throbber
NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 1 of 59
`
`MAR MAY
`
`JUN Close
`
`-olllll 13 .....
`201 0 ~
`
`2009
`
`2008
`
`http://wvwv.fda.gov/oashi!patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`48 captures
`17 Aug 00- 15 May 09
`
`ll.S. Food :md Drug AdministralioJJ
`
`THALIDOMIDE:
`POTENTIAL BENEFITS AND RISKS
`
`AN OPEN PUB LIC SCIENTIFIC WORKSHOP
`September 9-10, 1997
`
`N atch~r C onfer~I"I-CQ C9ntsr - National lnstltutes of Health - Bethasda. M ~ryland
`
`TRANSCRIPT
`
`THALIDOMIDE: POTENTIAL BENEFITS AND RISKS
`
`OPEN PUBLIC SCIENTIFIC WORKSHOP
`
`Sponsored By
`
`NATIONAL INSTITUTES OF HEALTH
`
`FOOD AND DRUG ADMINISTRATION
`
`CENTERS FOR DISEASE CONTROL AND PREVENTION
`
`Wednesday, September 10, 1997
`
`Auditorium
`N atcher Conference Center
`National Institutes of Health
`9000 Rockville Pike
`Bethesda, Maryland
`
`CONTENTS
`(with hyperlink to speakers presentations)
`
`RISK MANAGEMENT
`
`Louis A Morris, Ph.D. , Moderator
`Chief, Division of Drug Marketing, Advertising,
`and Communications
`Center for Drug Evaluation and Research, FDA
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 1 of 59
`
`CELGENE EXHIBIT 2024
`Coalition for Affordable Drugs VI LLC (Petitioner) v. Celgene Corporation (Patent Owner)
`Case IPR2015-01102
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 2 of 59
`
`Effective Risk Communication
`Louis A Morris, Ph.D. , Ph.D.
`
`Role of Communication to Influence Behavior
`Martin Fishbein, Ph.D.,
`University ofPennsylvania
`
`Clinical Ethical Considerations in the Use of Thalidomide:
`A Practitioner's Perspective
`Gail J. Povar, M.D., M.P.H., F.AC.P.
`George Washington University School of Medicine
`
`Patients' Rights and Physicians' Responsibilities
`Mark Senak, J.D.
`AIDS Project Los Angeles
`
`Risk Management -- Educational, Advertising,
`and Marketing Efforts: Industry Perspectives
`Bruce A Williams
`Celgene Corporation
`
`Questions
`
`PERSPECTIVES ON PRESENT AND FUTURE NEEDS
`
`Ann Ginsberg, M.D., Moderator
`National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, NIH
`
`Research Perspective
`Gilla Kaplan, Ph.D.
`The Rockefeller University
`
`Thalidomide: Bioethical and Legal Issues -(cid:173)
`Industry's Perspective on Present and Future Needs
`Peter Andrulis
`Andrulis Pharmaceuticals Corporation
`
`Thalidomide Revisited: The Nightmare to Come
`Thomas H. Bleakley, J.D.
`Bleakley & McKeen, P.C.
`
`Legal Perspective
`Frank C. Woodside, Ill, M.D., J.D.
`Dinsmore & Shohl
`
`Questions
`
`OPEN PUBLIC SESSION
`
`Stephen C. Groft, Pharm.D. , Chair
`Director, Office of Rare Diseases, NIH
`
`Iris Long, M.D.
`ACT UP/New York
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 2 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 3 of 59
`
`RESPONSES FROM WORKSHOPS ON RESEARCH
`ADVANCES AND OPPORTUNITIES
`
`Stephen C. Groft, Pharm.D. , Chair
`Pharmacology, Pharmacokinetics, and Teratology
`of Thalidomide and Analogs
`
`J. David Erickson, D.D.S., Ph.D.
`Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
`
`Dermatology
`Mervyn L. Elgart, M.D.
`George Washington University School of Medicine
`
`Immunology /Rheumatology
`Philip Fox, D.D.S.
`National Institute of Dental Research, NIH
`
`Hematology /Oncology
`James Pluda, M.D.
`National Cancer Institute, NIH
`
`Infectious Diseases
`Lawrence Fox, M.D., Ph.D.
`National Institute of Allergy and
`Infectious Diseases, NIH
`
`CLOSING REMARKS
`
`Theresa Toigo, R.Ph., M.B.A.
`Associate Commissioner for Special Health Issues, FDA
`
`Stephen Groft, Pharm.D.
`
`Janet Woodcock, M.D.
`Director, Center for Drug Evaluation
`and Research, FDA
`
`P R 0 C E ED IN G S (8:14a.m.)
`
`DR GROFT: Good morning, everyone. I see you were able to make it through what for Washington this
`summer is an unusual event, a day of rain. So despite the travel and everything else, the problems that go with
`traffic, we do appreciate the rain and need it.
`
`My name is Steve Groft, and myself and Terry Toigo have served as co-chairs of this workshop and had the
`opportunity to put together the program This morning we're ready to tum it over to risk communication, risk
`management. We've asked Lou Morris from the Food and Drug Administration to chair this session, be the
`moderator and put together the panel, which he has done an outstanding job of, so I'll just tum it over to Lou to
`get us started.
`
`I'll have some information later on about the breakout sessions, and then the CME credit for those who are
`filling out the forms. So between the first and second session, we'll put some information up on the overhead.
`
`Lou?
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 3 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 4 of 59
`
`DR. MORRIS: Thank you, Steve.
`
`Good morning, everybody. Congratulations. You made it here. If you walked, you were fine. If you had to
`drive, you were lucky.
`
`This morning we're going to talk about risk management from a number of perspectives. First, we will be
`talking about it from a theoretical perspective in which we'll talk about both perception and behavioral
`influences. We'll then talk about some issues in informed consent and consumer perspectives, and then, lastly,
`we'll hear from Bruce Williams regarding Celgene's plans for a risk management program for their thalidomide
`product. The way I'd like to structure the session is, first, I'm going to introduce everybody in the panel, then
`we'll each take our tum at the podium, and then, finally, we'll have a question and answer period.
`
`So let me start by introducing myself. My name is Lou Morris. I work in the Division of Drug Marketing,
`Advertising, and Communications at FDA, where we work on regulatory issues and patient information issue,
`and I also am scholar in residence at the American University Center for Marketing Policy Research.
`
`On my right is Dr. Martin Fishbein, who for the last five years has been at the Centers for Disease Control,
`where he's worked on behavioral initiatives regarding HIV and AIDS policy. He's currently moving to the
`Annenberg Center, where he'll be an active member of a new public policy unit.
`
`On his right is Dr. Gail Povar. Dr. Povar is clinical professor of medicine at George Washington University,
`where she not only teaches new and innovative courses-- she mentioned she's teaching an Internet course this
`year-- but she actually sees patients. She's one of those few people who have multiple perspectives not only
`from a broad public policy and ethical sense, but also from a sense of patients' perspective.
`
`On her right is Mark Senak, who is with the AIDS Project in Los Angeles. He's the director of public policy
`there. Mark has a long career in AIDS activism He's been in the Gay Men's Health Crisis Center and the AIDS
`Action Council.
`
`Then on his right is Bruce Williams, who is the vice president for marketing and sales at Celgene. Mark
`actually moved Celgene from a research to a commercial corporation, and he's primarily responsible for
`developing there the thalidomide distribution and communications program We'll be very interested in hearing
`from him
`
`So with that, I'm going to start off, set my own timer. Can I have the first slide?
`
`Thalidomide has special meaning for lots of people, and for someone who has worked at FDA, this is one piece
`of the meaning for thalidomide that I think FDA staff has. In the library in the Center for Drugs, there's a
`picture of Dr. Frances Kelsey receiving an award from President Kennedy. That picture, I think every time we
`go into the library, communicates to people at FDA in a very special way.
`
`I know critics of FDA frequently think that we see thalidomide as the reason why we keep drugs off the market.
`At least for me, that's never been my interpretation of this. It does remind us that we have a very important job
`and that what we do has huge implications for individual patients and for the country as a whole. I think that's
`the way we have seen thalidomide historically, and I think that's the way we see thalidomide today. This is
`symbolic for us and it's symbolic because it means an awful lot about defining who we are and what we do. So
`the things we do and say about thalidomide I think have important implications at a national level and on a
`personal level for the people who work at FDA
`
`So with that aside, if we can tum that slide off, I'll move to the high tech stuff. We can try talking about risk
`communication and thalidomide. What I would like to do is present more of kind of a general theory and then
`some specific data that we've collected on thalidomide and risk communication.
`
`First, from a marketing perspective, I think it's clear that we can characterize products in three different ways,
`and we as consumers characterize products in these ways. First, we can characterize a product in terms of its
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 4 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 5 of 59
`
`physical properties. We're taking a capsule of medicine, but it's more. It has more meaning to us than just that.
`
`It also has a functional property. Whatever this drug is, this thalidomide is, it has certain benefits that we
`anticipate, and it also has certain risks that we need to be aware of. For some products, products that have very
`high meaning, high involvement products, not only do we take in and think about what it is that people tell us,
`but we also elaborate on those meanings for these kinds of high involvement products and we integrate this
`information.
`
`There's still a third level of meaning by which we can characterize products, and that is that products have
`symbolic meaning. They mean an awful lot to us. For me, as that previous slide showed, thalidomide has a
`special meaning. I think that ifyou ask people, at least of my age cohort, what it means, they're going to talk
`about what they see. They have a very, very vivid understanding and a recall of certain pictures that they saw.
`If you speak to anyone, they say yes, and they'll see these pictures.
`
`How people use products depends upon several things. It depends upon, first, how they perceive them That
`first part, that perception, depends upon their beliefs about the product itself and their personal use of the
`product. In my presentation, I'm going to focus on this.
`
`But it also depends on beliefs about other things. For example, beliefs about what other people think. It also
`depends on situational factors. Dr. Fishbein will give us a much broader perspective on the behavioral
`outcomes related to people's use of different products.
`
`Let's now turn to the people's perceptions of risk. One of the things we know from research is that knowledge is
`not simply something that's transferred. It's actually constructed. When people process information they only
`retain small bits and pieces. What they retain from a message, their takeaway is going to be fairly limited. One
`of the things we do know is that very vivid images are more likely to be transferred to people's memory.
`
`When they then have to act on those behaviors, that information is integrated with their current context. So
`meaning changes. Memory is very creative because we're only retrieving bits and pieces and then we're
`integrating it with our current situation. There's a lot of differences in terms of how people behave depending
`upon how familiar they are with the product. If it's a product that we're very, very familiar with, we're going to
`have a very standard way of seeing it. If we take an aspirin every day, we know what that aspirin means to us,
`we know what it does, we know when to take it, how to take it, and we know what to expect from it.
`
`However, if we take a new product, a product that we've never taken before -- a new drug, for example -- how
`we behave towards that product is going to be very variable because a lot will depend on the contextual
`situation. So, for example, if we get a headache after we take this unfamiliar product, we may say that new
`product did it. If we get a headache after taking an aspirin, we'll say, well, the aspirin wasn't strong enough, for
`example. We know what to expect in one case. In the other case, we don't know what to expect.
`
`People build mental models of products. They also have situational inputs. What is retrieved when knowledge
`is put to use is a very important issue. One of the things that we've been concerned about in the risk
`communication package for thalidomide is what's going to happen at the point in time when an individual is
`faced with a sexual encounter. How will that patient behave? Part of that's going to be dependent upon what
`they retrieve in memory at that point in time and if those memories are what we're trying to build in the risk
`communication package.
`
`The reason I put up these data, one of the things we want to know is what do people already know about a
`product. We're lucky enough that a couple of weeks ago we started collecting data from another study.
`
`The PowerPoint is not on. Help!
`
`(Laughter.)
`
`DR MORRIS: Let me go on to talk about what it is and I can describe it. We collected some data from a small
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 5 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 6 of 59
`
`sample. This is an ongoing study and we literally just last week compiled the data that we had so far. In this
`study, we asked a very simple question. We gave people certain words. We just asked them to define them,
`what does this word mean. We collected these data from males and females and what I would show you with
`numbers, if we had them, is that there's an incredible age cohort effect if you look at these data, and I can tum
`this around and you can all see it.
`
`(Laughter.)
`
`DR MORRIS: That if you're over age 45, over 50 percent of the people, about two-thirds of the people, can
`correctly define at least partially what the word "thalidomide" means. If you're under 45, it's a total reversal.
`There it is. It's not real clear, but we separated out for genders. It's percent correct, partially correct, incorrect,
`or don't know.
`
`Ifyou look at the age groupings, ifyou look at the 45 split, about two-thirds of people either get it wrong or
`don't know if you're under 45. If you're over 45, two-thirds get it at least partially correct. It's a VANOVA, a
`visual analysis of variance here. You don't even need to do the statistics. It's an incredible cohort effect. We
`separated out for males and females, and we didn't find any difference in genders, but we do find this enormous
`cohort effect in terms of age.
`
`I think my explanation for it is, again, if you ask people over 45 about thalidomide, they just see these very
`vivid images. They retrieve it. Under 45, they just don't have those memories.
`
`So from a risk perception standpoint, one of the things I think we need to think about is how do we build this
`risk profile. We can think of this in three successive levels of building. For a lot of products what we do is
`simply put out a simple warning label. That warning label has a signal word, it tells people what they should
`do, and what negative outcomes should occur. I think if you look at just about any consumer product --over(cid:173)
`the-counter drugs, ladders, lawn mowers, anything like that -- you're going to see this kind of warning message.
`That's the minimal thing we can do. It tells people, "Pay attention to me. Here's what's going to happen to you
`unless you take the following preventive action."
`
`At the next level, I think what we need to do is build very, very vivid memorable inputs. I was very pleased that
`the advisory committee said we need to make sure there is a picture or a video. There needs to be something
`that people retrieve. Another thing about these age cohorts, one of the things that my understanding is from my
`reading, is that if you look at the way people learn in Generation Y, it is very visual. I think that matches well.
`So I think that's an important issue.
`
`At the highest level, I think what we need is a very persuasive argument. We did some research a few years ago
`on pregnant women's use of alcohol. What we found, to our surprise, was that women who continued to drink
`during pregnancy were the ones who were more educated, not less educated. In our analysis what we found was
`that those women believed that they could control the risks by simply moderating the amount of alcohol they
`consumed. When we gave them risk messages, they counterargued them They said, well, that doesn't happen, I
`can control the risks.
`
`Our sense is that for people who are very involved in an issue, they may believe that they can control the risk
`themselves. What we have to do is persuade them, we have to counterargue their other examples and say, no, it
`really is important, for example, that you use two forms of birth control. One form is not enough. We may have
`to think about those kinds of messages.
`
`In terms of our concerns, there are many. One of the things is we're dealing with a very heterogeneous
`audience. It's heterogeneous in terms of literacy, it's heterogeneous in terms of existing knowledge, it's
`heterogenous in any number of factors. We're dealing with a single system How well can we segment, how
`well can we target, and how well can we communicate?
`
`The second concern that I have is that we're dealing with a long-term behavior and that things learned under
`one state of emotions, normal, rational emotions, those behaviors may not be engaged in when people are in a
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 6 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 7 of 59
`
`different emotional state. Our learning doesn't generalize necessarily to those different states, and people may
`have conflicting motivations. I think Dr. Fishbein will talk more and more about how we have to understand all
`the relevant beliefs, not just the ones that we see, but the ones that consumers see as well.
`
`I think with that, I'll leave this, and I'll ask Dr. Fishbein to continue our trek through thalidomide. Thank you.
`
`(Applause.)
`
`DR. FISHBEIN: Good morning. I'd like to begin by thanking Steve Groft and the other organizers for inviting
`me to participate in this very important workshop, and, maybe more importantly, for recognizing the role of the
`behavioral and communication sciences in addressing what's clearly a very delicate and perplexing issue.
`
`I think what we just heard Lou talk about is kind of a traditional way oflooking at the product and information
`about the product and how this can impact on people's behavior, but I think the bottom line, unfortunately or
`fortunately, is that risk management in the context that we're dealing with it is really a question of behavioral
`change and it's a behavioral problem We're asking people to change their behavior.
`
`Yesterday, we heard quite a bit about what needs to be done or recommendations. Many panelists argued
`forcefully for the importance of education, but one of the things I'm going to try to show you today is that
`educating potential thalidomide users or the general public about the dangers of taking thalidomide during
`pregnancy, or simply telling them that what they must do in order to prevent pregnancy, is really unlikely to
`lead to behavioral change.
`
`I was asked to kind of provide an overview ofhow communications can effect behavior and behavioral change.
`It's really more of an overview of what do we know about factors that influence behavior. What I want to do is
`to briefly go over some lessons I think we've learned from behavioral science theory and research. Let me just
`go through a series of five lessons that I think are important.
`
`I think the first thing that we've learned is that we can change behaviors. We can change behaviors that many
`people felt were difficult, if not impossible, to change. As many of you may know, for the last five years I've
`been working in AIDS prevention and we've had to deal with some pretty difficult behaviors there, both sexual
`behavior and drug-taking behavior. There's now I think an abundant amount of evidence that well-designed,
`theoretically-based interventions or messages, or a combination of interventions and messages can, in fact,
`produce significant behavior change.
`
`There was a recent OMAR conference, a consensus development conference, on the effectiveness of behavioral
`interventions to produce behavior change in AIDS prevention that basically concluded that the evidence was
`now there that we can, in fact, produce behavior change. I think that's important to recognize, that even difficult
`behaviors can be changed if we know what we're doing.
`
`The second lesson that we've learned is that information in and of itself can, in fact, produce behavior change.
`We've been told many times that information in and of itself was insufficient. What the behavioral sciences has
`to offer in this arena is really an understanding of the kinds of information that's necessary to produce behavior
`change.
`
`As I mentioned earlier, knowledge about a disease and how it's spread, or knowledge about a drug and its
`potential dangers, in and of itself is insufficient to produce behavior change. Simply telling somebody that they
`must use condoms all the time if they're a male, or that they should use a minimum of at least two contraceptive
`techniques if they're a female, isn't enough to produce behavior change. Again, what the behavioral sciences
`and what the behavioral science theory has to offer is an understanding of the kinds of information that are
`necessary to provide to the public or to a person in order to get them to change their behavior.
`
`The third lesson that we've learned is that the most effective interventions will be those directed at specific
`behaviors, not at behavioral categories or goals. What I mean by that, let me use a simple example. Ifl try to
`convince people that they should lose weight, weight loss isn't a behavior, it's an outcome of performing a
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 7 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 8 of 59
`
`behavior. There are many different factors that are going to influence whether or not a person loses weight.
`Two people who follow exactly the same regime may wind up one of them losing a lot of weight, and the other
`not, because of metabolic factors. So simply trying to get somebody to increase their intentions to lose weight is
`no guarantee that they'll lose it.
`
`In the same way, preventing pregnancy is not a behavior. It's the outcome of a number of behaviors. So trying
`to convince people that they should prevent pregnancy isn't necessarily going to lead to some positive outcome.
`
`Well, if we're not going to try to get people to lose weight, what should we get them to do? Most people say
`you could have them diet or exercise. But dieting and exercising aren't behaviors either. They're categories of
`behavior. What I mean by dieting isn't necessarily what you mean by dieting, and what I mean by exercise isn't
`necessarily what you mean by exercise, so when we tell people that they should diet or exercise, we're not
`really giving them a very clear message.
`
`So "practice contraception" doesn't give you a very clear message. We're going to need to say something more
`specific. The more we get down to particular behaviors -- always use a condom every time you have vaginal
`intercourse with your spouse or with some other person --then we may start having some impact on behavior.
`
`Intentions to perform specific behaviors will tend to influence the likelihood that those behaviors will be
`performed. Intentions to reach goals or to engage in behavioral categories may or may not have any impact on
`specific behaviors. So, again, one of the lessons that we've really learned is that when we're developing
`interventions or messages, we've got to identify a particular behavior or set of behaviors that we want changed,
`and then worry about what kinds of information do we have to provide in order to produce that behavioral
`change.
`
`The next lesson we've learned, and perhaps the most important one, is despite the fact that behavior is very
`complex, and all of us can think oflots and lots and lots of variables that may influence behavior, there's a
`general consensus that's starting to be reached, at least in the behavioral sciences, that there really are only a
`limited number of variables that need to be considered in attempts to influence or maintain behaviors. Let me
`just try to briefly and quickly go through what those variables are.
`
`As I mentioned before, one of the things that we've found out is probably the best predictor of what people are
`going to do in the future is what they've done in the past, but we can't change past behavior.
`
`The next best single predictor, and sometimes it's even better than past behavior, are people's intentions or
`commitment to engage in a behavior. If someone commits or really says it's very likely that I'm going to do
`this, the chances are pretty good that they will in fact do it. It's not a perfect predictor, but it's there.
`
`But in order to perform a behavior you have to have necessary skills. You have to have the ability to actually
`carry it out. One of the things in this new area that many of you will be getting into is that if in fact you're
`recommending to men that they should consistently and correctly use condoms all the time, there is an ability
`factor.
`
`People don't always use condoms correctly. In fact, some 75 percent of condom users have used condoms
`incorrectly at least once, and by incorrectly, it has to do with when they put it on, when they take it off, how
`they put it on, whether they remember to leave a reservoir at the tip. There is a whole set of abilities related to
`proper condom use, as opposed to consistent condom use. All too often we neglect the skills.
`
`Another factor that we need to take into account is, is there anything out there in the environment that's actually
`preventing this behavior from occurring? Are there environmental constraints? I draw on AIDS issues because
`that's where I've been working for the last five years, but clearly, if condoms aren't available, you can't use
`them. It doesn't matter how strong your intentions are and how well you know about how to put them on. If
`they're not out there, if you don't have access to certain services, facilities, and skills, you can't carry out
`intentions that involve the utilization of those skills.
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 8 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 9 of 59
`
`These are three important variables. The other three are more psychosocial, I suppose, and they have to do with
`attitudes. These are attitudes towards performing a behavior. Lou is talking about the attitudes or perceptions
`toward the product itself, but these have to do with attitudes towards using the product.
`
`In terms of risk management, it's attitudes towards carrying out or conducting the behavior that you want
`people to engage in. So the feeling that it's good or bad to always use condoms every time I have vaginal sex
`with my main partner, or to always use two contraceptive methods when I'm engaging in vaginal sex.
`
`The norms have to do with perceptions about what others think one should or shouldn't do. One of the factors
`that is going to influence the likelihood that I engage in a behavior is my perception of what others expect me
`to do as well as my perception of what others are doing. I'll come back to this.
`
`Then the final variable that seems to be very important in influencing behavior and behavior change is the
`notion of self-agency or self-efficacy. People are unlikely to engage in behaviors that they don't believe they
`really can do. So it's a perception that I have the necessary skills and abilities. Unlike the skills and abilities at
`the top, which really have to do with whether you know what to do, this has to do with your perception. If you
`don't perceive that you're capable of carrying out a certain behavior, you're very unlikely to try to carry out that
`behavior. If you try to do something and it doesn't succeed, you're very likely to just give up. On the other hand,
`if you believe you can do something, even if you fail the first time, you may continue to persist.
`
`Let me try to make some sense. I've also talked about the fact that attitudes, this feeling that performing this
`behavior is good or bad, are based on underlying behavior or beliefs. That is, my beliefs that if I perform this
`behavior, certain outcomes will occur. The more I believe good outcomes will occur, the less I believe bad
`outcomes will occur, the more I think performing this behavior is a good thing.
`
`For norms, it's beliefs about what specific others think I should or shouldn't do, or what specific others are
`doing. Self-efficacy has to do with my perception of barriers to carrying out a behavior.
`
`Let me try to just summarize this and talk about a general model of behavior and behavior change. What the
`model is suggesting is that there really are three variables that are critical in determining whether or not a
`person will perform some specific behavior: skills, the intention or commitment to do it, and environmental
`constraints. What this is really saying is given that a person has the intention to perform some behavior, they
`are likely to perform it as long as they have the skills available and there are no environmental constraints.
`
`Clearly, what this suggests is that the kinds of messages or interventions that are going to be necessary to
`produce behavior change-- to get women to always use contraceptives, to get men to always use condoms-(cid:173)
`are going to be very different if they haven't yet formed an intention to do this, if they haven't made a
`commitment to engage in this behavior, than if they've made the commitment but either don't have the skills or
`there's something out there preventing them from doing it. What this is really saying is we need very different
`interventions, and probably very different messages, depending upon where people are in terms of their
`commitment to engage in some course of action.
`
`If they haven't made a commitment, then the likelihood of forming or making this commitment is going to
`depend on one of three factors: their attitudes, their perception of the norms, and their self-efficacy, which I
`described. One of the things that is important to recognize is that the relative importance of these three factors
`will vary as a function of both the behavior you're considering and the population you're considering. That is,
`for a given behavior, attitudes may be more important than norms, while for some other behavior self-efficacy
`may be what really influences intention.
`
`So we need to figure out which of these is the predominating factor. For the same behavior, it may be
`attitudinally influenced in one population, but normatively influenced in another. So again, we need to do our
`homework. You just don't put out a message and expect it to have an effect on behavior change. What we need
`to do is figure out whether the behavior we want, in fact, people intend to do it or not. If they don't intend to do
`it, then is their intention due to their attitudes, their norms, or their self-efficacy?
`
`http:/ /replay .web.archive.org/20090513023134/http:/ /www.fda.gov/oashi/patrep/nih91 O.html
`
`
`
`
`
`Page 9 of 59
`
`

`
`NIH Thalidomide Meeting, 9/10/97
`
`Page 10 of 59
`
`But saying I want to make somebody more favorable to performing a behavior isn't enough. We have to know
`where that attitude comes from, and that's talking about underlying beliefs and their valuative aspects, or
`normative beliefs and motivations to comply with the norms, and these efficacy beliefs about barriers. That is,
`is it more difficult to do it under some circumstances than others?
`
`I'm going through this very quickly because I'm trying to give you an overview, but what I hope I'm getting
`across is that behavior change is complex. Designing effective behavior change messages is no easier than
`developing vaccines. You don't just throw together a message, just as you don't throw together a vaccine or
`some other drug. You need to test it, you need to evaluate it, you need to quantify it, and you n

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket