throbber
GTL 1015
`IPR of U.S. Patent 6,636,591
`
`

`
`Editor-in-Chief
`Professor H. J. EYSENCK, Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Psychology,
`De Crespigny Park Road, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AZ
`
`Assistant Editor
`
`Dr. S. RACHMAN,.‘InStitute of Psychiatry, Department of Psychology,
`De Crespigny Park Road, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AZ
`
`Editorial Department
`
`_Dr. I. MARTIN, London
`Dr. T. AYLLON, Atlanta, Georgia
`Dr. J. S. MARZILLIER, Birmingham
`Professor A. BANDURA, Stanford. Calif.
`Dr. ANDREW MATHEWS, Oxford
`Professor J. T. BARENDREGT, Amsterdam
`Professor 0. H. MOWRER, Urbana, Ill
`Professor Dr. J. B. BERGOLD, Berlin
`Professor D. Mi'JLLER-I-IEGEMANN, Berlin
`‘Professor W. HORSLEY GANTT, Baltimore, Md.
`Professor P. PICHOT, Paris
`Dr. Jose GERMAIN, Madrid
`Professor J. WOLPE, Philadelphia, Pa.
`Professor H- GWYNNE JONES, Leeds
`Professor A. J. YATES, Nedlands, Western Australia
`Professor ANDRZEJ JUS, Warsaw
`])r_ w_ YULE, London
`Professor 0. R. LINDSLEY, Kansas City, Kansas
`Book Reviews D. HEMSLEY, London
`
`.
`
`
`
`Published bi-monthly
`
`Publishing and Advertising 0/flees
`
`Journals Dept., Pergamon Press Limited, Headington Hill Hall, Oxford, OX3 OBW, England;
`Pergamon Press, Inc., Maxwell House, Fairview Park, Elmsford, New York 10523, U.S.A.
`
`Annual Subscription Rates 1977
`For libraries, university departments, government
`laboratories,
`industrial and all other multiple-reader
`institutions US $56 (including postage and insurance).
`Specially reduced rate to individuals: In the interests of maximizing the dissemination of the research results
`published in this important
`international
`journal we have established a two-tier price structure. Any .
`individual whose institution takes out a library subscription may purchase a second or additional subscriptions
`for personal use at the much reduced rate of U.S. $30.00 per annum. All subscription enquiries should
`be addressed to the Subscriptions Fulfillment Manager, Pergamon Press Ltd., Headington Hill Hall, Oxford
`OX3 OBW, England.
`,
`
`Mieroform Subscriptions and Back Issues
`Back issues of all previously published volumes are available in the regular editions and on microfilm and
`microfiche. Current subscriptions are available on microfiche simultaneously with the paper edition and on
`microfilm on completion.of the annual index at the end of the subscription year.
`No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means: electronic,
`°l°°'-"°5ta¢i°» magnetic tape, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without permission in writing from the publishers.
`
`Copyright © 1976, Pergamon Press
`
`_ P
`
`ERGAMON PRESS
`
`
`
`I-IEADINGTON HILL HALL
`OXFORD, OX3 OBW
`
`.
`
`~
`
`MAXWELL. HOUSE
`FAIRVIEW PARK, ELMSFORD, N.Y. 10523
`
`

`
`Behav. Res. & Therapy, 1976, Vol. 14. pp. 323-331. Pergamon Press. Printed in Great Britain.
`
`This material may be protected by Copyright law (Title 17 U.S. Code)
`
`RAPID EDUCATIONAL REHABILITATION
`
`FOR PRISON INMATES*
`
`HENRY JAMES KANDEL and TEODORO AYLLONT
`
`Psychology Department. Georgia State University. Atlanta, GA 30303. U.S.A.
`and
`
`MICHAEL D. ROBERTS
`
`Community Mental Health Center of Palm’Beach, Florida, U.S.A.
`
`(Received 21 October 1975)
`
`Summary~One of the major goals of prisons has been the educational rehabilitation of inmates.
`Studies have shown that priedelinquents as well as delinquents could be motivated to achieve
`in academic areas through the use of an incentive system. The present study attempted to
`extend the behavioral yield of such a system with prison inmates. The academic performance
`v of two inmates in math and English were compared when two levels of incentives were made
`contingent on that performance. The results showed that
`the two inmates passed academic
`tests as much as nine times faster-under an enriched schedule of incentives than under a standard
`one. This increase in rate of performance was translated into actual academic achievement
`as one of the inmates passed 9th through 12th grade algebra in only 14 days. A standardized
`test confirmed this academic advancement for both inmates. These high rates of performance
`and academic achievement are even more dramatic in view of the fact that five months prior
`to this procedure, both inmates had been tested as being below average in intelligence.
`
`In recent years, correctional programs have committed themselves to a philosophy of
`rehabilitation rather than punishment. This philosophy is based on the reasoning that
`in many cases people break the law because they did not have the skills necessary
`to adapt to civilian life. One of the major programs in rehabilitation has been the
`academic training of inmates. Although this objective appears to be easy enough to
`meet on paper, in practice it is a difficult task to motivate inmates to participate and
`achieve in academic programs.
`A
`Within the past seven years,
`there has been empirical evidence demonstrating that
`there is a procedure which will motivate predelinquents (Phillips, 1968), delinquents
`(Cohen, Filipczak and Bis, 1970), and adult offenders (Milan, Wood, Williams, Rogers,
`Hampton and McKee, 1974) to engage in academic training. The major motivational
`procedure is a reinforcement system in which a standard number of points are adminis—
`tered to offenders who perform academically. These points can in turn be exchanged
`for a wide variety of rewards and privileges.
`Probably the most ambitious study in the area of academic training of delinquents
`was carried out by‘ Cohen et al.
`in a reformatory school
`(1970).
`In this study, the
`authors investigated the effects of a reinforcement system on the number of academic
`units passed by approximately twenty delinquents (passing was defined as 90 per cent
`accuracy). In addition to receiving points for passing tests, the delinquents could also
`lose points if they fell below a weekly average of 90 per cent on all final exams. The
`academic areas investigated were language, reading, math and spelling skills. The pro-
`gram showed that the average improvement for the twenty delinquents ranged from
`a 0.6 grade level improvement in language to an average increase in spelling of 1.3
`grade levels. The effects of the reinforcement program were measured only on a pre
`and post basiswithout the benefit of day—to-day measures or a control group.
`
`*This research was supported in part by the Georgia Department of Offender Rehabilitation, Grant No.
`433, and by the Georgia Bureau of State Planning and Community Affairs, Grant No. 72E-0006.
`TReprints can be obtained from T. Ayllon, Psychology Dept., Georgia State University, Atlanta GA 30303,
`U.S.A.
`
`/
`
`323
`
`

`
`earn rewards. The results showed that academic performance increased above -baseline
`levels regardless of whether the inmate’s contract was managed by the experimenter
`or self-managed.
`-
`l
`-
`'
`.
`Using different criteria than passing tests, Milan et al. (1974) showed that reinforce-
`ment procedures could be used to improve prison inmates’ participation in an academic
`program and also increase the amount of time each inmate devoted to study.
`These studies, taken as a whole, show that the presence or absence of reinforcement
`affects inmates’ participation in academic programs and in some instances actual aca-
`demic performance.
`.
`Recently, the effect of magnitude of reinforcement on rates of academic performance
`in school children has been studied. For example, Rickard, Melvin, Creel and Creel
`(1973) explored the use of bonus tokens upon the academic productivity of a group
`of emotionally disturbed boys and girls in a remedial classroom. Bonus tokens were‘
`administered to the students if they had a mean ‘three-day frame count (number of
`frames in a programmed English text) above the median of their best previous three
`days. When the bonus contingency was in effect, the number of frames passed for the
`group increased by approximately 200 per cent over the period when only a standard
`number of points could be earned.
`In another study with school children, Lovitt and Esveldt (1970) selected math to
`investigate the effects of different reinforcement schedules. In this study,
`the authors
`controlled the variability of the academic stimuli by using mathcmaterial
`that was
`already mastered by the students. Their major objective was to determine the effects
`of differential magnitudes of reinforcement on a relatively homogeneous response.,Lovitt
`and Esveldt showed that high rates of academic performance can be produced byrelating
`the rate to differential magnitudes of reinforcement. The question arises as to whether
`such a procedure could be extended to increase the rate of academic performance in
`prison inmates when the complexity of the academic stimuli are not controlled as is
`the case under standard rather than laboratory-based conditions. An opportunity to
`implement a variation on the above procedures presented itself in the context of a
`rehabilitation project involving inmates in a medium security prison. In the course
`of assessing the relative effects of a motivational system on academic progress, it was
`noted that upon the introduction of incentives, the rate of academic performance for
`some inmates increased notably but then became stable, suggestingeither an intellectual
`ceiling had been reached or sufficient motivation was not available (Ayllon and Roberts,
`unpublished).
`e
`
`METHOD
`
`v
`Subjects
`Two black inmates incarcerated for violent crimes and deficient in their academic
`work were chosen for study. One of the inmates, Sanford, was 22 years old and was
`functioning at the 6.1 grade level as measured on the California Test of Basic Skills
`(CTBS) and had an I.Q. of 65 as measured on the Stanford—Binet. The other inmate,
`Martin, was functioning at the 7.7 overall grade level andvhad an I.Q. of 91. Sanford
`was known to be rather withdrawn, while Martin had a reputation of being» hostile
`and aggressive, which resulted in his being sent to the ‘hole’ on several occasions. Though
`they were enrolled in vocational programs in the prison, the two men engaged in little
`work, possibly because of their lack of interest or lack of component academic skills
`required for successful job performance. Both inmates had been in prison for -approxi-
`mately a year prior to the initiation of the present study, and no signs of rehabilitation
`
`

`
`Rapid educational rehabilitation
`
`325 .
`
`,_ were apparent. To help the inmates gain from the vocational and social programs
`offered, intensive academic training was required immediately.
`
`Setting and personnel
`
`The study took place in a state prison in the South. The prison housed over 160
`inmates and had a vocational program as well as an academic program..All inmates
`were enrolled in some type of vocational training and attended their respective voca-
`tional schools five days a week, eight hours a day. During the eight—hour day,
`the
`inmates were permitted to take two hours time oil" to engage in academic training.
`The school room for academic training consisted of individualized study Carrells where
`teachersand tutoring were available to the two inmatesduring the academic"‘period
`except when they were engaged in taking tests.
`
`Academic materials and recording of academic performance
`
`The target response was passing Skill Unit tests at a criterion level of 80 per cent
`or better in both math and English. These tests were based upon two to six assignments.
`A seriesof six Skill Units comprised a Skill Level which was approximately equivalent
`to a grade level.
`Programmed materials were utilized for both. subject areas. All material to prepare
`for assignments and for Skill Unit tests was included in workbooks and programmed
`booklets*. Since the material was designed for individual usage, Sanford and Martin
`could be working on different levels simultaneously and progress at their own unique
`rates.
`
`Placement on the appropriate level at which they could be tested was based on
`CTBS scores. The inmates were then placed in the appropriate workbook in both Eng— _
`lish and math. At any time during their two-hour academic study period,
`they could
`receive assistance from the three certified teachers in the school.
`Skill Unit tests consisted of 15-20 written objective questions requiring short written
`answers or math computations. If the inmate ‘passed’ the skill unit test, he was permitted
`to "go on to the next test. If, however, he failed the test, he had to re-study the material
`or receive any additional help he needed to pass a test on the same skill unit before
`he proceeded.
`All materials were signed in and out of the school otfice by the inmates. When an
`inmate felt prepared to take a test, he had to return all his materials and" sign out
`a Skill Unit test for which he had a specified amount of time to complete and return.
`The return of all tests and the actual testing of the two inmates was directly monitored
`by one of the co—authors so that cheating could not occur.
`
`‘Criteria for incentives
`~ Points were administered on two different schedules during first and last condition
`of the study. During the standard schedule of reinforcement (which served as baseline),
`twenty points were earned by passing each skill unit test in a particular academic area.
`In addition, 120 points were earned for every skill level completed. During the treatment
`condition, an enriched schedule of reinforcement for passing Unit Tests as well as com-
`pleting Skill Levels was employed. Under this schedule, the inmates received a greater
`magnitude of points as the temporal period decreased between tests passed. In short,
`the faster the inmates passed tests under the rate schedule, the more points they received
`per -test. The slower the inmates passed tests, the fewer points they received. A similar
`type of rate schedule was used for completing a Skill Level.
`;The. base level of payment on the enriched schedule was computed by the average
`number of days it took inmates to pass one test under previous standard conditions.
`*T‘he-materials used in math were 7th and 8th Grade Mathematics, A Programinetl Course; Algebra I,
`A Pliogrammed Course; TEM AC Plane Geometry; and TEMAC Trigonometry, all published by Encyclopedia
`Brittanica, 1965. The materials used in English were Keys to Good Language; The Economy Company, 1965,
`for "grades 3 through 6. Also used were English 2200, Englisli 2600, and English 3200, Harcourt, Brace and
`World; Inc;, 1964, for grades 7 and 8, 9 and 10, and 11 and 12 respectively.
`
`

`
`fifty points; but if he passed two tests in one day, he earned 150 points. Ultimately,
`if he passed six tests in one day he earned 700 points. The number of points available
`for passing a Skill Level increased in a similar fashion. If the individual completed
`one Skill Level in sixteen days, he earned 500 points; but if he completed one Skill
`Level in one. day, he earned 4,700 points.
`i
`’
`-
`'
`
`Table
`
`reinforcement
`of
`1. Schedules
`academics
`
`in
`
`Standard Schedule of Reinforcement)
`(A) Passing 1 Unit Test earns 20 points (B)
`Completing 1 Skill Level earns 120 points
`
`Enriched Reinforcement Schedule
`
`(A) Passing Unit Tests
`Number of
`Number of
`tests passed
`days required
`
`Points
`earned
`
`O\U1.|>.L,\J[\))—+i—~>—-i—-
`
`4 or more
`
`>-y-—-i—->—~i—>—»l\)b.>
`
`20
`25
`35
`50
`150
`250
`400
`550
`700
`
`(B) Completing Skill Levels
`Points
`Number of days
`required to complete 1 skill level earned
`
`1
`2
`3
`416
`90 or more
`
`4700
`2000
`1000
`90(%500
`120
`
`Table 1 summarizes ‘the point-earning schedules. When the inmates took a test, they
`were corrected by both the prison clerk and one of the co—authors. Hundred per cent
`agreement was always arrived at.
`
`Incentives which could be exchanged for points
`Points were earned and spent through a credit card system (see Ayllon and Roberts,
`in press). Points were recorded on the day the inmates passed their tests. Each inmate’s
`current point earnings were tallied at a central office and his totalamount of credit
`T was assessed and distributed to areas such as the prison canteen, where the men could
`spend their points. Points could be exchanged for a variety of items and privileges.
`These included consumables and cosmetic goods, extra phone calls, extra visiting‘ privi—
`leges and extra letters. Points could also be exchanged for trading stamps with which
`the inmates could purchase items for themselves and their families. All incentives avail.-
`able for the inmates were additional to the rights and privileges they already had ‘within
`the prison. The inmates were never deprived of their current
`incentives orgdeprived
`of their basic needs to enhance the value of the incentives introduced.
`3 Q .,
`:. T.
`The incentives and their cost did not vary over baseline and treatment conditions.
`The following is a small but representative sample of the incentives availableto the
`
`

`
`Rapid educational rehabilitation
`
`327
`
`inmates and their cost: ten Gold Bond Trading Stamps or cakes and cookies ranged
`from 1
`to 5 points; cigarettes and extra privileges ranged from 20 to 45 points; a
`new radio was 1,000 points.
`
`Baseline: assessment of rate of academic performance under standard level of reinforcement
`
`The two inmates involved in this study had been participating in a motivational
`program where they receivedistandard amounts of reinforcement for three months prior
`to the investigation of the effects of the enriched reinforcement schedule. Therefore,
`baseline measures of math and English performance were taken for the two inmates
`when passing tests was on a standard reinforcement schedule. The rate of academic
`work in both math and English for Martin and Sanfordvwas fairly stable throughout
`the three-month standard reinforcement condition; therefore for purposes of parsimony
`and clarity, only the data for the last twenty days for each inmate are presented here.
`
`PROCEDURE AND RESULTS
`
`_Measures of math and English performance were taken for Sanford and Martin when
`passing tests was on an enriched reinforcement schedule. For Sanford, the enrichment
`phase was 22 days in math and 7 days in English. For Martin, the enrichment phase
`lasted 14 days in math and 8 days in English. Pre (before the standard schedule was
`implemented) and post (after the enriched schedule was implemented) measures were
`taken in Reading, “Language and Arithmetic for both inmateson the California Test
`of Basic Skills.
`
`The results show that the number of tests passed by the two inmates in math and
`English was dramatically higher under the enriched schedule of reinforcement than under
`the standard reinforcement schedule.
`
`Sanford
`
`‘ Math. When Sanford was receiving reinforcement on the standard schedule for math,
`his performance was stable and relatively high. However, under the enriched schedule
`of reinforcement, Sanford’s rate of performance rose from 11 tests in 20 days to 42
`"tests in 22 days. Sanford, therefore, was passing math tests approximately four times
`faster under the enriched schedule than under the standard schedule. This increase in
`
`rate was related to academic progress. Under the standard schedule of reinforcement,
`Sanford passed 9th and part of 10th grade algebra; however,
`in the same period of
`time under enriched conditions he passed and completed 10th, 11th and 12th grade
`algebra and geometry and began trigonometry.
`,
`1 English. Under the standard schedule of reinforcement, Sanford passed relatively few
`English tests. When Sanford was under the enriched schedule, he went from six tests
`in 20 days to 14 tests in seven days. Sanford, therefore, was passing English tests over
`seven times faster under the enriched schedule than under the standard schedule. During
`thegperiod Sanford was under the standard schedule of reinforcement he only completed
`9th grade English. However, in only 7 days when the enriched schedule was in effect
`Sanford passed 10th, 11th and part of 12th grade English.
`
`Martin ’
`
`Math. When Martin was receiving reinforcement on the standard schedule for math
`his performance was consistently at a low level. Under the enriched schedule conditions
`his rate rose from five tests in 20 days to 30 tests in only 14 days. Martin, therefore,
`passed almost nine times as many tests under the enriched schedule than under the
`standard schedule. During the period Martin was under the standard schedule he passed
`8th grade math, however, in almost half the time when the enriched schedule was in
`effect he passed 9th through 12th grade algebra as well as geometry.
`English. Under the standard schedule of reinforcement, Martin was working at a
`relatively stable and moderate rate. Under the enriched schedule of reinforcement he
`went from eight tests in 20 days to passing nine tests in eight days. Martin was, therefore,
`
`

`
`
`
`
`Standard
`Incentives
`
`2 6
`
`IO I4 I822263o343842
`Days
`
`Enriched
`incentives ,
`
`/’
`
`Standard
`incentives
`. . I O O 0
`
`2 6
`
`10 i4
`
`ia® 25
`
`Days
`
`Fig. 1. Cumulative number of math (top of graph) and English (bottom) tests passed for Sanford
`during two incentive conditions. Standard incentives were available for passing tests during
`the first 20 days. The arrow signifies the point where enriched incentives were available for
`passing tests. Each data point is the total number of tests passed in a two-day period except
`where the day is circled.
`
`V
`
`Martin
`
`
`
`
`
`MathsTests(cum.No.)
`
`'0
`
`to
`
`
`
`
`
`English(cum.No.)tests
`
`40
`55
`
`'8
`Z_ so
`E3 25
`
`X8
`
`20
`I5
`,4;
`5 IO
`
`5
`
`
`
`Ennched
`incentives
`
`Standard
`incentives
`
`2
`
`6
`
`IO
`
`I4
`
`I8 22 26 30-34
`Days
`
`Enriched
`incentives ,/
`
`/
`
`'
`
`3 25
`2.
`E 20
`gg
`I5
`:3
`_:
`g
`g»
`Lu
`
`'0
`
`5
`
`~
`
`
`
`Standard
`incentives
`
`'
`
`2
`
`6
`
`IO
`
`I4
`
`i82226
`Days
`
`Fig. 2. Cumulative number of math (top of graph) and English (bottom) tests passed for M’artin'.~ —'
`during two incentive conditions. Standard incentives were available for passing tests during
`the first 20 days. The arrow signifies the point where enriched incentives were available for
`passing tests. Each data point is the total number of tests passed in a two—day/ period.
`
`-~ ‘
`
`

`
`Rapid educational rehabilitation
`
`329
`
`Table 2. Pre-test and post-test scores on the CTBS for both inmates
`Sanford
`
`Reading
`
`Language
`
`Arithmetic
`
`Pre-test
`II-Form A
`Post—test
`III-Form B
`
`Difference
`
`‘
`
`6.3
`
`11.2
`
`+ 4.9
`
`8.0
`
`12.6
`
`+ 4.6
`
`4.8
`
`11.4
`
`+ 6.6
`
`Martin
`
`Reading
`
`Language
`
`Arithmetic
`
`Pre-test
`II-Form A
`Post~test
`III-Form B
`
`7.7
`
`8.9
`
`7.0
`
`8.0
`
`Difference
`
`+ 1.2
`
`+ 1.0
`
`8.3
`
`10.6
`
`+ 2.3
`
`Numbers are in terms of academic grade level.
`
`Overall
`
`grade
`placement
`
`6.1
`
`11.4
`
`+ 5.3
`
`Overall
`grade
`placement
`
`7.7
`
`9.2
`
`+ 1.5
`
`passing'English tests approximately three times faster under the enriched schedule than
`under the standard "schedule. During the period Martin was under the standard schedule
`he passed 6th grade English. However, in less than half the time when he was under
`the enriched schedule he passed 7th and part of 8th grade English
`‘
`' Comparison of California Test of Basic Skills pre test and post test scores indicated
`increased achievement for both inmates during the five-month period the motivation
`system was in effect. Martin increased 1.2 grade levels in reading,
`1 grade level
`in
`language, and 2.3 grade levels in arithmetic. His overall grade placement increase was
`1.5 grade levels. Sanford’s increases were even greater: his reading increased" 4.9 grade
`levels, his language increased 6.6 grade levels, and his arithmetic increased 4.6 grade
`le-vels. His overall grade placement increase was 5.3 grade levels. Table 2 summarizes
`these scores.
`
`DISCUSSION
`
`The findings demonstrate that the application of an enriched schedule will produce
`high.‘ rates of academic performance with adult criminals who were academically defi-
`cient, had a' long history of academic failure, were unmotivated to engage in academic
`work and who were in an environment that far from encouraged academic achievement.
`These results expand upon the findings of Lovitt and Esveldt (1970) who found that
`an enriched schedule of reinforcement resulted in high rates of math performance in
`elementary school children.
`_
`Further,
`these findings demonstrate that the enriched schedule which Lovitt and
`Esveldt found effective on the rate of passing math material already known to the
`children isuseful in facilitating the learning of new materials such as Math and Reading.
`For example, the data show that Sanford could progress through 9th grade math to
`12th grade advanced trigonometry when an enriched schedule of reinforcement was
`available for passing tests. Observations by the prison staff and anecdotal reports of
`other inmates showed that Sanford and Martin spent longer periods of time studying
`for tests as they moved up in levels of difficulty. The instructors reported that the
`men often studied academics during recreational periods. Correctional officers and in-
`
`

`
`This arrangement of reinforcement is in contrast to the schedule proposed by Lovitt
`and Esveldt where responses within ‘a wide range paid off the same amount of points.
`In their study, some children responded at the lower end of the response range and
`gained the same amount of reinforcement as they could have if they responded on
`the higher end of the range.
`As one might expect,
`the two inmates earned a fantastic number of points. This
`resulted in their purchasing a good deal of canteen items; the point-spending records
`showed, however, that most of the points were spent on privileges or gifts for their
`families. The men now had the opportunity to call or write to their families and friends
`and even have these important people visit them more often. Both Sanford and Martin
`used the majority of their points to buy Gold Bond Stamps with which they purchased
`goods for their wives and children. These incentives naturally increased the Contact
`of the inmates with their families and friends and gave them an opportunity to enhance
`relationships. The number of points earned and spent can also be looked at from another
`point of view. A cost—analysis of the program might show that the enriched schedule
`speeds up rehabilitation in such a short period that in the long run it costs less than
`a standard schedule, which prolongs the rehabilitation and thus might result in excessive
`costs.
`An enriched schedule of reinforcement such as this one may be effective when the
`individual is largely deprived of much contact with people or events. A separate source
`of evidence suggests that such limitation may not obtain in practice.*
`In this study, standardized tests had placed Sanford at an I.Q. level of. 65 and Martin
`at a 91 I.Q.
`level. These intelligence scores are below normal and Sanford could be
`considered in the educable mentally retarded range. Nevertheless, both inmates were
`working in 12th grade algebra and trigonometry by the end of their work in the incentive
`program, an accomplishment not predictable from their I.Q. scores. The California Test
`of Basic Skills (CTBS) scores taken before and after the five—month baseline and treat-
`ment period indicated that both men made substantial gains in each subtest areaand
`in their overallgrade placement. Since the day—to—day measures demonstrate that the
`major academic gains took place during the enriched schedule conditions, it can be
`assumed that the change in CTBS scores can be attributed to the brief period of time
`the enriched schedule was in effect. That overall gains ranging from 1.5 to 5.3 grade
`levels were achieved in such a short time, suggests that either the I.Q. measures used
`for these men inaccurately reflected their I.Q., or,
`in these cases at least, I.Q. scores
`do not accurately reflect ability to learn. Because there were only two students involved
`and as there was no control group.
`it is possible that the achievement score changes
`may have been a Hawthorne type effect (Cook, 1962) since this procedure was presented
`to the"’men as a special project. Their measured performance, and their observable
`progress through increasingly diflicult material, however, suggest that the changes in
`achievement scores did result from the experimental procedures and were not spurious.
`This study clearly indicated the usefulness of an enriched schedule and indicated
`the high levels of performance possible with such schedules. Not only were the men
`working harder but, as the achievement tests indicated, they were learning a great deal.
`*The enriched schedule has been shown to be extremely powerful
`in generating academic performance
`with over 30 underachieving elementary school children in a public school in Atlanta, Georgia (for details,
`see Research and Development Report, Atlanta Public Schools, 1974). It is also presently being applied
`in a community~based program for approximately ten juvenile delinquents. In this program, the de1inquent’s
`length of commitment to the community program can be reduced substantially depending on the rate that
`the adolescents perform in several rehabilitation areas. After one year,
`the results show this program to
`be highly promising.
`.
`
`r_
`
`

`
`Rapid educational rehabilitation
`
`331
`
`The most significant finding of this study was that it enabled the staff to properly
`assess the potential in both the inmates who participated in the study. In so doing,
`it became clear that Sanford’s ability had been grossly underestimated. Clearly too,
`Sanford himself had come to believe that he had low intellectual capability. Sanford’s
`I.Q. was tested at 65 when he entered prison the previous year. Yet, it was Sanford
`who rose approximately five to six grades across reading,
`language and arithmetic.
`This study indicates that exposure to learning opportunities and unlimited motivation
`can lead to great strides in education growth. This appears far better than sheltering
`people with limited educational skills from the failure they may never meet.
`
`REFERENCES
`
`AYLLON T. and ROBERTS M. Behavior Modification and Prison Rehabilitation: Toward an Effective Humanism,
`in preparation.
`CLEMENTS C. and MCKEE J. (1968) Programmed instruction for institutionalized offenders: Contingency mana-
`gement and performance contracts. Psychol. Rep. 22, 957—964.
`COHEN H. L., FILIPCZAK J. and BIS J. (1967) A study of contingencies applicable to special education: Case
`I. Educational Facility Press—1BR, Silver Springs, Maryland.
`COOK D. (1962) The Hawthorne effect in educational research. Phi Delta Kappan, December, 116-112.
`LOVITT T. and ESVELDT K. (1970) The relative effects of Inath performance of single Versus multiple ratio
`schedules: A case study. J. appl. Behav. Analysis 3, 261-270.
`MILAN M., WOOD L. WILLIAMS R., ROGERS J., HAMPTOM L. and MCKEE J. (1965) Applied Behavior Analysis
`and the Imprisoned Adult Felon, Project I: The Cellbloclc Token Economy. Rehabilitation Research Founda-
`tion, Montgomery, Alabama.
`‘
`PHILLIPS E. L.
`(1968) Achievement place: Token reinforcement procedures in a home-style rehabilitation
`setting for pre—delinquent boys. J. appl. Behau. Analysis 1, 213-221.
`~
`Rehabilitation Research Foundation (1971) Individually Prescribed Instruction (IPI) System for Delivery of
`Adult Basic Education Skills. Rehabilitation Research Foundation, Montgomery, Alabama.
`RICKARD H., MELVIN K., CREEL J. and CREEL L. (1973) The effects of bonus tokens upon productivity in
`a remedial classroom for behaviorally disturbed children. Behav. Ther. 4, 378—385.
`
`n.R.T. I4/5—I3

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket