throbber
Research in Translation
`
`Can Animal Models of Disease Reliably Inform Human
`Studies?
`
`H. Bart van der Worp1*, David W. Howells2, Emily S. Sena2,3, Michelle J. Porritt2, Sarah Rewell2, Victoria
`O’Collins2, Malcolm R. Macleod3
`
`1 Department of Neurology, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, University Medical Centre Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2 National Stroke Research Institute &
`University of Melbourne Department of Medicine, Austin Health, Melbourne, Australia, 3 Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United
`Kingdom
`
`Animal experiments have contributed
`much to our understanding of mechanisms
`of disease, but their value in predicting the
`effectiveness of
`treatment
`strategies
`in
`clinical trials has remained controversial
`[1–3]. In fact, clinical trials are essential
`because animal studies do not predict with
`sufficient certainty what will happen in
`humans. In a review of animal studies
`published in seven leading scientific jour-
`nals of high impact, about one-third of the
`studies translated at the level of human
`randomised trials, and one-tenth of the
`interventions, were subsequently approved
`for use in patients [1]. However, these
`were studies of high impact
`(median
`citation count, 889), and less frequently
`cited animal research probably has a lower
`likelihood of translation to the clinic. De-
`pending on one’s perspective, this attrition
`rate of 90% may be viewed as either a
`failure or as a success, but it serves to
`illustrate the magnitude of the difficulties
`in translation that beset even findings of
`high impact.
`Recent examples of therapies that failed
`in large randomised clinical trials despite
`substantial reported benefit in a range of
`animal studies include enteral probiotics
`for the prevention of infectious complica-
`tions of acute pancreatitis, NXY-059 for
`acute ischemic stroke, and a range of
`strategies
`to reduce lethal
`reperfusion
`injury in patients with acute myocardial
`infarction [4–7]. In animal models of
`acute ischemic stroke, about 500 ‘‘neuro-
`protective’’ treatment strategies have been
`reported to improve outcome, but only
`aspirin and very early intravenous throm-
`bolysis with alteplase (recombinant tissue-
`plasminogen activator) have proved effec-
`
`Research in Translation discusses health interven-
`tions in the context of translation from basic to
`clinical
`research, or
`from clinical evidence to
`practice.
`
`Linked Research Article
`
`This Research in Translation discuss-
`es the following new study pub-
`lished in PLoS Biology:
`Sena ES, van der Worp HB, Bath
`PMW, Howells DW, Macleod MR
`(2010) Publication bias in reports
`of animal stroke studies leads to
`major overstatement of efficacy.
`PLoS Biol 8(3): e1000344. doi:10.
`1371/journal. pbio.1000344
`at-
`Publication bias
`confounds
`tempts to use systematic reviews
`to assess the efficacy of various
`interventions tested in experiments
`modeling acute ischemic stroke,
`leading to a 30% overstatement of
`efficacy of interventions tested in
`animals.
`
`tive in patients, despite numerous clinical
`trials of other treatment strategies [8,9].
`
`Causes of Failed Translation
`
`The disparity between the results of
`animal models and clinical trials may in
`part be explained by shortcomings of the
`clinical
`trials. For instance,
`these may
`have had insufficient statistical power to
`detect a true benefit of
`the treatment
`
`under study. For practical or commercial
`purposes,
`the designs of some clinical
`trials have also failed to acknowledge the
`limitations of efficacy observed in animal
`studies, for example by allowing therapy
`at later time points when the window of
`opportunity has passed [10,11]. Second-
`ly,
`the failure of apparently promising
`interventions to translate to the clinic
`may also be caused by inadequate ani-
`mal data and overoptimistic conclusions
`about efficacy drawn from methodologi-
`cally flawed animal
`studies. A third
`possible explanation is the lack of exter-
`nal validity, or generalisability, of some
`animal models; in other words, that these
`do not
`sufficiently reflect disease in
`humans. Finally, neutral or negative
`animal studies may be more likely to
`remain unpublished than neutral clinical
`trials, giving the impression that the first
`are more often positive than the second.
`This article aims to address the possible
`sources of bias that threaten the internal
`and external validity of animal studies, to
`provide solutions to improve the relia-
`bility of such studies, and thereby to im-
`prove their translation to the clinic.
`
`Internal Validity
`
`Adequate internal validity of an animal
`experiment
`implies that
`the differences
`observed between groups of animals
`
`Citation: van der Worp HB, Howells DW, Sena ES, Porritt MJ, Rewell S, et al. (2010) Can Animal Models of
`Disease Reliably Inform Human Studies? PLoS Med 7(3): e1000245. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000245
`
`Published March 30, 2010
`
`Copyright: ß 2010 van der Worp et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the
`Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
`medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
`
`Funding: This work was supported in part by the MRC Trials Methodology Hub and the National Health and
`Medical Research Council. The funders played no role in the decision to submit the article nor in its preparation.
`
`Competing Interests: Malcolm R. MacLeod is on the Editorial Board of PLoS Medicine.
`
`Abbreviations: ALS, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis; CAMARADES, Collaborative Approach to Meta-Analysis And
`Review of Animal Data from Experimental Stroke; CONSORT, CONsolidated Standards Of Reporting Trials
`
`* E-mail: H.B.vanderWorp@umcutrecht.nl
`
`Provenance: Commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
`
`PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org
`
`1
`
`March 2010 | Volume 7 |
`
`Issue 3 | e1000245
`
`Page 1 of 8
`
`JAZZ EXHIBIT 2007
`Ranbaxy Inc. (Petitioner) v. Jazz Pharms. Ireland Ltd. (Patent Owner)
`Case IPR2016-00024
`
`

`
`Summary Points
`
`N The value of animal experiments for predicting the effectiveness of treatment
`strategies in clinical trials has remained controversial, mainly because of a
`recurrent failure of interventions apparently promising in animal models to
`translate to the clinic.
`N Translational failure may be explained in part by methodological flaws in animal
`studies,
`leading to systematic bias and thereby to inadequate data and
`incorrect conclusions about efficacy.
`N Failures also result because of critical disparities, usually disease specific,
`between the animal models and the clinical trials testing the treatment
`strategy.
`N Systematic review and meta-analysis of animal studies may aid in the selection
`of the most promising treatment strategies for clinical trials.
`N Publication bias may account for one-third or more of the efficacy reported in
`systematic reviews of animal stroke studies, and probably also plays a
`substantial role in the experimental literature for other diseases.
`N We provide recommendations for the reporting of aspects of study quality in
`publications of comparisons of treatment strategies in animal models of
`disease.
`
`allocated to different interventions may,
`apart from random error, be attributed to
`the treatment under investigation [12].
`The internal validity may be reduced by
`four types of bias through which system-
`atic differences between treatment groups
`are introduced (Table 1). Just
`like any
`clinical
`trial, each formal animal study
`testing the effectiveness of an intervention
`should be based on a well-designed study
`protocol addressing the design and con-
`duct of the study, as well as the analysis
`and reporting of its results. Aspects of the
`design, conduct, and analysis of an animal
`experiment that help to reduce bias and to
`improve the reliability and reproducibility
`of the results are discussed below. As the
`impact of study quality has been studied
`much more extensively in clinical trials
`than in animal studies, the backgrounds
`and recommendations
`regarding these
`issues are largely based on the clinical
`CONsolidated Standards of Reporting
`Trials (CONSORT) statement, and to a
`smaller extent on published recommenda-
`tions and guidelines for the conduct and
`
`studies of acute
`reporting of animal
`ischemic stroke [13–17].
`
`Randomisation
`treatment
`To prevent
`selection bias,
`allocation should be based on randomisa-
`tion (Box 1), a method that
`is almost
`ubiquitous in clinical treatment trials. In
`part, this prevents the investigator from
`having to choose which treatment a
`particular animal will receive, a process
`which might result (consciously or subcon-
`sciously) in animals which are thought to
`do particularly well or particularly badly
`being overrepresented in a particular
`treatment group. Foreknowledge of treat-
`ment group assignment may also lead to
`selective exclusion of animals based on
`prognostic factors [13]. These problems
`can arise with any method in which group
`allocation is known in advance or can be
`predicted. Such methods include both the
`use of predetermined rules (e.g., assign-
`ment in alternation or on the basis of the
`days of the week) or of open randomisation
`schedules. Picking animals ‘‘at random’’
`
`Table 1. Four types of bias threatening internal validity.
`
`the risk of
`from their cages also has
`conscious or subconscious manipulation,
`and does not represent true randomisation.
`Randomisation may appear redundant
`if the animals form a homogeneous group
`from a genetic and environmental per-
`spective, as often is the case with rats and
`other rodents. However, it is not only the
`animal itself but mainly the induction of
`the disease that may give rise to variation.
`For example, there is a large variation in
`infarct size in most rat models of ischaemic
`stroke not only because of interindividual
`differences in collateral circulation—even
`in inbred strains—but also because in
`some animals the artery is occluded better
`than in others and because the models are
`inherently vulnerable to complications
`that may affect outcome, such as peripro-
`cedural hypotension or hypoxemia. It is
`because of this variation that randomisa-
`tion, ideally occurring after the injury or
`disease has been induced, is essential.
`In clinical trials, automated randomisa-
`tion techniques such as random number
`generation are most commonly used, but
`manual methods (such as tossing a coin or
`throwing dice) are also acceptable as long
`as
`these cannot be manipulated. By
`preference,
`such manual
`techniques
`should be performed by an independent
`person.
`
`Blinding
`In studies that are blinded throughout
`their course, the investigators and other
`persons involved will not be influenced by
`knowledge of the treatment assignment,
`thereby preventing performance, detec-
`tion, and attrition bias. Knowledge of
`treatment assignment may subconsciously
`or otherwise affect the supply of additional
`care, outcome assessment, and decisions to
`withdraw animals from the experiment.
`In contrast to allocation concealment
`(Box 1), blinding may not always be
`possible in all stages of an experiment,
`for example when the treatment under
`investigation concerns a surgical proce-
`
`Definition
`
`Solution
`
`Biased allocation to treatment groups
`
`Randomisation; allocation concealment
`
`Systematic differences in care between the treatment groups,
`apart from the intervention under study
`
`Blinding
`
`Blinding
`
`Type of Bias
`
`Selectionbias
`
`Performancebias
`
`Detection(ascertainment,assessment,or
`observer)bias
`
`Systematic distortion of the results of a study that occurs when the
`person assessing outcome has knowledge of treatment assignment.
`
`Attritionbias
`
`Unequal occurrence and handling of deviations from protocol
`and loss to follow-up between treatment groups
`
`Blinding; intention-to-treat analysis
`
`Adapted from [12,13].
`doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000245.t001
`
`PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org
`
`2
`
`March 2010 | Volume 7 |
`
`Issue 3 | e1000245
`
`Page 2 of 8
`
`

`
`Box 1. Glossary
`
`N Allocation concealment: Concealing the allocation sequence from those
`assigning animals to intervention groups, until the moment of assignment.
`N Bias: Systematic distortion of the estimated intervention effect away from the
`‘‘truth,’’ caused by inadequacies in the design, conduct, or analysis of an
`experiment.
`N Blinding (masking): Keeping the persons who perform the experiment,
`collect data, and assess outcome unaware of the treatment allocation.
`N Eligibility criteria: Inclusion and exclusion criteria: the characteristics that
`define which animals are eligible to be enrolled in a study.
`N External validity: The extent to which the results of an animal experiment
`provide a correct basis for generalisations to the human condition.
`N Intention-to-treat analysis: Analysis of data of all animals included in the
`group to which they were assigned, regardless of whether they completed the
`intervention.
`N Internal validity: The extent to which the design and conduct of the trial
`eliminate the possibility of bias.
`N Power: The probability that a study will detect a statistically significant effect of
`a specified size.
`N Randomisation: Randomly allocating the intervention under study across the
`comparison groups, to ensure that group assignment cannot be predicted.
`N Sample size: The number of animals in the study
`
`Definitions adapted from [13] and from Wikipedia (http://www.wikipedia.org,
`accessed on 9 November 2009).
`
`dure. However, blinding of outcome as-
`sessment is almost always possible.
`In clinical trials, the most common form
`of blinding is double blinding, in which the
`patients, the investigators, and the care-
`givers are unaware of
`the intervention
`assignment. Because the patient does not
`know which treatment is being adminis-
`tered, the placebo effect will be similar
`across the comparison groups. As animals
`are not susceptible to the placebo effect,
`double blinding is not an issue in animal
`studies. Notwithstanding the influence that
`unblinded animal handling can have on
`performance in neurobehavioural
`tasks
`[18],
`the fact
`that
`in some articles of
`animal studies ‘‘double blinding’’
`is re-
`ported raises questions about the authors’
`knowledge of blinding as well as about the
`review and editorial processes of
`the
`journals in which the studies were pub-
`lished [19,20].
`
`Sample Size Calculation
`Selection of target sample size is a critical
`factor in the design of any comparison
`study. The study should be large enough to
`have a high probability of detecting a
`treatment effect of a given size if such an
`effect truly exists, but also pay attention to
`legal requirements and ethical and practical
`considerations
`to keep the number of
`animals as small as possible. The required
`sample size should be determined before
`the start of the study with a formal sample
`
`size calculation, of which the fundamental
`elements of statistical significance (a), effect
`size (d), power (1–b), and standard devia-
`tion of the measurements have been ex-
`plained in numerous articles [13,21]. Un-
`fortunately, the assumptions on variation of
`the measurements are often based on
`incomplete data, and small errors can
`lead to a study that is either under- or
`overpowered. From an ethical point of
`view, underpowered studies are undesir-
`able, as
`they might
`lead to the false
`conclusion that the intervention is without
`efficacy, and all included animals will have
`been used to no benefit. Overpowered
`studies would also be unethical, but these
`are much less prevalent.
`
`Monitoring of Physiological
`Parameters
`Depending on the disease under inves-
`tigation, a range of physiological variables
`may affect outcome, and inadequate
`control of
`these factors may lead to
`erroneous conclusions. Whether or not
`physiological parameters should be assess-
`ed, and for how long, therefore depends
`on the model and on the tested condition.
`
`Eligibility Criteria and Drop-Outs
`Because of
`their complexity, many
`animal models are inherently vulnerable
`to complications—such as
`inadvertent
`blood loss during surgery to induce
`cerebral or myocardial
`ischemia—that
`
`are not related to the treatment under
`study but that may have a large effect on
`outcome. Given the explanatory character
`of preclinical studies,
`it
`is justifiable to
`exclude animals with such complications
`from the analyses of
`treatment effects,
`provided that the eligibility criteria are
`predefined and not determined on a post-
`hoc basis, and that the person responsible
`for the exclusion of animals is unaware of
`the treatment assignment.
`In clinical trials, inclusion and exclusion
`criteria are usually applied before enrol-
`ment
`in the study, but
`for the reason
`above, in animal studies it is justifiable also
`to apply these criteria during the course of
`the study. However,
`these should be
`limited to complications that are demon-
`strably not related to the intervention
`under study, as this may otherwise lead
`to attrition bias. For example, if a potential
`novel
`treatment
`for colorectal cancer
`increases instead of reduces tumour pro-
`gression, thereby weakening the animals
`and increasing their susceptibility to infec-
`tions, exclusion of animals dying prema-
`turely because of respiratory tract infec-
`tions may lead to selective exclusion of
`animals with the largest
`tumours and
`mask the detrimental effect of the novel
`intervention.
`
`Statistical Analysis
`The statistical analysis of the results of
`animal experiments has been given elab-
`orate attention in review articles and books
`[22]. However, even when data appear
`simple and their analysis straightforward,
`inadequate techniques are often used.
`Common examples include the use of a
`t-test for nonparametric data, calculating
`means and standard deviations for ordinal
`data, and treating multiple observations
`from one animal as independent.
`In clinical trials, an intention-to-treat
`analysis is generally favoured because it
`avoids bias associated with nonrandom
`loss of participants [13]. As explained
`above, the explanatory character of most
`studies
`justifies
`the use of an analysis
`restricted to data from animals that have
`fulfilled all eligibility criteria, provided that
`all animals excluded from the analysis are
`accounted for and that those exclusions
`have been made without knowledge of
`treatment group allocation.
`
`Control of Study Conduct
`The careers of investigators at academic
`institutions and in industry depend in part
`on the number and impact of
`their
`publications, and these investigators may
`be all
`too aware of
`the fact
`that
`the
`prospect of
`their work being published
`
`PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org
`
`3
`
`March 2010 | Volume 7 |
`
`Issue 3 | e1000245
`
`Page 3 of 8
`
`

`
`increases when positive results are ob-
`tained. This underscores not only the
`importance of randomisation, allocation
`concealment, and blinding, but also the
`need for adequate monitoring and audit-
`ing of
`laboratory experiments by third
`parties. Indeed, adopting a multicentre
`approach to animal
`studies has been
`proposed, as a way of securing transparent
`quality control [23].
`
`Bias in Animal Studies
`The presence of bias in animal studies
`has been tested most extensively in studies
`of acute ischemic stroke, probably because
`in this field the gap between the laboratory
`and the clinic is both very large and well
`recognised [8]. In systematic reviews of
`different
`interventions
`tested in animal
`models of acute ischemic stroke, other
`emergencies, Parkinson’s disease, multiple
`sclerosis, or amyotrophic lateral sclerosis,
`generally about a third or less of
`the
`studies reported random allocation to the
`treatment group, and even fewer studies
`reported concealment of treatment alloca-
`tion or blinded outcome
`assessment
`[2,16,19,24,25]. Even when reported, the
`methods used for
`randomisation and
`blinding were rarely given. A priori sample
`size calculations were reported in 0%–3%
`of the studies (Table 2).
`Complications of
`the disease and/or
`treatment under study were reported in
`
`19% of the studies of hypothermia for acute
`ischemic stroke. All but one of these com-
`plications concerned premature death, and
`about 90% of these animals were excluded
`from the analyses [20]. In another review of
`several treatment strategies for acute ische-
`mic stroke, only one of 45 studies men-
`tioned predefined inclusion and exclusion
`criteria, and in just 12 articles
`(27%)
`exclusion of animals from analysis was
`mentioned and substantiated. It is difficult
`to believe that in every other study every
`single experiment went as smoothly as the
`investigators had planned [19].
`Two factors limit the interpretation of
`the above-mentioned data. First, the as-
`sessment of possible confounders in system-
`atic reviews was based on what was
`reported in the articles, and may have been
`incomplete because the authors considered
`these aspects of study design not sufficiently
`relevant
`to be mentioned. In addition,
`definitions of
`randomisation, allocation
`concealment, and blinding might vary
`across studies, and, for example, randomly
`picking animals from their cages may have
`been called ‘‘randomisation.’’ Indeed, a
`survey of a sample of authors of publica-
`tions included in such reviews suggested
`that this was sometimes the case [26].
`
`Quality Checklists
`At least four different but largely over-
`lapping study-quality checklists have been
`
`proposed for use in animal studies of
`focal cerebral
`ischemia. These check-
`lists have included items relating first to
`the range of circumstances under which
`efficacy has been shown and second to
`the characteristics that might act as a
`source of bias in individual experiments
`[16].
`Assessment of overall methodological
`quality of
`individual studies with these
`checklists is limited by controversy about
`the composition of
`the checklists and,
`more importantly, because the weight of
`each of
`the individual components has
`remained uncertain. For example, in the
`most
`frequently used CAMARADES
`checklist,
`‘‘adequate allocation conceal-
`ment’’ may have a much larger impact
`on effect
`size than ‘‘compliance with
`regulatory requirements’’ [16].
`
`Does Methodological Quality
`Matter?
`Several systematic reviews and meta-
`analyses have provided empirical evi-
`dence that
`inadequate methodological
`approaches
`in controlled clinical
`trials
`are associated with bias. Clinical trials in
`which authors did not report randomisa-
`tion, adequately conceal treatment allo-
`cation, or use double blinding yielded
`larger estimates of treatment effects than
`trials in which these study quality issues
`were reported [12,27–32].
`
`Table 2. Randomisation, blinded outcome assessment, and sample size calculation in systematic reviews of animal studies.
`
`Disease Modeled
`
`Heart failure [24]
`
`Emergency medicine [33]
`
`Ischemic stroke [19]
`
`Ischemic stroke [49]
`
`Ischemic stroke [50]
`
`Ischemic stroke [51]
`
`Traumatic brain injury [2]
`
`Year of
`Publication
`
`Number of
`Publications
`
`Randomisation,
`n (%)
`
`Blinded Outcome
`Assessment, n (%)
`
`A Priori Sample Size
`Calculation, n (%)
`
`2003
`
`2003
`
`2005
`
`2005
`
`2005
`
`2006
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`9
`
`290
`
`45
`
`73
`
`25
`
`27
`
`17
`
`8
`
`6 (67)
`
`94 (32)
`
`19 (42)
`
`17 (23)
`
`8 (32)
`
`2 (7)
`
`2 (12)
`
`3 (38)
`
`9 (100)
`
`31 (11)
`
`18 (40)
`
`9 (12)
`
`1 (4)
`
`1 (4)
`
`3 (18)
`
`4 (50)
`
`0 (0)
`
`N/A
`
`0 (0)
`
`N/A
`
`N/A
`
`N/A
`
`N/A
`
`N/A
`
`Hemorrhage in surgery [2]
`
`Neonatal RDS [2]
`
`Osteoporosis [2]
`Ischemic stroke [16]a
`
`Parkinson’s disease [16]
`
`Multiple sclerosis [16]
`
`ALS [45]
`
`Brain injury [52]
`
`Ischemic stroke [25]
`
`Ischemic stroke [53]
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`2007
`
`2008
`
`2008
`
`2009
`
`56
`
`16
`
`288
`
`118
`
`183
`
`85
`
`18
`
`9
`
`19
`
`14 (25)
`
`5 (31)
`
`103 (36)
`
`14 (12)
`
`4 (2)
`
`21 (25)
`
`12 (67)
`
`3 (33)
`
`1 (5)
`
`3 (5)
`
`0 (0)
`
`84 (29)
`
`18 (15)
`
`20 (11)
`
`21 (25)
`
`7 (39)
`
`4 (44)
`
`5 (26)
`
`N/A
`
`N/A
`
`8 (3)
`
`0 (0)
`
`0 (0)
`
`1 (1)
`
`N/A
`
`2 (22)
`
`0 (0)
`
`aSummarises the data of six systematic reviews of treatment strategies for acute ischemic stroke. There is an overlap of 18 publications between references [16] and [19].
`ALS, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis; N/A, data not available; RDS, respiratory distress syndrome.
`doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000245.t002
`
`PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org
`
`4
`
`March 2010 | Volume 7 |
`
`Issue 3 | e1000245
`
`Page 4 of 8
`
`

`
`The impact of methodological quality
`on the effect size in animal studies has
`been examined less extensively. In animal
`studies testing interventions in emergency
`medicine, the odds of a positive result were
`more than three times as large if
`the
`publication did not report randomisation
`or blinding as compared with publications
`that did report
`these methods [33]. In
`systematic reviews of FK-506 or hypother-
`mia for acute ischemic stroke, an inverse
`relation was found between effect size and
`study quality, as assessed by a ten-item
`study-quality checklist [20,34]. The same
`review on hypothermia found large over-
`statements of
`the reduction in infarct
`volume in animal stroke studies without
`randomisation or blinded outcome assess-
`ment when they were compared with
`randomised or blinded studies, but a
`meta-analysis of 13 meta-analyses in ex-
`perimental stroke describing outcomes in a
`total of 15,635 animals found no statisti-
`cally significant effect of these quality items
`on effect size. In this meta-meta-analysis,
`only allocation concealment was associat-
`ed with a larger effect size [35].
`A limitation of the meta-analyses assess-
`ing the effect of study quality aspects on
`effect size is the fact that no consideration
`has been given to possible interactions
`between quality items, and that only uni-
`variate analyses were performed. Howev-
`er,
`individual quality aspects that may
`affect the results of meta-analyses of ani-
`mal studies are unlikely to operate inde-
`pendently. For example, nonrandomised
`studies may be more likely than rando-
`mised studies to disregard other quality
`issues, such as allocation concealment or
`blinding, or to use shorter delays for the
`initiation of treatment, all of which may
`affect study results. The relative impor-
`tance of the various possible sources of bias
`is therefore not yet known and is the
`subject of ongoing research.
`
`External Validity
`
`Even if the design and conduct of an
`animal study are sound and eliminate the
`possibility of bias, the translation of
`its
`results to the clinic may fail because of
`disparities between the model and the
`clinical trials testing the treatment strategy.
`Common causes of such reduced external
`validity are listed in Box 2 and are not
`limited to differences between animals and
`humans in the pathophysiology of disease,
`but also include differences in comorbid-
`ities, the use of co-medication, timing of
`the administration and dosing of the study
`treatment, and the selection of outcome
`measures. Whereas the issues for internal
`
`Box 2. Common Causes of Reduced External Validity of Animal
`Studies
`
`N The induction of the disease under study in animals that are young and
`otherwise healthy, whereas in patients the disease mainly occurs in elderly
`people with co-morbidities.
`N Assessment of the effect of a treatment in a homogeneous group of animals
`versus a heterogeneous group of patients.
`N The use of either male or female animals only, whereas the disease occurs in
`male and female patients alike.
`N The use of models for inducing a disease or injury with insufficient similarity to
`the human condition.
`N Delays to start of treatment that are unrealistic in the clinic; the use of doses
`that are toxic or not tolerated by patients.
`N Differences in outcome measures and the timing of outcome assessment
`between animal studies and clinical trials.
`
`validity probably apply to the majority of
`animal models regardless of the disease
`under study,
`the external validity of a
`model will
`largely be determined by
`disease-specific factors.
`
`Stroke Models
`As mentioned above, the translation of
`efficacy from animal studies to human
`disease has perhaps been least successful
`for neurological diseases in general and
`for ischaemic stroke in particular. As there
`is also no other animal model of disease
`that has been more rigorously subjected
`to systematic review and meta-analysis,
`stroke serves as a good example of where
`difficulties in translation might arise.
`The incidence of stroke increases with
`age, and stroke patients commonly have
`other health problems that might increase
`their stroke risk, complicate their clinical
`course, and affect functional outcome. Of
`patients with acute stroke, up to 75% and
`68% have hypertension and hyperglycae-
`mia, respectively [9,36]. While it is im-
`portant to know whether candidate stroke
`drugs retain efficacy in the face of these
`comorbidities, only about 10% of
`focal
`ischaemia studies have used animals with
`hypertension, and fewer than 1% have
`used animals with induced diabetes. In
`addition, animals used in stroke models
`were almost invariably young, and female
`animals were highly underrepresented.
`Over 95% of the studies were performed
`in rats and mice, and animals that are
`perhaps biologically closer to humans are
`hardly ever used [16,19]. Moreover, most
`animal studies have failed to acknowledge
`the inevitable delay between the onset
`of symptoms and the possibility to start
`treatment
`in patients.
`In a systematic
`review of animal studies of five different
`neuroprotective agents that had also been
`tested in 21 clinical trials including a total
`
`of more than 12,000 patients with acute
`ischaemic stroke,
`the median time be-
`tween the onset of ischaemia and start of
`treatment in the animal studies was just 10
`minutes, which is infeasible in the clinic
`[19]. In the large majority of clinical trials,
`functional outcome is the primary mea-
`sure of efficacy, whereas animal studies
`usually rely on infarct volume. Several
`studies have suggested that
`in patients
`the relation between infarct volume and
`functional outcome is moderate at best
`[37,38]. Finally, the usual time of outcome
`assessment of 1–3 days in animal models
`contrasts sharply with that of 3 months in
`patients [19]. For these reasons, it is not
`surprising that, except for thrombolysis, all
`treatment strategies proven effective in the
`laboratory have failed in the clinic.
`
`Other Acute Disease Models
`Differences between animal models and
`clinical trials similar to those mentioned
`above have been proposed as causes of the
`recurrent failure of a range of strategies to
`reduce lethal reperfusion injury in patients
`with acute myocardial
`infarction [6,7].
`The failure to acknowledge the presence of
`often severe comorbidities in patients, and
`short and clinically unattainable onset-to-
`treatment delays, have also limited the
`external validity of animal models of
`traumatic brain injury [2].
`
`Chronic Disease Models
`The external validity of models of
`chronic and progressive diseases may also
`be challenged by other factors. For the
`treatment of Parkinson’s disease, research-
`ers have mainly relied on injury-induced
`models that mimic nigrostriatal dopamine
`deficiency but do not recapitulate the slow,
`progressive, and degenerative nature of
`the disease in humans. Whereas in clinical
`trials interventions were administered over
`
`PLoS Medicine | www.plosmedicine.org
`
`5
`
`March 2010 | Volume 7 |
`
`Issue 3 | e1000245
`
`Page 5 of 8
`
`

`
`a prolonged period of time in the context
`of this slowly progressive disease, putative
`neuroprotective agents were administered
`before or at the same time as an acute
`Parkinson’s disease-like lesion was induced
`in the typical underlying animal studies
`[39].
`Based on the identification of single
`point-mutations
`in the gene encoding
`superoxide dismutase 1 (SOD1) in about
`3% of
`the patients with amyotrophic
`lateral sclerosis (ALS), mice carrying 23
`copies of the human SOD1G93A trans-
`gene are considered the standard model
`for therapeutic studies of ALS. Apart from
`the fact that this model may be valid only
`for patients with SOD1 mutations,
`the
`mice may suffer from a phenotype that is
`so aggressive and so overdriven by its 23
`copies of the transgene that no pharma-
`cological intervention outside of the direct
`inhibition of SOD1 will ever affect ALS-
`related survival. In addition, it has been
`suggested that these mice may be more
`susceptible to infections and other non-
`ALS related illnesses and that it is this
`illness rather than the ALS that is alle-
`viated by the experimental
`treatment.
`Consistent with this hypothesis, several of
`the compounds reported as efficacious in
`SOD1G93A mice are broad-spectrum
`antibiotics and general anti-inflammatory
`agents [40].
`
`Publication Bias
`
`Decisions to assess the effect of novel
`treatment strategies in clinical trials are,
`ideally, based on an understanding of all
`publicly reported information from pre-
`clinical
`studies. Systematic review and
`meta-analysis are techniques developed
`for the analysis of data from clinical trials
`and may be helpful in the selection of the
`most promising strategies [16]. However,
`if studies are published selectively on the
`basis of their results, even a meta-analysis
`based on a rigorous systematic review will
`be misleading.
`Th

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket