throbber
Volume 104
`Number 3
`
`flight © 1933
`American
`psychological
`Association, Inc.
`
`Psychological
`Bulletin
`
`Review Articles
`
`307 Etiology and Treatment ofthe Psychological Side Effects Associated With Cancer Chemotherapy:
`A Critical Review and Discussion
`
`Michael P. Carey and Thomas‘G. Burish
`
`326 Stress Management During Noxious Medical Procedures: An Evaluative Review of Outcome
`Studies
`
`Robin Ludwick—Rosenthal and Richard W. J. Neufild
`
`343 Atypical laterality and Retardation
`Margaret-Ellen Pipe
`
`348 Elucidating the Effects of Reinforcement Magnitude
`Marilyn Bonem and Edward K. Crossman
`
`363 The Amplitude Transition Function and the Manual and Oculomotor Control Systems
`Nicholas C. Barrett and Denis J. Glencrass
`
`373 Coronary Heart Disease and Type A Behaviors: Update on and Alternative to the Booth—Kewley
`and Friedman (1987) Quantitative Review
`Karen A. Matthews
`
`381 Validity of the Type A Construct: A Reprise
`Howard S. Friedman and Stephanie Booth-Kewley
`
`Quantitative Methods in Psychology
`
`385 Data Analysis Using Item Response Theory
`David Thissen and Lynne Steinberg
`
`396 Heterogeneity of Variance in Experimental Studies: A Challenge to Conventional Interpretations
`Anthony S. Brylc and Stephen W Raua'enbush
`
`405 Validity Inferences From Interobserver Agreement
`John S. Uebersax
`
`417 Random Monotone Data Fit Simple Algebraic Models: Correlation Is Not Confirmation
`Scott Parker; Jay Casey: John M. Ziriax, and Alan Silberberg
`
`'
`
`Other
`
`347 Calls for Nominations for JCCR Educational, JPSP: Attitudes, and JPSP: Interpersonal
`
`425 Editorial Consultants
`
`395 Hunt Appointed Editor of JEP: General. 1990—1995
`
`This issue completes Volume 104 and contains the author index for the volume.
`
`(Contents continued on next page)
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 1
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`|PR2014-00676
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 1
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`Important Announcement About Your Subscription
`306
`424 Instructions to Authors
`
`362 Mineka Appointed Editor ofJournal ofAbnormal Psychology 1990—1995
`
`306 Rayner Appointed Editor of JEP: Learning, Memory; and Cognition. 1990—1995
`
`384 Split of JCCP—New Section on Assessment
`
`
`
`Important Announcement About Your Subscription
`
`Due to the increase in high-quality submissions from psychologists
`around the world, Psychological Bulletin will publish approximately
`20% more pages in [989. Therefore, subscription prices will be $50 for
`members, $100 for individuals, and $200 for institutions in 1989.
`
`Rayner Appointed Editor of JEP: Learning, Memory,
`and Cognition, 1990—1995
`
`The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association an-
`nounces the appointment ofKeith Rayner, University ofMassachusetts, as editor ofthe Journal
`ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition for a 6-year term beginning in
`1990. As of Ianuary l, 1989, manuscripts should be directed to
`
`volume.
`
`Keith Rayner
`Department of Psychology
`Tobin Hall
`
`University of Massachusetts
`Amherst, Massachusetts 01003
`
`Manuscript submission patterns for JEP: Learning, Memory and Cognition make the precise
`date of completion of the 1989 volume uncertain. The current editor, Henry Roediger, will
`receive and consider manuscripts until December 31, 1988. Should the 1989 volume be com-
`pleted before that date, manuscripts will be redirected to Rayner for consideration in the l990
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 2
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`
`i,, iPR2014-00676 _,,_m
`
`
`
`
`
`306
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 2
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`This material may be protected by Copyright law (Title 17 U.S. Code)
`
`Psychological Bulletin
`1988, Vol. 104, No. 3. 307-325
`
`Cwyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
`0033-2909188/300JS
`
`Etiology and Treatment of the Psychological Side Effects
`Associated With Cancer Chemotherapy:
`A Critical Review and Discussion
`
`Michael P. Carey
`Syracuse University
`
`Thomas G. Burish
`Vanderbilt University
`
`Cancer patients receiving chemotherapeutic treatments routinely experience a wide range of dis-
`tressing side effects, including nausea, vomiting and dysphoria. Such symptoms often compromise
`patients’ quality of life and may lead to the decision to postpone or even reject future, potentially
`life-saving, treatments. In ihis article, we discuss the hypotheses that have been offered to explain
`the development of such symptoms. We also review, in greater detail, the research evidence for the
`eflicacy offive treatments for such symptoms: hypnosis, progressive muscle relaxation training with
`glided imagery, systematic desensitization, attentional diversion or redirection, and biofeedback.
`We discuss the implications ofthis treatment research, paying particular attention to factors associ-
`ated with treatment outcome, mechanisms of treatment efi‘ectiveness, and issues associated with
`clinical application.
`
`Chemotherapy is the treatment of choice for hundreds of
`thousands of cancer patients diagnosed each year in the United
`States (Silverberg & Lubera, 1986). Its frequent use with cancer
`patients is the result of recent advances in antineoplastic medi-
`cation; new and more effective medications have increased the
`life expectancy for many patients and, in some cases, have re-
`sulted in remission and cure. Unfortunately, such long—term
`gain can come at considerable short-term cost to the cancer pa-
`tient in the form of aversive and debilitating side effects. Among
`the more common drug-induced side efi‘ects are alopecia, sto-
`matitis, immunosuppression, anorexia, nausea, and vomiting.
`In addition to these pharmacological side effects, chemotherapy
`patients also experience psychological side efi‘ects.
`Psychological side effects, which should not necessarily be re-
`garded as abnormal or indicative of psychopathology, are those
`that cannot be attributed directly to the antineoplastic medica-
`tions; instead, such symptoms are believed to result from psy-
`chological processes (e.g., learning) that occur in the chemo-
`therapy context. These symptoms can occur before chemother-
`apy (in which case they are referred to as anticipatory side
`effects) as well as during and alter the actual chemotherapy infu-
`sion. When they occur after chemotherapy has been adminis-
`tered (and while the drugs remain pharmacologically active
`within the system), it is practically impossible to distinguish
`
`We wish to thank Kate B. Carey and the anonymous reviewers for
`their many helpful suggestions on an earlier draft of this review. The
`writing of this manuscript was supported in part by Grant No. 25516
`from the National Cancer Institute, Grant No. PER-29 from the Ameri-
`can Cancer Society, and Grant No. 24 from Syracuse University.
`Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mi-
`chael P. Carey, Department of Psychology, 430 Huntington Hall, Syra-
`cuse University, Syracuse, New York 13244; or to Thomas G. Burish,
`221 Kirkland Hall, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240.
`
`such psychological side effects from their pharmacological
`counterparts. Unfortunately, there has been much inconsis-
`tency in the literature concerning the definition of these symp-
`toms and the terminology used to describe them. For the most
`part, however, research with humans has focused on three
`symptoms, namely, nausea, vomiting, and dysphoria. However,
`it should be noted that considerable animal research and recent
`human research have also focused on other side efi‘ects ofcancer
`
`treatments, especially learned side efi‘ects such as conditioned
`taste and food aversions (e.g., Bernstein & Borson, 1986; Smith,
`Blumsack, & Bilek, 1985) and conditioned immunosuppres~
`sion (c.g., Ader, 1981; Ader & Cohen, 1985). These phenomena
`may develop through mechanisms that are similar to those that
`are the focus of this article.
`
`Symptoms such as nausea, vomiting, and dysphoria are not
`only frequent among cancer chemotherapy patients but can also
`be extremely stressful. In addition to the physical and affective
`distress they cause, many patients are embarrassed by their dis-
`play of symptoms (e.g., anticipatory vomiting), and others even
`fear for their sanity. In fact, some patients eventually discon-
`tinue chemotherapy, abandoning the hope for remission and
`cure rather than suffer from such symptoms (Wilcox, Petting,
`Nettesheim, & Abelofi‘, 1982). It has been suggested that still
`other patients will turn to ineffective and expensive “quack"
`treatments rather than tolerate the paradoxical worsening qual-
`ity of life that chemotherapy can bring. Consequently, oncolo-
`gists (e.g., Laszlo & Lucas, 1981), oncology nurses (cg, Oberst,
`1978), and cancer patients themselves (e.g., Cohn, 1982) have
`all implored researchers to identify an effective treatment for
`the side efl‘ects associated with cancer chemotherapy.
`Pharmacological agents (e.g., prochlorpcrazine, delta-9-tet-
`rahydrocannabinol) have been used to control the psychological
`responses to chemotherapy, but standard antiemetics have been
`found largely inefi'ective for this type ofsymptom (Laszlo, 1983;
`
`307
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 3
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`lPR2014-00676
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 3
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`308
`
`MICHAEL P. CAREY AND THOMAS G. BURISH
`
`Morrow, Aiseneau, Asbury, Bennett, & Boros, 1982). In addi-
`tion, there is evidence that these medications can actually
`worsen the symptomatology under some conditions (Zeltzer,
`LeBaron, & Zeltzer, 1984a). Moreover, even when antiemetics
`provide some relief, they often have side effects of their own
`(e.g., sedation, dystonic reactions) or administration demands
`(e.g., the need for inpatient hospitalization) that limit their acv
`ceptance or usefulness among some patients. The ineffective-
`ness, the paradoxical worsening of symptoms, and the practical
`limitations of pharmacological agents have all prompted re-
`searchers to consider psychological treatments as an alternative
`method of controlling such symptoms.
`In recent years, research on the etiology and treatment of an-
`ticipatory and exacerbatory side effects ofcancer chemotherapy
`has burgeoned and has attracted researchers from several
`health-care disciplines. This increasingly widespread interest is
`based on at least two primary factors. First, from a theoretical
`point of view, the psychological side effects of cancer chemo-
`therapy present an unusual opportunity to study the natural
`development of reactions to repeated aversive treatment within
`a clinical population. As we shall see, these reactions share some
`commonalities with other aversive responses but also appear to
`have some notable differences. Second, from a clinical point of
`view, these side effects are quite prevalent and can be aversive
`and debilitating. As a result, they represent an important clini-
`cal problem.
`The primary purpose of this article is to review the research
`evidence on the etiology and treatment of the most common
`psychological side efl'ects associated with cancer chemotherapy,
`namely, nausea, vomiting, and dysphoria. We begin with an
`overview and evaluation of the etiological formulations that
`have been proffered to explain the development of such symp-
`toms. After this discussion of etiology, we review and critique
`the treatment literature, focusing on investigations that provide
`quantitative outcome data. We discuss the implications of this
`research, paying particular attention to patient factors associ-
`ated with outcome, hypothesized mechanisms by which the
`treatments may exert their impact, and clinical issues in the
`application of such interventions.
`
`Etiology of Psychological Side Effects Associated
`With Cancer Chemotherapy
`
`Psychological side efi‘ects are believed to be relatively com-
`mon. For example, prevalence data obtained from prospective,
`longitudinal studies indicate that approximately 45% of adult
`cancer patients experience nausea, vomiting, or both in the 24
`hr preceding their chemotherapy (Burish & Carey, 1986). Al-
`though precise estimates ofthe prevalence ofpostchemotherapy
`psychological side effects in adults are not available, they are
`believed to be even more common (Burish & Carey, 1986).
`Several causal explanations have been ofl‘ered to explain the
`development ofpsychological side effects. One hypothesis is that
`these symptoms “may be surfacing manifestations of underly-
`ing psychological readjustment problems, associated with life-
`threatening illness" (Chang, 1981, p. 707). This view suggests
`that nonpharmacological, symptoms represent the negative
`affect that patients harbor toward their chemotherapy treat-
`
`ments. To date, no data are available to support this assertion.
`A second hypothesis is that patients may display such symp-
`toms in order to gain attention and sympathy. Inconsistent with
`this hypothesis, however, is the observation that the punishing
`side efi‘ects of chemotherapy far outweigh any secondary gains
`that may be realized by cancer patients; moreover, there are no
`data to support the notion that removal of attention can reduce
`nonpharmacological symptoms. A third hypothesis is that the
`observed symptoms may “be produced by brain metastasis or
`local cancer involvement of the gastrointestinal tract” (Chang,
`I981, p. 707). Although this explanation may be accurate for a
`few patients, it has been ruled out as an explanation for most
`patients (e.g., Morrow, 1982).
`In contrast with the first three hypotheses, which are specula-
`tive and lack empirical support, the fourth hypothesis has been
`supported by the research literature. This hypothesis holds that
`nonpharmacological or psychological
`side efl'ects develop
`through an associative learning process. According to the most
`widely accepted conditioning viewpoint, after one or more pair-
`ings, an association is established between the pharmacological
`side effects (the unconditioned responses; UCRs) caused by the
`chemotherapy (the unconditioned stimulus; UCS) and various
`stimuli (e.g., sights, smells, thoughts; the conditioned stimuli;
`CSs) associated with the chemotherapy setting. As a result of
`repeated associations, the CSs begin to elicit nausea, vomiting,
`and dysphoria (the conditioned responses; CRs), even in the ab-
`sence of the UCS. Two variations of the conditioning model
`have also been suggested. The first, proposed by Leventhal, Eas-
`terling, Nerenz, and Love (1988), is that postchemotherapy
`nausea and vomiting might occasionally serve as the UCS, with
`responses to this nausea and vomiting (e.g., anxiety and second-
`ary nausea occurring later in time) being the UCRs. These
`UCRs then become conditioned to various stimuli in the che-
`motherapy environment and thereby take the form of CR5.
`Thus, in this first variation of the conditioning model, the mor-
`phology of the CS and CR is similar to that of the original
`model, but the UCS and UCR are not. The second variation
`was suggested by Garcia y Robertson and Garcia (1985), who
`believe that conditioned responses to cancer chemotherapy may
`develop through a process that closely resembles taste aversion
`learning. Although the published literature on conditioned re-
`sponses to cancer chemotherapy has been based almost exclu-
`sively on the first model of conditioning, it should be noted that
`these two variations do provide viable conceptualizations of al-
`ternative, though not necessarily mutually exclusive, processes.
`There are several sources of data that converge to support the
`hypothesis that associative learning is the primary phenomenon
`underlying the etiology of psychological symptoms. In no case
`were the data generated by experimental research that was de-
`signed deliberately to induce conditioned nausea and vomiting
`in cancer chemotherapy patients through controlled experi-
`mental manipulations, a procedure that would be ethically un-
`acceptable. Rather, the data are based on analogous phenomena
`or experimental outcomes that consistently, logically, or exclu-
`sively point to associative learning as the most reasonable expla-
`nation. At least four sources of supporting data can be identi-
`lied.
`
`First, the symptoms that are displayed by chemotherapy pa-
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 4
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`|PR2014—00676
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 4
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`PSYCHOLOGICAL SIDE EFFECTS IN CHEMOTHERAPY PATIENTS
`
`309
`
`tients have several topographical similarities to those of labora-
`tory animals that ingest a gastrotoxic substance or that are irra-
`diated while eating a certain food. The animals subsequently
`avoid that substance or food during future feedings, a phenome-
`non referred to as learned taste aversion (for an extended discus-
`sion of the similarities of conditioned nausea and vomiting in
`cancer patients and learned taste aversions, see Garcia y Robert-
`son & Garcia, 1985). The symptoms have been shown to result
`from a learning process that is associative in nature, although
`it deviates, as does the conditioned response of chemotherapy
`patients, from the traditional classical conditioning paradigm
`in some interesting respects (e.g., the symptoms ofien develop
`after only one or a few associations and despite the fact that
`there may be several hours between the UCS and UCR). An-
`other example of documented animal conditioning that bears
`even closer resemblance to the chemotherapy situation was
`demonstrated by Collins and Tatum (1925) and Pavlov (1927).
`These investigators showed that dogs repeatedly injected with
`an emetic drug developed conditioned vomiting in response to
`stimuli associated with the injection.
`Second, several human studies provide data that support an
`associative learning explanation. For example, I. L. Bernstein
`and her colleagues (e.g., I. L. Bernstein, 1978; I. L. Bernstein &
`Webster, 1980) demonstrated experimentally that taste aver-
`sions can develop in chemotherapy patients as a result of the
`emetic properties of the infused drugs. For example, in one
`study the investigators assigned pediatric cancer patients receiv-
`ing emetogenic chemotherapy agents to one of two groups: to
`an experimental group that received a novel-flavored ice cream
`shortly before their scheduled drug treatment or to a control
`group that did not receive the ice cream. A second control
`group of patients receiving nonemetic chemotherapy drugs was
`also included. After 2 or more weeks, patients in all groups were
`offered either some of the novel-flavored ice cream or an oppor-
`tunity to play with a game. Patients in the two control groups
`overwhelmingly chose the ice cream; patients in the experimen-
`tal group showed an aversion to the ice cream, generally prefer-
`ring the game. Similar results were snbsequently demonstrated
`in adult cancer patients (see I. L. Bernstein & Webster, 1985,
`for a review).
`Third, there have been reports of cancer chemotherapy pa-
`tients becoming conditiOned to antiemetic treatments. In these
`situations, the antiemetic was apparently given each time the
`patient became nauseated or was vomiting; as a result, it be—
`came associated with nausea and vomiting and later was able
`to elicit, on its own, nausea and vomiting. For example, Kutz,
`Borysenko, Come, and Benson (1980) reported the case of a
`patient with neurofibrosarcoma who smoked marijuana to alle-
`viate severe nausea and vomiting. After chemotherapy was dis-
`continued, the smell of marijuana in social situations elicited
`nausea and vomiting. In another case reported by the same au-
`thors, the marijuana was administered in brownies and cookies.
`For a year after the chemotherapy was discontinued, the taste
`or sight of these foods produced nausea. Similar conditioning
`to antiemetics has been reported by other investigators (e.g.,
`Morrow et al., 1982).
`Fourth, research has shown that factors related to the devel-
`opment of conditioned symptoms in cancer chemotherapy pa-
`
`tients conform to the principles of associative learning. For ex-
`ample, Andrykowski et al. (in press) and Andrykowski, Redd,
`and Hatfield (1985) conducted two longitudinal studies of the
`development of anticipatory nausea in cancer chemotherapy
`patients. In these investigations, which together involved the
`study of over 150 patients, the authors found that anticipatory
`nausea never occurred without the prior occurrence ofpostche-
`motherapy nausea, that is, consistent with the principles of as-
`sociative learning, the presence of a UCR was necessary for the
`acquisition of a CR. Moreover, afier a careful analysis of other
`factors that contributed to the development of anticipatory
`symptoms, the authors concluded that, consistent with an asso-
`ciative learning model, “all of the factors that reliably predicted
`the development of AN [anticipatory nausea] were either di-
`rectly or indirectly linked to the magnitude” of the uncondi-
`tioned symptoms (Andrykowski et al., in press, p. 11). As has
`been noted elsewhere (Burish & Carey, 1986), other descriptive
`data on the development and nature of conditioned responses
`in cancer chemotherapy patients also consistently conform, in
`prospective as well as retrospective studies, to the principles of
`associative learning.
`In addition to supporting the conditioning model, the avail-
`able data suggest that several factors can serve to mediate or
`potentiate the learning process and thereby produce consider-
`able variation in symptom development. These individual
`difference factors may arise independently of, but nonetheless
`contribute to, the development of conditioned responses.
`One major individual difference may be proneness to nausea
`and vomiting. Research has suggested that patients who have a
`history ofmotion sickness or ofexperiencing nausea and vomit-
`ing to various foods or situations (e.g., pregnancy) are more
`likely to report posttreatment and anticipatory nausea and
`vomiting in response to cancer chemotherapy (Jacobson et al.,
`1988; Morrow, 1985). Morrow (1985) has suggested that there
`is a neurological basis for this relationship. The experience of
`nausea and vomiting is thought to result from‘activation of the
`“vomiting center," located in the lateral reticular formation of
`the medulla oblongata (Borison & McCarthy, 1983). The vom-
`iting center has four major inputs, including one from the yes
`tibular system, which is thought to play a role in motion sick-
`ness. It has been suggested that in addition to affecting the other
`major inputs, chemotherapy may afi‘ect the vestibular system,
`which in patients with a susceptibility to motion sickness may
`lead to additional stimulation of the vomiting center and there-
`fore an increased likelihood of nausea and vomiting (Morrow,
`1985). Redd and his colleagues (Jacobsen et al., 1988; Andry-
`kowski et al., in press) have suggested that there may be consti-
`tutional differences in cancer patients’ susceptibility to gastro-
`intestinal distress, including that due to chemotherapy. Patients
`with a greater constitutional vulnerability to gastrointestinal
`distress may be more likely to respond to chemotherapy with
`high levels ofposttreatment nausea and vomiting, which in turn
`increases the likelihood that they will develop conditioned nau-
`sea and vomiting, in comparison with patients without this di-
`athesis. In summary, the data suggest that patients with a past
`history of nausea and vomiting resulting from motion sickness,
`certain foods, or other experiences are more likely to develop
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 5
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`|PR2014-00676
`
`
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 5
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`310
`
`MICHAEL P. CAREY AND THOMAS G. BURISH
`
`conditioned nausea and vomiting in response to cancer chemo-
`therapy.
`A second major factor that appears to affect the development
`of conditioned responses to chemotherapy is a patient’s anxiety
`level.l Specifically, state anxiety levels have been positively re—
`lated to the presence of conditioned responses in a number of
`retrospective studies (e.g., lngle, Burish, & Wallston, 1984; van
`Komen & Redd, 1985) and prospective investigations (e.g., An-
`drykowski et a1., 1985, in press; Nerenz, Leventhal, Easterling,
`& Love, 1986). For example, Nerenz et a1. (1986) interviewed
`cancer patients before each of their first six treatment cycles.
`The authors found that the incidence of anticipatory nausea
`was related to the level of pretreatment anxiety: for mildly anx-
`ious patients the incidence averaged 9.8%; for highly anxious
`patients the incidence was approximately twice as much, aver-
`aging 18.1%. Andrykowsld et al. (1985) found that a patient’s
`self-reported anxiety level before treatments accounted for
`more variance (13.6%) than any other single variable except
`posttreatment nausea in determining whether a patient devel-
`oped anticipatory nausea.
`Although data from numerous studies appear to suggest that
`heightened anxiety levels facilitate the development of condi-
`tioned symptoms, two questions remain. First, what are the
`temporal parameters that determine the relationship between
`state anxiety and the development of conditioned responses?
`That is, exactly when during the course of chemotherapy do
`elevated anxiety levels produce this relationship? Second, why
`is heightened anxiety associated with conditioned symptoms?
`A number of possible answers have been discussed, although
`none have been tested in the chemotherapy context. in most
`cases, investigators have speculated that anxiety directly or indi-
`rectly affects the associative learning process in ways that lead to
`enhanced conditioning. For example, some of the explanations
`suggest that anxiety levels directly alfect conditioning by influ-
`encing the speed with which associative learning takes place.
`According to this explanation, highly anxious patients condi-
`tion more quickly than do less anxious patients (e.g., Spence,
`1958). Data recently repoited by Andrykowski and Redd
`(1987) conflict with this explanation, however. These authors
`interviewed patients before each of their chemotherapy treat-
`ments to determine pretreatment anxiety levels and pre- and
`posttreatment nausea levels. They found that patients who de-
`veloped anticipatory symptoms late in the course ofchemother—
`apy (i.e., alter their seventh treatment) generally had higher anx-
`iety levels during all treatments than did patients who devel-
`oped anticipatory symptoms early in the treatment course.
`Andrykowski and Redd (1987) also noted that late—onset pa-
`tients generally had lower posttreatment nausea levels until the
`session just before they developed anticipatory nausea, at which
`time their posttreatment levels increased substantially. The au-
`thors speculated that the heightened pretreatment anxiety levels
`eventually led to the increased posttreatment nausea and that
`once posttreatment nausea increased, patients were more likely
`to develop anticipatory symptoms (i.e., the greater the intensity
`of the unconditioned response, the more likely the development
`of a conditioned response).
`Others have focused on dilferent parameters in trying to ex-
`plain the relationship between anxiety and conditioning. Dol-
`
`gin, Katz, McGinty, and Siegel (1985), for example, have sug-
`gested that highly anxious patients tend to show high levels of
`vigilance to their environments and, as a result, tend to notice
`more closely various stimuli in the clinic setting. Such attention
`increases the likelihood that these stimuli will develop into CSs.
`They found that consistent with this hypothesis, pediatric can-
`cer patients with anticipatory symptoms attended to and pro-
`cessed more stimuli in their environments, and habituated to
`these stimuli more slowly, than did matched patients without
`anticipatory symptoms.
`Although most of the speculation about the relationship be-
`tween anxiety and conditioned symptoms focuses on the role of
`anxiety within the learning paradigm, J. H. Fetting (personal
`communication, March 1, 1988) has suggested a neurologically
`based explanation that involves the relationship between anxi-
`ety and neurotransmitter changes. In the first stage ofa hypoth-
`esized two-stage process, a cancer patient’s propensity for expe-
`riencing nausea and vomiting is increased as a result of either
`a decreased nausea/vomiting threshold or enhanced neuronal
`firings, either or both of which might be caused by repeated
`bouts of nausea and vomiting during periods of high anxiety.
`As a result of this enhanced propensity, a variety of stimuli will
`more readily cause gastrointestinal upset in the future.
`In the second stage, increased noradrenergic activity, caused
`by the heightened anxiety or stress levels, contributes to the de-
`velopment of anticipatory nausea and vomiting in patients with
`the increased propensity. The process by which the increased
`noradrenergic activity leads to these efi'ects is as yet undeter-
`mined, but J. H. Fetting (personal communication, March 1,
`1988) speculated that with repeated exposure to emetogenic
`chemotherapy, the noradrenergic terminals in areas adjacent to
`the vomiting center in the cortex may show increased activity,
`leading to greater stimulation of the vomiting center. In an ini-
`tial test of this hypothesis, Fetting et a1. (1987) administered
`clonidine, a drug that reduces noradrenergic activity, to 8 chen
`motherapy patients who displayed anticipatory symptoms to
`their chemotherapy. After one trial of clonidine, the anticipa-
`tory symptoms were completely eliminated in 4 (50%) of the
`patients. Postchemotherapy nausea and vomiting appeared to
`be unafl'ected.
`
`Overall, then, it appears that the psychological symptoms
`that occur during cancer chemotherapy, particularly the nausea
`and vomiting that occur prior to drug infusion, are acquired
`through an associative learning process; moreover, the data sug-
`gest that the development of such symptoms is moderated by
`
`‘ For the sake of completeness, it should be noted that in addition to
`anxiety level, a number of other individual diflerence factors have been
`found to be correlated with the presence of anticipatory symptoms, for
`example, experiencing taste sensations during chemotherapy, having an
`inhibitive rather than facilitative coping style, being treated in a large
`group room rather than in a small private room, being younger rather
`than older, experiencing itching sensations during chemotherapy, and
`receiving chemotherapy treatments through long infusions rather than
`through short push injections (see Burish & Carey, 1986, for a review).
`However, most of these factors have been reported in retrospective in-
`vestigations exclusively, have been found in only one or two studies and
`not in others, and have not been linked causally to the development of
`conditioned symptoms.
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 6
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`|PR2014—00676
`
`IMMUNOGEN 2182, pg. 6
`Phigenix v. Immunogen
`IPR2014-00676
`
`

`

`
`
`PSYCHOLOGICAL SIDE EFFECTS IN CHEMOTHERAPY PATIENTS
`
`311
`
`one’s prior experience with gastrointestinal upset from factors
`such as motion sickness or certain foods and by one’s treat-
`ment-related anxiety level. Thus many writers have concluded
`that the psychological symptoms that occur in the chemother«
`apy setting are conditioned responses. In this regard, it is impor-
`tant to note that conditioned responses to cancer chemotherapy
`are similar, developmentally and phenomenologically, to condi-
`tioned aversive responses that develop routinely in other clinical
`contexts. For example, Garcia y Robertson and Garcia (1985)
`have related the animal and human literature on learned taste
`
`aversions to the research on conditioned nausea and vomiting in
`cancer chemotherapy patients. Conditioned responses to cancer
`chemotherapy can also be viewed as a subset of other condi-
`tioned drug responses (e.g., to morphine or alcohol; Siegel,
`1979). Although the review of the extensive literature on condi-
`tioned aversive responses is beyond the scope of this article, it
`is important to keep these similarities in mind when reviewing
`the treatment literature because many of the procedures that
`have been used to ameliorate or prevent conditioned responses
`in cancer chemotherapy patients are similar to or are based on
`the same principles as those that have been used with other
`types of conditioned aversive responses. Many of the treatment
`advances that have been made in the cancer area are, therefore,
`potentially applicable to other areas as well.
`Although associative learning appears to play an important
`role in the etiology ofpsychological side effects, we propose that
`side effects in the chemotherapy context might also result from,
`or be exacerbated by. a fifth mechanism, namely, psychological
`stress (i.e., the process that occurs when a person appraises a
`situation or stimulus as taxing his or her resources and endan-
`gering his or her well-being; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Thus,
`we hypothesize that a person who appraises the chemotherapy
`process as threatening or who is unprepared to cope

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket