processing without the cognitive
`components of the other tests. The simple reaction time test con-
`sists of a white square that appears on a computer screen and turns
`red after a variable interval. The subject has to press a single button
`as fast as possible. The choice reaction time task is similar to the
`simple reaction time task, with the exception that two squares are
`presented and the subject should press one of two buttons corre-
`sponding to the left or right square turning red. Both tasks consist
`of 48 trials and the dependent measure is the median reaction time
`(ms).
`Critical tracking task – The critical tracking task measures the
`ability to control an unstable triangle, which is displayed on a hor-
`izontal axis on a computer screen, using a joystick (Jex et al.,
`1966). An error signal causes the triangle to become increasingly
`unstable and therefore, it tends to diverge from the centre of the
`axis. The subject has to make compensatory movements to null the
`error in order to keep the triangle in the middle. As the correction
`frequency of the cursor deviations increases as a stochastic function
`of time, the subject is required to make compensatory movements
`with an increasingly higher frequency to the limit of her ability,
`whereupon control is lost. This frequency decreases under the influ-
`ence of sedating drugs. The dependent measure is the average
`frequency at which control is lost of five trials after removing
`the lowest and highest score. This is called the ‘critical frequency’
`or ‘lambdac’ (rad/s).
`Divided attention task – The divided attention task (Moskowitz,
`1973) assesses the ability to perform two tasks simultaneously and
`evaluates cognitive processing resources. The primary task is simi-
`lar to the critical tracking task described above, with the exception
`that the level of difficulty is held constant at 50% of that which is
`just controllable by the subject. Tracking error is measured by the
`absolute distance (in mm) between the cursors position and the
`centre. The secondary task involves the monitoring of 24 digits
`(0–9) that are arranged around the display’s periphery. The
`digits change asynchronously every 5 s. The requirement is to
`respond as rapidly as possible by lifting her foot from a pedal any-
`time the digit ‘2’ appears. Because relative long reaction times are
`
`recorded, outliers are expected to be present at both ends of the dis-
`tribution. Therefore, average reaction time to targets is recorded as
`the response measure in this task. Performance scores in the sub-
`tasks were combined to overall performance scores before analysis,
`because performance in the two subtasks is related within subjects
`and tests. First, average reaction times and tracking error of each
`test were transformed to z-scores using data from all subjects, test
`days and test sessions. Secondly, the standardized scores of the
`subtasks were summed to yield an overall performance score for
`each subject, test day and test session. Overall scores were used
`for further analysis.
`Subjective alertness – Subjective alertness was assessed using a
`mood rating scale consisting of 16 visual analogue scales (i.e.,
`100 mm lines) each representing a continuum between two
`extremes of a certain mood [e.g., alert and drowsy (Bond and
`Lader, 1974)]. Subjects are required to indicate how they feel by
`placing a vertical line on the scale corresponding to their mood at
`that moment. Together these scales provide three factor-analytically
`defined summary scores– ‘alertness’, ‘contentedness’ and ‘calm-
`ness’, of which the factor ‘alertness’ was of primary interest.
`
`Statistical analysis
`
`After unblinding treatments turned out to be not completely
`balanced over periods, due to errors in the ordering of replacement
`medication. Since baseline scores of some tasks showed significant
`Period effects in spite of prior training, assessments at t2 and t4 were
`analysed as changes from baseline at the same day. This is a valid
`method of analysing the data (Van Breukelen, 2006). Changes from
`baseline were screened for normality of the distributions. No sig-
`nificant deviations were found.
`Dependent variables expressed as differences from baseline
`were analysed in repeated measures multivariate analysis of vari-
`ance, according to a 2 (Time) ⫻ 4 (Treatment) factorial model to
`test the main effect of Treatment and the interaction of Treatment
`and Time. The data from the critical tracking task were analysed
`according to a 5 (Time) ⫻ 4 (Treatment) factorial model, but
`otherwise in a similar fashion. Regardless of the outcome of the
`overall F-tests, three planned univariate comparisons were carried
`out between the treatments and placebo for t2 and t4 separately. This
`is a legitimate procedure as the comparisons are suggested by the
`theoretical basis of the experiment (Winer, 1971). All data
`were analysed using SPSS for Windows (version 12.0.1).
`
`Results
`
`Memory
`A summary of mean (⫾SE) performance scores of tasks assessing
`memory performance is presented in Table 1.
`No significant main effects of Treatment or interactions between
`Treatment and Time were found in any memory test except for the
`simplified spatial paired associate learning task. Analysis showed
`that there was a main effect of Treatment on speed of responses to
`⫽ 5.5, P ⬍ 0.008). Longer reaction times
`repeated stimuli. (F3,17
`as compared with placebo were observed after administration of
`
`Downloaded from at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008 http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
` © 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`

`
`Histamine H1-receptor blockade in humans
`
`667
`
`Table 1 Summary of data obtained from tests assessing memory performance after administration of placebo (PLA) dexchlorpheniramine 2 mg (D2), dex-
`chlorpheniramine 4 mg (D4) and scopolamine 1 mg (S1)
`
`Overall analyses of
`changes from baseline
`
`Mean (⫾SEM) scores
`
`Main effect of
`Treatment
`
`P ⫽
`
`Interaction of
`Treatment ⫻
`Time
`P ⫽
`
`Test Session
`
`Treatment
`PLA
`
`D2
`
`D4
`
`S1
`
`Syntactic reasoning task
`Number correct (#)
`
`0.620
`
`0.253
`
`Mean reaction time (ms)
`
`0.865
`
`0.509
`
`Spatial paired associate learning task
`Median reaction time (ms)
`repeated items
`
`0.008
`
`0.176
`
`Median reaction time (ms)
`non-repeated items
`
`0.097
`
`0.432
`
`Word learning task
`Immediate recall (# words correct)
`Delayed recall (# words correct)
`Recognition (ms)
`Pattern recognition task
`Immediate recognition
`Patterns correct (#)
`
`0.075
`
`0.767
`
`Median reaction time (ms)
`
`0.056
`
`0.663
`
`Delayed recognition
`Patterns correct (#)
`
`0.760
`
`0.308
`
`Median reaction time (ms)
`
`0.135
`
`0.370
`
`Sternberg memory scanning task
`Slope (ms/letter)
`
`0.880
`
`0.337
`
`Intercept (ms)
`
`0.379
`
`0.340
`
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`
`t4
`t4
`t4
`
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`Baseline
`t2
`t4
`
`28.4 ⫾ 1.0
`28.4 ⫾ 1.0
`29.2 ⫾ 1.0
`1651 ⫾ 169
`1594 ⫾ 138
`1582 ⫾ 141
`
`509 ⫾ 15
`501 ⫾ 16
`479 ⫾ 12
`521 ⫾ 22
`504 ⫾ 19
`472 ⫾ 15
`
`28.1 ⫾ 1.0
`27.8 ⫾ 1.0
`29.0 ⫾ 1.0
`1591 ⫾ 163
`1546 ⫾ 155
`1464 ⫾ 139
`
`495 ⫾ 15
`520 ⫾ 20*
`485 ⫾ 16
`501 ⫾ 16
`502 ⫾ 20
`489 ⫾ 19
`
`28.2 ⫾ 1.0
`28.4 ⫾ 1.1
`28.9 ⫾ 0.0
`1652 ⫾ 118
`1654 ⫾ 124
`1629 ⫾ 115
`
`503 ⫾ 18
`512 ⫾ 17
`488 ⫾ 17
`494 ⫾ 16
`514 ⫾ 22
`481 ⫾ 16
`
`29.2 ⫾ 1.1
`28.3 ⫾ 1.0
`29.3 ⫾ 1.0
`1614 ⫾ 121
`1544 ⫾ 105
`1556 ⫾ 120
`
`481 ⫾ 13
`504 ⫾ 16*
`497 ⫾ 17*
`487 ⫾ 15
`503 ⫾ 14
`495 ⫾ 18
`
`45.3 ⫾ 1.8
`15.1 ⫾ 1.0
`647 ⫾ 20
`
`42.9 ⫾ 1.9
`14.6 ⫾ 0.8
`644 ⫾ 17
`
`46.9 ⫾ 1.7
`15.6 ⫾ 0.9
`652 ⫾ 17
`
`44.7 ⫾ 1.5
`14.6 ⫾ 0.8
`643 ⫾ 13
`
`26.5 ⫾ 0.5
`26.6 ⫾ 0.7
`27.1 ⫾ 0.5
`972 ⫾ 59
`916 ⫾ 42
`880 ⫾ 50
`
`25.8 ⫾ 0.5
`24.8 ⫾ 0.7
`24.5 ⫾ 0.7
`1033 ⫾ 74
`1004 ⫾ 63
`986 ⫾ 59
`
`44 ⫾ 4
`47 ⫾ 3
`44 ⫾ 3
`345 ⫾ 11
`338 ⫾ 9
`333 ⫾ 10
`
`26.9 ⫾ 0.5
`26.5 ⫾ 0.7
`27.3 ⫾ 0.5
`904 ⫾ 51
`905 ⫾ 46
`858 ⫾ 60
`
`25.7 ⫾ 0.9
`24.5 ⫾ 0.8
`24.4 ⫾ 0.9
`995 ⫾ 65
`1039 ⫾ 78
`965 ⫾ 67
`
`49 ⫾ 3
`50 ⫾ 4
`50 ⫾ 4
`334 ⫾ 9
`338 ⫾ 1
`317 ⫾ 9
`
`26.7 ⫾ 0.5
`25.5 ⫾ 0.8
`26.8 ⫾ 0.6
`928 ⫾ 70
`936 ⫾ 57
`886 ⫾ 68
`
`25.5 ⫾ 0.8
`23.8 ⫾ 0.8
`24.3⫾ 0.7
`970 ⫾ 74
`1022 ⫾ 70
`993 ⫾ 63
`
`52 ⫾ 5
`56 ⫾ 5
`49 ⫾ 5
`331 ⫾ 10
`336 ⫾ 12
`328 ⫾ 10
`
`27.4 ⫾ 0.5
`25.2 ⫾ 1.0
`26.7 ⫾ 0.5
`945 ⫾ 46
`871 ⫾ 43
`891 ⫾ 48
`
`25.9 ⫾ 0.6
`23.3 ⫾ 0.9
`24.2 ⫾ 0.9
`961 ⫾ 47
`1022 ⫾ 82
`978 ⫾ 64
`
`47 ⫾ 4
`53 ⫾ 5
`50 ⫾ 4
`332 ⫾ 11
`338 ⫾ 8
`329 ⫾ 9
`
`Significant (P ⬍ 0.05) a-priori drug-placebo contrasts are indicated as asterisk.
`
`Downloaded from at Universiteit Maastricht on August 28, 2008 http://jop.sagepub.com
`
`
` © 2008 British Association for Psychopharmacology. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
`
`
`
`

`
`668 Histamine H1-receptor blockade in humans
`
`dexchlorpheniramine 2 mg at t2 (F1,19 = 14.5, P < 0.001), and after
`⫽ 12.7, P ⬍ 0.002)
`administration of scopolamine 1 mg at t2 (F1,19
`⫽ 13.6, P ⬍ 0.002).
`and t4 (F1,19
`
`Psychomotor performance and attention
`A summary of mean (⫾SE) performance scores of tasks assessing
`psychomotor functioning and attention is presented in Table 2.
`Analysis of variance of simple reaction time showed a signifi-
`cant main effect of Treatment and a significant interaction between
`⫽ 4.9,
`⫽ 5.2, P ⬍ 0.010, F3,17
`Treatment and Time (F3,17
`P ⬍ 0.012, respectively). Drug-placebo comparisons showed that
`dexchlorpheniramine 2 mg, dexchlorpheniramine 4 mg and scopo-
`lamine 1 mg all significantly increased reaction times at t2
`⫽ 15.5,
`⫽ 5.3, P ⬍ 0.032; F1,19
`⫽ 8.4, P ⬍ 0.009; F1,19
`(F1,19
`P ⬍ 0.001, respectively). These differences were no longer signifi-
`cant at t4.
`Similar results were obtained from the choice reaction time
`task. The main effect of Treatment on response speed was nearly
`⫽ 2.7, P ⬍ 0.078) and drug-placebo comparisons
`significant (F3,17
`at t2 showed significantly longer reaction times after administration
`⫽ 6.3, P ⬍ 0.021), dexchlor-
`of dexchlorpheniramine 2 mg (F1,19
`⫽ 5.9, P ⬍ 0.025) and scopolamine 1 mg
`pheniramine 4 mg (F1,19
`⫽ 10.4, P ⬍ 0.004).
`(F1,19
`
`A main effect of Treatment was also found on critical tracking
`⫽ 3.3, P ⬍ 0.044). Tracking was impaired at
`performance (F3,17
`both 1.5 h as 2.5 h following the administration of dexchlorpheni-
`⫽ 5.5, P ⬍ 0.030), dex-
`⫽ 7.6, P ⬍ 0.012, F1,19
`ramine 2 mg (F1,19
`⫽ 14.7, P ⬍ 0.001, F1,19
`⫽ 7.8,
`chlorpheniramine 4 mg (F1,19
`P ⬍ 0.012) and scopolamine 1 mg (F1,19
`⫽ 13.7, P ⬍ 0.001,
`⫽ 12.3, P ⬍ 0.002). The effects were no longer significant at
`F1,19
`3.5 and 4.5 h after administration (Figure 1).
`Owing to a technical error, reaction times in the visual search
`task of the divided attention test were not recorded for one subject
`during baseline measurements before administration of dexchlor-
`pheniramine 4 mg. Her data were excluded from all analyses of
`performance in this task.
`A significant main effect of Treatment was found on the
`⫽ 7.7, P ⬍ 0.002).
`overall performance measure
`(F3,16
`Dexchlorpheniramine 4 mg produced a significant impairment
`⫽ 7.4, P ⬍ 0.014).
`⫽ 9.9, P ⬍ 0.006) and at t4 (F1,18
`at t2 (F1,18
`Similarly, scopolamine 1 mg impaired performance at both times of
`⫽ 15.6, P ⬍ 0.001).
`⫽ 19.9, P ⬍ 0.001 and F1,19
`assessment (F1,19
`
`Subjective alertness
`
`There were significant main effects of Treatment and a significant
`interaction between Treatment an

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket