throbber
0 145-6008/93/ 1704-0758$3.00/0
`ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL A N D EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
`
`Vol. 17, No. 4
`July/August 1993
`
`Influence of Socially Desirable Responding in a Study of
`Stress and Substance Abuse
`
`John W. Welte and Marcia Russell
`
`Socially desirable responding is the reluctance to admit unpopular
`beliefs or behavior in order to avoid making a negative impression.
`It poses a problem for researchers who rely on self-report of heavy
`drinking and drug use. The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale
`(SDS), which measures socially desirable responding, was adminis-
`tered to 1933 respondents in a general population survey. The
`relationships between SDS and various self-report measures were
`examined. SDS was not correlated with gender or race; it increased
`with greater age and decreased with higher socioeconomic status.
`With age and socioeconomic status controlled, SDS had a moderate
`negative relationship with alcohol and drug use, and a strong nega-
`tive relationship with variables reflecting the expression of anger.
`Correlations between drinking/drug use and such variables as age,
`marital conflict, and stressful life events were not substantially
`changed by controlling for SDS. It was concluded that social desir-
`ability response bias probably results in underestimates of rates of
`heavy drinking and drug use, but does not compromise the study of
`predictors of heavy drinking or drug use.
`Key Words: Marlowe Crowne, Social Desirability, Alcohol, Drug
`Abuse.
`
`S for respondents to avoid admitting unpopular actions
`
`OCIALLY DESIRABLE responding is the tendency
`
`or beliefs in order to favorably impress an interviewer.
`The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (SDS)’
`was developed to measure socially desirable responding.
`It contains 33 categorically strong statements that are
`almost never (or almost always) true, but which tempt
`subjects to respond falsely in order to place themselves in
`a positive light. Examples are: “I have never been irked
`when people expressed ideas very different from my own,”
`and “There have been occasions when I took advantage
`of someone.” The developmental work of Marlowe and
`Crowne showed the SDS to have good internal consistency
`reliability (i.e., the items tended to be positively correlated
`with each other) and to be unrelated to any mental illness.
`The absolute nature of the statements means that few, if
`any, could give socially desirable answers with complete
`honesty. Therefore, the SDS was originally interpreted as
`a measure of bias or contamination in self-report data-
`bias caused by subjects’ lying to make a good impression.
`
`From the Research Institute on Addictions, 1021 Main Street, Buffalo,’
`New York.
`Receivedfor publication January 8, 1992; accepted January 28, 1993
`This study was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse
`and Alcoholism Grant ROI AA05702 awarded to M.R. This study was
`conducted at the Research Institute on Addictions.
`Reprint requests: John W. Welte, Ph.D.. Research Institute on Addic-
`tions, 1021 Main Street, Buffalo, NY 14203.
`Copyright 0 I993 by The Research Society on Alcoholism.
`750
`
`In previous studies of socially desirable responding it
`was reported that SDS scores increased with age and
`decreased with education, but were not correlated with
`Racial differences in SDS scores were observed by
`some researcher~,~ but not all.* Not surprisingly, socially
`desirable responding has been found to have a negative
`relationship with self-report of undesirable behavior.
`Henly and Winters4 found a strong negative correlation
`with a drug problems scale in a sample of adolescent drug
`abusers. Bradburn et a1.2 found a negative correlation of
`approximately the same magnitude between SDS and
`heavy drinking and SDS and marijuana use.
`The possibility that people lie about socially undesirable
`behavior is of concern to researchers who must evaluate
`the validity of self-reports of such socially undesirable
`behaviors as heavy drinking and drug use. However, the
`correlation between SDS and self-report of socially unde-
`sirable behavior is not necessarily due to contamination.
`High SDS scores could reflect the personality of individ-
`uals highly concerned about societal approval. Such indi-
`viduals might be expected to both represent themselves as
`having socially acceptable behavior and behave in socially
`approved ways.
`Evidence for the latter view has been put forth by
`Bradburn et a1.2 They interpreted the positive relationship
`between age and SDS as a cohort effect caused by older
`people having been brought up to think in categorical
`terms about behavior, arguing that older respondents were
`not portraying themselves as virtuous to impress the in-
`terviewer but that they truly thought of themselves as, for
`example, always being helpful. It was also hypothesized
`that if socially desirable lying were operating, the correla-
`tion between the SDS and marijuana use would be
`stronger than that between the SDS and alcohol, reflecting
`the fact that marijuana is illegal and more strongly pros-
`cribed than drinking.
`The researcher’s dilemma is to determine “whether the
`differences between persons with low and high Marlowe-
`Crowne scores are part of the real variance in (the) data
`or part of the error variance” (Bradburn et al., p. 88).2 In
`the latter case, it would be necessary to adjust socially
`sensi.tive self-report data from subjects with high SDS
`scores in order to discern the true nature of the relation-
`ships under study; in the former, adjusting for SDS scores
`might obscure relationships of interest.
`In this study we will investigate the effect of socially
`desirable responding on self-reports of alcohol and drug
`Alcohol Clin Exp Res. Vol 17, No 4, 1993: pp 758-761
`
`CFAD VI 1032-0001
`
`

`

`SoClAuY DESIRABLE RESPONDING IN STRESS AND SUBSTANCE ABUSE
`use in a general population survey. The contamination
`theory would predict that the SDS would be most highly
`correlated with those variables that involve the most so-
`cially damaging admissions. We will also investigate the
`effect of controlling statistically for SDS on relationships
`between measures of substance abuse and other variables
`of interest.
`
`METHODS
`
`h p l e
`Data for the present analyses came from a representative adult house-
`hold survey of stress, alcohol use, and hypertension conducted in Erie
`County, New York (n = 1933). Respondents were identified using a
`stratified, three-stage probability sampling procedure designed to yield
`approximately equal numbers of African Americans and all others at
`thne levels of education (less than high school, high school, at least some
`college). The overall sample completion rate was 78.3%, with the major-
`ity (84.5%) of noncompletions due to refusals. The present analyses were
`conducted without weighting.
`The sample was 42% White, 52% African American, and 6% others.
`It was 62% female, and ranged in age from 19 to 9 1 years.
`
`Racedures
`Data were collected by a corps of 27 interviewers in the summer and
`MI of 1986. Interviewers received 5 days of intensive training on general
`aad survey-specific interviewing techniques and 3 days of training on
`physical measurement, including blood pressure measurement. Inter-
`views were conducted in respondents’ homes using a highly structured
`interview schedule that included diet, smoking, and physical activity, as
`well as medical history. The entire procedure took about 90 min;
`nspondents were paid $25.
`
`Measures
`*Social desirabilify was measured using a 10-item version of the
`Mdowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale developed by Strahan and
`Gerbasi? It was demonstrated to have correlations of 0.8 or better with
`tbG low form in various population^.^ To adjust for missing data, the
`~ u m k r of items answered in a socially desirable manner was divided by
`the IIUmbcr of items completed. Thus, SDS scores ranged from 0 to 1 .O.
`showed an Cronbach’s of 0.62, which might be
`Reliability anal*
`characterized as acceptable although not good. Alphas of 0.6 or better
`WCR observed among race- and sex-specific subgroups (data available
`from the authors).
`*Heavy drinking was defined as the number of days in the last 30 that
`five or more drinks were drunk.
`*Drug use was defined as the number of different types of illegal drugs
`used in a respondent’s life.
`*Job prestige rankings were derived from respondents’ reports of their
`job titles and duties using Hollingshead’s6 categories for occupational
`rank.
`*Depression was assessed using a 20-item scale developed by the
`Center for Epidemiological Studies.’
`*Drinking fo cope was estimated using a 5-item scale developed by
`Pobch and Onis* to measure the tendency to use alcohol to relieve
`dis$eJs.
`*Negative lfi? events was used to assess the extent to which the
`nspondent was distressed or upset by events in his/her life. Our measure
`was derived from the life events measure developed by Dohrenwend et
`d.9
`*Sel/.esteem, respondent’s perception of self-worth, was assessed using
`RoSenberg’~’~ measure of self-esteem.
`*Mastery, the extent to which the respondent feels in control of his
`or ber life, was based on Pearlin et al.’s” measure.
`
`7 59
`
`*Socia/ competence was assessed using a measure culled from the
`Social Avoidance and Distress Scale developed by Watson and Friend.”
`This scale reflects ease in social situations.
`.Anger-in/anger-out/anger-reflect measure styles of dealing with an-
`ger. They were assessed using anger-in (suppressing it and “sulking”) and
`anger-out (letting it out) scales developed by Spielberger et al.l3 and an
`anger-reflect (keep cool and think) scale developed by Harburg and
`Gleiberman. l 4
`‘Experience qf anger is a measure developed by Siegel15 to assess the
`frequency, duration, and strength of anger.
`*Marital conflict was estimated using a measure based on the work of
`Kessler. l 6
`
`RESULTS
`Bivariate analyses were done that examined the rela-
`tionship between SDS scores and several important de-
`mographic characteristics. SDS scores were not correlated
`with sex or race. SDS increased markedly with age, how-
`ever. Respondents aged 18-29 answered 5 l % of the items
`in the socially desirable direction, whereas the correspond-
`ing figure for those over 59 was 70%. SDS was negatively
`correlated with both education and income. Higher socio-
`economic status respondents answered fewer items in the
`socially desirable direction.
`Bivariate relationships between SDS and various meas-
`ures from our stress model (listed in “Measures”) were
`examined. Correlation coefficients were used, both Pear-
`son’s correlations ( r ) and partial correlations adjusted for
`age, income and education. These control variables were
`used to ensure against spurious correlations; for example,
`a negative correlation between SDS and substance abuse
`created solely because older respondents have both high
`SDS and low substance abuse. A comparison of the un-
`adjusted and adjusted correlations showed that holding
`age, education, and income constant generally lowered
`the absolute value of the correlation slightly, but did not
`change the basic pattern.
`The SDS was negatively associated with substance use,
`as expected. The magnitude of the correlation increased
`as level of social disapproval likely to be associated with
`the use increased. For cigarette smoking the association
`was not statistically significant, and for alcohol-related
`variables the magnitude of the correlations was in the
`modest -0.09 to -0.17 range. However, the correlation
`with number of illegal drugs used in the respondent’s
`lifetime was considerably higher at -0.27. Desirable per-
`sonality traits such as self-esteem, mastery, and social
`competence tended to be positively associated with socially
`desirable responding, whereas the experience of anger, a
`less desirable trait, was negatively associated. Sources of
`stress such as negative life events, depression, and marital
`conflict were negatively associated with the SDS. The SDS
`was positively associated with active coping styles, prob-
`lem-solving and anger-reflect, and negatively associated
`with avoidant styles of coping, anger-in, and anger-out.
`As mentioned earlier, adjusting for the effect of age, edu-
`cation, family income, and job prestige had no influence
`
`CFAD VI 1032-0002
`
`

`

`760
`
`WELTE AND RUSSELL
`
`on the direction of the relationships and little influence
`on the strength of the observed associations.
`The relationship of socially desirable responding to self-
`report of substance abuse is examined in greater detail in
`Table 1, in which the unadjusted and adjusted mean days
`of heavy drinking/month and lifetime use of illegal drug
`types are summarized for 10 levels of the SDS. Displayed
`in this manner, the relationships seem much stronger. For
`example, respondents in the lowest tenth of SDS reported
`an average of 0.96 different types of illegal drugs, whereas
`those in the top tenth report an average of only 0.02. As
`in the bivariate analyses mentioned earlier, adjusting for
`the effect of age, education, family income, and job pres-
`tige attenuated the relationship between substance abuse
`and SDS somewhat, but it was still strong. Therefore,
`confounding by age and socioeconomic status does not
`appear to account for the correlations between the SDS
`and alcohol/drug use.
`The possibility that the correlations observed between
`SDS and self-report of alcohol/drug use indicate a contam-
`ination effect cannot be ruled out. Therefore, we must be
`concerned about the impact of socially desirable respond-
`ing on the type of relational analyses often performed with
`such data. Table 2 shows the correlations of five variables
`with heavy drinking and illegal drug use. These five were
`selected a priori because their correlations with substance
`abuse have theoretical interest and are commonly studied.
`The correlations with SDS scores partialled out are also
`shown. None were changed significantly.
`Table 3 illustrates more directly the point made in Table
`2. For example, a marked decline in drug use with increas-
`
`Social
`desirability
`(deciles)
`
`Table 1. Heavy Drinking and Illegal Drug Use by Level of Social Desirability
`Frequency of heavy
`drinking (5+ drinks, mean
`daysponth)
`Unadjusted
`Adjusted'
`Unadjusted
`3.3
`3.0
`0.96
`1
`0.83
`2.6
`2.2
`2
`3
`1.6
`1.8
`0.78
`2.5
`4
`0.54
`2.7
`2.3
`5
`0.56
`2.7
`1.9
`6
`0.37
`1.9
`7
`1.3
`0.29
`1.5
`1 .I
`0.7
`8
`0.23
`0.9
`9
`0.23
`0.9
`1 .o
`10
`0.02
`0.6
`' Adjusted for age, education, family income, and job prestige.
`
`(mean no.
`''legal drug
`used in lifetime)
`
`Adjusted'
`0.78
`0.81
`0.64
`0.60
`0.60
`0.41
`0.44
`0.40
`0.39
`0.24
`
`Table 2. Influence Of Social Desirability on the Relationship of Heavy Drinking
`and Drug Use with Selected Variables (correlation coefficients)
`
`mean days/month)
`
`Unadjusted
`
`Adjusted
`for SDS
`
`Unadjusted
`
`Adjusted
`for SDS
`
`-0.05
`0.20'
`0.01
`0.30'
`0.05
`
`-0.02
`0.19'
`0.01
`0.29'
`0.03
`
`-0.44'
`0.16.
`0.07
`0.15'
`0.18'
`
`~~
`
`-0.39'
`0.15'
`0.06
`0.11'
`0.15'
`
`Age
`Cigarettes/day
`Negative life events
`Drinking to cope
`Marital conflict
`' p < 0.01.
`
`Table 3. Heavy Drinking and Illegal Drug Use Stratified by Social Desirability,
`Age, and Drinking to Cope
`Social desirability
`Medium
`
`~~
`
`High
`
`Low
`
`Frequency of heavy drinking.
`Age
`18-30
`31-50
`50+
`Drinking to cope
`Low
`Medium
`High
`Illegal drug uset
`Age
`18-30
`31 -50
`50+
`Drinking to cope
`Low
`Medium
`High
`Days drink 5+/month
`0
`1-5
`6+
`
`2.4
`1.9
`0.9
`
`0.54
`1.01
`4.13
`
`1.1
`0.8
`0.1 1
`
`0.51
`0.77
`1.18
`
`0.62
`1.29
`1.31
`
`2.6
`2.3
`1.7
`
`0.29
`2.26
`4.99
`
`0.8
`0.5
`0.08
`
`0.26
`0.64
`0.68
`
`0.34
`0.86
`0.98
`
`1.3
`1.3
`0.63
`
`0.24
`0.86
`3.46
`
`0.6
`0.4
`0.02
`
`0.20
`0.36
`0.34
`
`0.18
`0.62
`0.62
`
`* Cell entries are mean dayslmonth drank 5 or more drinks.
`t Cell entries are mean number of drug types used in lifetime.
`
`ing age was observed at each level of SDS, although the
`mean number of drugs reported was much lower for high
`SDS respondents. Put another way, the conclusion about
`the relationship between age and drug use would be the
`same whether the sample was composed solely of respond-
`ents with low SDS scores or solely of respondents with
`high SDS scores. This also holds true for the relationship
`between drinking to cope and substance abuse and that
`between heavy drinking and drug use.
`
`DISCUSSION
`These analyses indicated that socially desirable respond-
`ing was significantly correlated with self-reported sub-
`stance use and measures from our stress model, but that
`it did not appreciably influence correlational analyses of
`substance abuse with other factors of,interest. These results
`offer some hope that high SDS might not be just a contam-
`ination factor, but reflects a person who both answers and
`acts in a socially desirable manner. The strongest correla-
`tions with the SDS were variables that involve anger. The
`highest correlation was with anger-out, a high score on
`which means that the respondent admitted to doing things
`such as slamming doors and making sarcastic remarks.
`The correlation with depression was much lower, even
`though the depression measure includes items such as "I
`thought my life had been a failure." To the extent that
`most people would rather admit to sarcasm than to their
`life being a failure, the contamination view would predict
`that the SDS would be more highly correlated with depres-
`sion than with anger-out. The result observed is clearly
`not consistent with the contamination hypothesis. Anger-
`out is directly related to the presentation of oneself in a
`
`CFAD VI 1032-0003
`
`

`

`SOCIALLY DESIRABLE RESPONDING IN STRESS AND SUBSTANCE ABUSE
`
`76 I
`
`social situation. and the visible display of petulance may
`be disturbing to people who actually behave in a socially
`approved manner.
`Also encouraging is the observation that, even assuming
`the contamination view to be correct. many relational
`analyses do not seem to be affected. For example, we have
`demonstrated that the tendency for older persons to report
`less drug use is still strong after controlling for SDS,
`strengthening our belief that they really use fewer drugs
`than young adults. However, to the extent that socially
`desirable responding reflects contamination of the data.
`point estimates of behavior such as drug use will be greatly
`affected, even if the conclusions drawn from relational
`analyses don’t change. A glance at Table 1 shows that self-
`reported drug use and heavy drinking are much lower for
`those with high SDS scores. These results are in fact
`consistent with the conventional wisdom about drinking
`surveys. From comparisons with excise tax data, we know
`that drinking surveys underestimate average alcohol con-
`sumption,” although correlations with alcohol consump-
`tion are widely assumed to be valid.
`
`REFERENCES
`I. Crowne DP. Marlowe D: A new scale of social desirability
`independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol 24:349-354, 1960
`2. Bradburn NM. Sudman S. Blair E. Locander W. Miles C. Singer
`E, Stocking C: Reinterpreting the Marlowe-Crowne Scale. in Bradburn
`NM, Sudman S (eds): Improving Intenleu Method and Questionnaire
`Design. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass Publishers. 198 I. pp 85-106
`3. Massen D. Hornstra RK, Anderson PB: Influence of social
`desirability on symptom and mood reporting in a community sample. J
`Consult Clin Psychol 43:488-452. 1975
`
`4. Henly G A ? Winters KC: Development of problem severity scales
`for the assessment of adolescent alcohol and drug abuse. Int J Addict
`23:05-85. 1988
`5. Strahan R. Gerbasi KC: Short. homogeneous versions of the
`Marlowe-Crowne social desirabllit> scale. .I CIin Psbchol ?8:191-193.
`1972
`6. Hollinghhead AH: Four factor index ofsoc~al status. Department
`of Sociology. Yale Ilniversity, unpublished manuscript. 1975
`7. Radloff’ LS: The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for
`rescarch in the general population. Appl Psychol Measurement 1385-
`401. 1977
`8. Polich JM. Orvis BR: Alcohol problems: Patterns and prevalence
`in the U.S. Air Force (Rept. no. R-2308-AF). Santa Monica. CA. Rand
`Corporation, 1979
`9. Dohrenwend BS. Krasnoff‘ L, Askenas) AR. Dohrenwend BP:
`Exemplification o f a method for scaling life events: The PER1 Life Events
`Scale. J Health Soc Behav 19:2Oj-229. 1978
`10. Roscnberg M: Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton.
`NJ. Princeton llnivcrsity Press. 1Y65
`11. Pearlin LI. Menaghan EG, Lieberman MA. Mullan JT: The
`stress process. J Health Soc Behav 12337-356, 1981
`11. Watson D. Friend R: Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety.
`J Consult Clin Psycho1 33:448-457, I969
`13. Spielbcrger CD. Johnson Efj. Russell SF, Crane RJ. Jacobs GA,
`Worden TJ: The experience and expression of anger: Construction and
`validation of an anger expression scale, in Chesney MA, Rosenman RH
`(eds): Anger and Hostilit? in Cardiovascular and Behavioral Disorders.
`Washington, DC, Hemisphere. 1985. pp 5-30
`14. Harburg E. Gleiberman I-: Unpubliahcd questionnaire. Ann
`Arbor, MI, Institute for Social Rcsearch. Liniversity of Michigan. 1986
`15. Siege1 JM: The measurement of anger as a multidimensional
`construct. in Chesney MA, Roscnman R11 (cds): Anger and Hostility in
`Cardiovascular and Behavioral Disorders. Washington. DC. Hemisphere.
`1985. pp 59-81
`16. Kcssler RC: Personal communication. 1986
`17. Midanik L: The validity of self-reported alcohol consumption
`and alcohol problems: A literature review. Br J Addict 77:357-382, 1982
`
`CFAD VI 1032-0004
`
`

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket