throbber
Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13, 336–346  doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.106.003194
`
`Why don’t patients take their medicine?
`Reasons and solutions in psychiatry†
`Alex J. Mitchell & Thomas Selmes
`
`Abstract  Over the course of a year, about three­quarters of patients prescribed psychotropic medication will 
`discontinue, often coming to the decision themselves and without informing a health professional. 
`Costs associated with unplanned discontinuation may be substantial if left uncorrected. Partial non­
`adherence (much more common than full discontinuation) can also be detrimental, although some 
`patients rationally adjust their medication regimen without ill­effect. This article reviews the literature 
`on non­adherence, whether intentional or not, and discusses patients’ reasons for failure to concord 
`with medical advice, and predictors of and solutions to the problem of non­adherence. 
`
`This is the first of two articles by Mitchell & Selmes in APT on patient
`engagement and retention in treatment. The second, which addresses
`missed appointments, will appear in the next issue of the journal
`(Mitchell & Selmes, 2007).
`
`The degree to which an individual follows medical 
`advice is a major concern in every medical specialty 
`(Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). Much attention has 
`focused on methods to persuade patients to adhere 
`to recommendations, without sufficient acknowl­
`edgement  that  avoidance  of  sometimes  complex, 
`costly  and  unpleasant  regimens  may  be  entirely 
`rational  (Mitchell,  2007a).  Equally  overlooked  is 
`the influence of communication between patients 
`and  healthcare  professionals.  Put  simply,  if  no 
`clear agreement is formed with the patient at the 
`onset of treatment then it should be of no surprise 
`if concordance turns out to be less than ideal. In one 
`study the chance of premature discontinuation was 
`found to be less than half in patients who recalled 
`being told to take the medication for at least 6 months 
`compared  with  those  not  given  this  information 
`(Bull et al, 2002b). This task is made more difficult 
`when patients lack insight into their condition (see 
`below).
`Given  the  necessity  of  therapeutic  agreement, 
`the term compliance has given way to adherence 
`and  concordance  (Box  1)  (Haynes et al,  2002).  In 
`considering  the  nosology  of  concordance  and 
`adherence a useful distinction is between individuals 
`
`† For a commentary on this article see pp. 347–349, this issue.
`
`who do not start a medication (similarly those who 
`do  not  attend  their  first  appointment)  and  those 
`who  start  the  course  but  either  take  medication 
`incompletely (partial compliance or adherence) or 
`discontinue prematurely against medical advice (Fig. 
`1). There has been little research on reluctance to start 
`medication which equates to treatment refusal or 
`overt non­adherence. Kasper et al (1997) found in a 
`group of 348 newly admitted psychiatric in­patients 
`that 12.9% refused treatment but that 90% of these 
`ended their refusal within 4 days. Synthesising data 
`on adherence behaviour is difficult because of the 
`wide range of assessment methods (for review see 
`Velligan et al, 2006). Few studies have examined what 
`
`Box 1 Working definitions about health
`behaviour
`
`Adherence (compliance)  is  the  extent  to  which 
`an individual changes their health behaviour 
`to coincide with medical advice
`Concordance is  the  degree  to  which  clinical 
`advice and health behaviour agrees
`Therapeutic alliance  is  an  agreement  between 
`patients  and  health  professionals  to  work 
`together
`Therapeutic disagreement is a divergence in the 
`views  of  patients  and  doctors  on  the  subject 
`of treatment
`
`Alex Mitchell is Consultant and Honorary Senior Lecturer in Liaison Psychiatry at University Hospitals Leicester (Department of Liaison 
`Psychiatry, Brandon Unit, Leicester General Hospital, Gwendolen Road, Leicester LE5 4PW, UK. Email: alex.mitchell@leicspart.nhs.uk) 
`and author of the prize­winning book Neuropsychiatry and Behavioural Neurology Explained: : Diseases, Diagnosis, and Management (2003). 
`Thomas Selmes is a senior house officer on the York psychiatry scheme and was previously a medical student at Leicester University.
`
`336
`
`

`
`Why don’t patients take their medicine?
`
`Course  
`of medication started
`
`No
`
`Refusal of primary 
`(initial) treatment
`
`Yes
`
`Overt non­adherence
`
`Medication stopped
`
`Medication interrupted
`
`Medication adjusted
`
`Discontinuation
`
`Full non­adherence
`
`Conversion to discontinuation
`
`Missed doses
`
`Extra doses
`
`Partial non­adherence
`
`Excess adherence
`
`Patient wished to stop 
`taking medication?
`
`Yes
`
`No
`
`Patient wished  
`to alter dose?
`
`Yes
`
`No
`
`Intentional
`
`Unintentional
`
`Intentional
`
`Unintentional
`
`External
`
`Internal
`
`Barrier
`
`Lapse or slip
`
`External
`
`Internal
`
`Barrier
`
`Lapse or slip
`
`Disclosed to clinician?
`
`Yes
`
`No
`
`Disclosed to clinician?
`
`Yes
`
`No
`
`Refusal of secondary 
`treatment
`
`Self­discontinuation
`
`Collaborative  
`self­medication
`
`Autonomous  
`self­medication
`
`Overt discontinuation
`
`Covert discontinuation
`
`Overt partial adherence Covert partial adherence
`
`Fig. 1 Nosology of adherence behaviour.
`
`advice patients actually receive and thus it is often 
`impossible to test whether patient behaviour is truly 
`at odds with what was agreed.
`It  is  useful  to  remember  than  many  predictors 
`are generic and applicable to all patients (Box 2). 
`Three important predictors are complexity of the 
`regimen, duration of the course and frequency of 
`follow­up contact. Regular contact reduces rates of 
`both missed medication and missed appointments 
`(Rittmannsberger et al, 2004; this will be discussed 
`further in Mitchell & Selmes, 2007). The complexity 
`of a treatment regimen (number of tablets, number 
`of medications and drug interval) adversely affects 
`adherence. Equally, regimens that require disruption 
`to lifestyle, or special techniques or arrangements 
`are less welcome by patients. A systematic review 
`found that the number of doses prescribed per day 
`was inversely related to adherence (Claxton et al, 
`2001). In elderly patients acceptable adherence is 
`common  in  those  taking  only  one  medicine  but 
`rapidly falls in those taking four or more. Regarding 
`the  length  of  the  prescribed  course  of  treatment, 
`most imagine it is relatively simple to follow a 5­
`day antibiotic course but in reality only two­thirds 
`
`manage to do so successfully. Ask patients to follow 
`a 7­day course, with antibiotics four times a day, 
`and ideal concordance is achieved by less than 40%. 
`In psychiatry, we expect patients to agree to much 
`longer courses of medication (occasionally for an 
`indefinite period if risks are judged sufficiently high) 
`(Mitchell, 2006a). 
`In this review, we will examine the predictors of 
`both partial adherence and discontinuation in those 
`taking psychotropic medication, in an attempt to 
`understand why some patients have difficulty fol­
`lowing the advice of mental health professionals and 
`why adherence rates appear to be lower than in other 
`specialties (see also Cramer & Rosenheck, 1998).
`
`Costs of less-than-ideal adherence
`
`There is considerable evidence that premature medi­
`cation discontinuation is costly (Sullivan et al, 1995). 
`Undisclosed (covert) non­adherence appears to be 
`particularly hazardous (Task Force for Compliance, 
`1994).  Kang  et al  (2005)  examined  antipsychotic 
`adherence  of  individuals  prior  to  admission  to 
`
`Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13. http://apt.rcpsych.org/
`
`337
`
`

`
`Box 2 Predictors of treatment concordance
`problems
`
`General
`Duration and complexity of regimen
`Lack of informal support
`Patient (intentional)
`Concerns about the side­effects 
`Few perceived benefits
`Stigma of taking medication
`Adjustment to suit daily routine
`Concerns about cost
`Concerns about availability
`Concerns about dependency
`Patient (non-intentional)
`Slips and lapses
`External distractors
`Misunderstanding instructions
`Clinician
`Poor doctor–patient relationship
`Poor empathy
`Poor explanation/communication
`Inadequate follow­up
`Illness
`Severe illness
`Depression or distress
`Psychosis
`Cognitive impairment
`
`• • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • •
`
`Specific adverse effects
`
`Weight gain due to medication has been linked with 
`non­adherence and subjective distress (Fakhoury et
`al, 2001). Obese individuals are more than twice as 
`likely as those with a normal body mass index to 
`miss their medication (Weiden et al, 2004a). Fakhoury 
`(1999) found that more than 70% of patients described 
`weight  gain  due  to  antipsychotics  as  extremely 
`distressing, which was higher than that for any other 
`side­effect.
`Sexual dysfunction is a significant source of distress 
`and may be linked to poor adherence. Olfson et al 
`(2005) studied sexual dysfunction in 139 out­patients 
`with DSM–IV schizophrenia who were receiving an 
`antipsychotic but no other medications associated 
`with sexual side­effects. Sexual dysfunction occur­
`red in 45.3% of the group and was associated with 
`significantly  lower  ratings  on  global  quality  of 
`life. Rosenberg et al (2003) examined the effects of 
`sexual side­effects on adherence. They found that 
`62.5% of men and 38.5% of women felt that their 
`psychiatric medications were causing sexual side­
`effects; 41.7% of men and 15.4% of women admitted 
`
`Mitchell & Selmes
`
`hospital with schizophrenia: 37.1% had been totally 
`non­adherent during the 6 months prior to admission. 
`Although the majority had missed medication on 
`fewer  than  20%  of  days,  many  relapsed  anyway. 
`Reduced  adherence  weakens  treatment  benefits, 
`especially when no alternatives are explored (Irvine
`et al, 1999). 
`Fewer studies in psychiatry have looked at the 
`complications of partial concordance. Three of the 
`most influential of these have been conducted in 
`the USA. In a sample of 7864 Medicaid patients tak­
`ing atypical antipsychotics, those who achieved less 
`than 80% of ideal concordance were about 50% more 
`likely than fully concordant patients to have been 
`hospitalised (Eaddy et al, 2005). Jeste’s group (Gilmer
`et al, 2004) found a hierarchy of risk of admission, 
`from those who were non­adherent, those who took 
`medication to excess and finally those who were 
`partially adherent. In the largest study, Valenstein et
`al (2002) showed that those with less than 80% con­
`cordance were 2.4 times more likely to be admitted 
`than patients with good concordance. 
`
`General predictors of missed
`medication
`
`A number of authors have reviewed factors predict­
`ing non­adherence, but only a handful have con­
`sidered predictors in psychiatric populations (Fenton 
`et al, 1997). Predictors may be usefully divided into 
`patient factors, clinician factors and illness factors 
`(Box 2).
`
`Patient factors
`Intentional non­adherence
`
`Distinguishing intentional non­adherence (missing 
`or  altering  doses  to  suit  one’s  needs)  from  un­
`intentional non­adherence (e.g. forgetting to take 
`medication) is a relatively recent development. In 
`fact, in long­term studies understandable reasons for 
`discontinuation are more common than irrational 
`reasons. Intentional non­adherence is predicted by 
`the balance of an individual’s reasons for and against 
`taking medication, as suggested by utility theory. 
`Intentional non­adherence is a common reason not 
`to start a course of medication, but it may be less 
`common than accidental non­adherence in relation 
`to  missing  individual  doses  (Lowry et al,  2005). 
`Predictors  of  intentional  non­adherence  include 
`less severe disease (and feeling well), the desire to 
`manage independently of the medical profession 
`(self­efficacy),  disagreement  with  or  low  trust 
`in clinicians, and receipt of low levels of medical 
`information (Piette et al, 2005). 
`
`338
`
`Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13. http://apt.rcpsych.org/
`
`

`
`that  they  had  stopped  their  medications  at  some 
`point  during  treatment  because  of  sexual  side­
`effects.  Importantly,  50%  of  the  sample  ‘never  or 
`infrequently’ spoke about sexual functioning with 
`their  primary  mental  healthcare  providers,  and 
`80% of the women with sexual side­effects had not 
`discussed  sexual  dysfunction  with  their  mental 
`healthcare providers. 
`
`Illness beliefs and knowledge of medication
`
`Concepts of health and disease are important factors 
`in  adherence  in  mental  health  (Kelly  et al,  1987). 
`Patients’ understanding of their condition and its 
`need for treatment is positively related to adherence, 
`and  in  turn  adherence,  satisfaction  and  under­
`standing are all related to the amount and type of 
`information given. Studies have shown that patients 
`who  understand  the  purpose  of  the  prescription 
`are twice as likely to collect it than those who do 
`not understand (Daltroy et al, 1991). Awareness of 
`treatment accounted for 20% and 34% of variation 
`in subjective responses (satisfaction with medication 
`and tolerability) to atypical and typical antipsychotic 
`drugs in one recent report (Ritsner et al, 2004). 
`It is often assumed that patients understand a rea­
`sonable amount about their illness, but how often 
`is this assumption tested? In a classic report, Joyce 
`et al (1969) demonstrated that patients were unable 
`to recall half of the information given to them by 
`their physician. Two­thirds of individuals recently 
`discharged from hospital did not know what time to 
`take medication and less than 15% recalled the com­
`mon side­effects (Makaryus & Friedman, 2005). 
`Preliminary  work  suggests  that  figures  in  psy­
`chiatry are comparable. Only 1 in 10 of those tak­
`ing clozapine are aware of potential haematological 
`risks (Angermeyer et al, 2001). Of those prescribed 
`antipsychotics most do not feel involved in treatment 
`decisions and state that they take medication only 
`because they are told to (Gray et al, 2005). Patients 
`typically leave the clinic with a poor understanding 
`of the rationale for therapy (Weiden et al, 2004a). In 
`one study, two­thirds of psychiatric in­patients did 
`not understand why they were taking medication, 
`and the vast majority could not be said to have given 
`informed consent to their treatment (Brown et al, 
`2001). This finding was consistent in both detained 
`and informal in­patients. Similarly, many patients 
`misunderstand prescription instructions. Col et al 
`(1990) found that 50% of patients with depression be­
`lieved they did not need their antidepressants when 
`they began to feel better or that the medication could 
`be taken as required. The UK public campaign Defeat 
`Depression revealed that many people were wary 
`of taking antidepressants because they believed that 
`individuals with depression should ‘pull themselves 
`
`Why don’t patients take their medicine?
`
`together’ and more than three­quarters believed that 
`the medications are addictive (Paykel et al, 1998). In a 
`small sample with mood disorder or schizophrenia, 
`Adams & Scott (2000) found that two components of 
`the health beliefs model (perceived severity and per­
`ceived benefits) and two modifying factors (dysfunc­
`tional attitudes and locus of control beliefs regarding 
`health) differentiated significantly between highly 
`adherent and poorly adherent individuals. Although 
`little work has examined how illness beliefs (good 
`and bad) are formed, previous bad experiences are 
`an important predictor of future non­attendance and 
`non­adherence (Gonzalez et al, 2005). 
`
`Clinician factors
`The doctor–patient relationship
`
`The importance of good communication between 
`patient  and  health  professional  is  increasingly 
`acknowledged in relation to adherence (Stevenson
`et al, 2004). At its essence this means forging a joint 
`therapeutic agreement with full patient involvement. 
`This  is  a  two­way  process  in  which  willingness 
`to  discuss  mental  health  issues  with  a  doctor  is 
`predicted largely by the perceived helpfulness of 
`and trust in that doctor (Wrigley et al, 2005). This can 
`be quantified using a tool that measures therapeutic 
`alliance perceived by patient or clinician. 
`In practice, the process of making a joint thera­
`peutic  plan  is  often  abbreviated.  Doctors  tend  to 
`overestimate the amount of information they have 
`given to patients (Makoul et al, 1995). At the same 
`time, patients often misunderstand medical words. 
`According  to  one  study  (Thompson  &  Pledger, 
`1993), 22% of patients have difficulty with the word 
`‘symptom’, 38% with the word ‘orally’ and 76% with 
`the word ‘stroke’.
`Patients who are very unwell or without insight 
`are unlikely to tolerate an extensive dialogue about 
`possibilities. Yet collaborative decision­making has 
`been shown to be a consistent predictor of health 
`outcomes. Kaplan et al (1996) examined participatory 
`decision­making style on a three­item scale. Higher 
`scores  for  clinicians’  participatory  styles  were 
`associated with greater patient satisfaction and less 
`likelihood of changing doctor. Bultman & Svarstad 
`(2000) conducted an impressive study on reasons 
`for missed antidepressants. They found that 25% of 
`clients were not satisfied with their medication and 
`82% reported missing doses or stopping treatment 
`earlier than recommended. Path analysis showed 
`that patients with more positive beliefs about the 
`treatment are more likely to attend for follow­up and 
`are more satisfied with treatment after attempting 
`medication  use.  Physicians’  communication  style 
`during  follow­up  and  client  satisfaction  were 
`
`Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13. http://apt.rcpsych.org/
`
`339
`
`

`
`Mitchell & Selmes
`
`both predictive of better medication adherence. A 
`collaborative communication style by the clinician 
`enhanced  client  knowledge  of  the  medication, 
`improved satisfaction with medication and improved 
`reliability of medication use. 
`
`Predictors of missed medication
`in specific psychiatric disorders
`Depression and antidepressants
`
`The  adherence  habits  of  those  prescribed  anti­
`depressants  have  recently  been  summarised 
`elsewhere (Mitchell, 2006b). Roughly 10% of patients 
`prescribed antidepressants fail to pick up their first 
`prescription and about a third collect only the initial 
`(typically 4­week) prescription. Of those who start 
`medication,  non­adherence  rates  increase  with 
`time (Bultman & Svarstad, 2002). In a study of 200 
`patients attending 14 family doctors in five different 
`practices, non­adherence rates were 16% at week 
`one, 41% at week two, 59% at week three and 68% 
`at week four (Johnson, 1981). In those on long­term 
`maintenance  treatment,  discontinuation  rates  for 
`selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are 
`above 70% (Mullins et al, 2005). Of all those who 
`discontinue medication, 60% have not informed their 
`doctor by 3 months and a quarter have not done 
`so by 6 months (Maddox et al, 1994; Demyttenaere
`et al, 2001). This is sometimes referred to as covert 
`non­adherence.
`
`Predictors of missed antidepressant medication
`
`Sirey et al (2001) found that perceptions of stigma 
`about depression at the start of treatment predicted 
`subsequent medication adherence. Aikens et al (2005) 
`asked 81 primary care patients given maintenance 
`antidepressant medications about their adherence. 
`Variation in adherence could be primarily explained 
`by the balance between patients’ perceptions of need 
`v. perceptions of harm, with adherence being lowest 
`when  the  perceived  harm  of  the  antidepressant 
`exceeded  the  perceived  need.  Two  consistent 
`predictors of stopping medication are feeling better 
`and adverse effects. About 35% of patients stop after 
`feeling better at 3 months (Maddox et al, 1994) and 
`55% stop after feeling better at 6 months (Demyttenaere
`et al,  2001).  Ayalon et al  (2005)  examined  both 
`intentional and non­intentional discontinuation of 
`antidepressants. After controlling for ethnicity and 
`medication  type,  intentional  non­adherence  was 
`associated with concerns about the side­effects of 
`the medication and the stigma associated with taking 
`antidepressants. Unintentional non­adherence was 
`associated with greater cognitive impairment. In a 
`study of adherence to SSRIs, Bull et al (2002a) found 
`
`predictors  of  discontinuation  to  be  length  of 
`treatment, low­dose prescription and the occurrence 
`of one or more moderately or extremely bothersome 
`adverse effect. If the adverse effect had been discussed 
`in advance then the likelihood of discontinuation 
`was halved. This is consistent with the finding that 
`individuals with depression generally want more 
`information  about  their  condition  than  they  are 
`offered and want to be involved in decision­making 
`(Garfield et al, 2004). In depression, as in other areas, 
`the  more  information  that  is  given  the  better  is 
`adherence (Maidment et al, 2002).
`
`Mania and mood stabilisers
`Studies  appear  to  support  the  observation  that 
`one­quarter to one­third of patients maintained on 
`lithium are poorly adherent (Cochran, 1986). About 
`a third of those prescribed lithium report that their 
`adherence behaviour is poor and in a similar number 
`sub­optimal serum levels indicate poor adherence 
`(Scott & Pope, 2002). Johnson & McFarland (1996) 
`performed  a  6­year  longitudinal  cohort  study  to 
`determine  patterns  of  lithium  use  in  a  large  US 
`‘health  maintenance  organisation’.  Lithium  users 
`took the drug on an average of 34% of the days. As 
`in other areas, more patients are partially adherent 
`than entirely non­adherent (Colom et al, 2000). 
`In contrast to the above results, MacLeod & Sharp 
`(2001) found much higher rates of adherence, but all 
`of their sample were attending a lithium maintenance 
`clinic, which may have influenced this finding.
`
`Predictors of missed mood stabiliser medication
`
`In  the  handful  of  studies  that  have  examined 
`adherence predictors in bipolar disorders, adverse 
`effects  and  insight  are  common  themes.  In  one 
`study 61% of patients prescribed lithium reported 
`slight or moderate side­effects compared with 21% 
`on  carbamazepine  (Greil  &  Kleindienst,  1999). 
`Recently Bowden et al (2005) reported premature 
`discontinuation rates of about 70% during a 1­year 
`trial  of  lithium,  placebo  and  divalproate:  23.2% 
`of those initially dysphoric at entry who received 
`lithium  maintenance  therapy  and  17.1%  of  those 
`treated  with  divalproate  maintenance  therapy 
`discontinued  prematurely  because  of  side­effects 
`compared with 4.8% of those treated with placebo. 
`Regarding illness severity and insight, Yen et al (2005) 
`found that insight at baseline predicted concordance 
`up to 1 year later.
`
`Schizophrenia and antipsychotics
`Perhaps the most striking data come from studies 
`of full discontinuation by people with schizophrenia. 
`
`340
`
`Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13. http://apt.rcpsych.org/
`
`

`
`Even in highly monitored randomised controlled 
`trials, discontinuation rates can be greater than 50%. 
`At  least  five  large­scale  studies  have  shown  that 
`adherence with both old and new antipsychotics is 
`poor in the long term. For example, Rosenheck et al 
`(1997)  found  that  68%  of  patients  treated  with 
`haloperidol and 43% of those treated with clozapine 
`had discontinued medication before the end of a 
`1­year trial. In the 18­month Clinical Antipsychotic 
`Trials for Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) study 
`(Lieberman et al, 2005) a remarkable 74% of patients 
`discontinued medication prematurely. The most com­
`mon reasons for discontinuation were patient choice, 
`lack of effect or intolerability of side­effects. 
`Outside  of  randomised  controlled  trials  many 
`groups have looked at more subtle forms of poor 
`concordance, including under­ and excessive dosing 
`by people with schizophrenia. Seven studies have 
`recruited more than 1000 patients (these are reviewed 
`by Velligan et al, 2006). Rijcken et al (2004) found that 
`33% of the patients in their sample had sub­optimal 
`prescription  renewals  (less  than  90%),  56%  had 
`good concordance and 11% had excess prescription 
`renewals. Weiden et al (2004b) found that 90% of a 
`sample of 675 individuals with schizophrenia had 
`some degree of partial adherence and on 36% of the 
`study  days  someone  had  run  out  of  medication. 
`Patient surveys suggest that almost half (44%) have at 
`some point stopped taking their medication without 
`agreement of their doctor (Hogman & Sandamas, 
`2000).
`
`Predictors of missed antipsychotic medication
`
`There  is  a  concern  that  people  diagnosed  with 
`schizophrenia  are  infrequently  involved  in  treat­
`ment decisions and sometimes not even told their 
`diagnosis  (Bayle et al,  1999).  One  patient  survey 
`(Gray et al, 2005) reports that most of the participants 
`prescribed  antipsychotics  did  not  feel  involved 
`in  treatment  decisions  and  had  not  been  given 
`written information about their treatment, warned 
`about side­effects or offered non­pharmacological 
`alternatives.
`Several  cross­sectional  studies  link  severity  of 
`psychopathology to medication non­adherence (Van 
`Putten et al, 1976; Pan & Tantam, 1989). Grandiose 
`thoughts and persecutory thoughts may carry partic­
`ular risk (Van Putten et al, 1976). Low insight predicts 
`non­adherence and, importantly, improvements in 
`insight are often accompanied by improvements in 
`concordance (Rittmannsberger et al, 2004). However, 
`low insight may be linked to poorer cognitive func­
`tion and one study suggests that during the first 
`year of treatment, patients with poorer premorbid 
`cognitive functioning are more likely to discontinue 
`(Robinson et al, 2002). 
`
`Why don’t patients take their medicine?
`
`Lack of insight has been extensively examined 
`but other factors have been relatively overlooked. 
`Perkins  (2002)  reviewed  articles  published  up 
`to  December  2002.  Correlates  of  poor  adherence 
`included patients’ beliefs about their illness and the 
`benefits of treatment (insight into illness, belief that 
`medication can ameliorate symptoms), perceived 
`costs  of  treatment  (medication  side­effects),  and 
`barriers to treatment (ease of access to treatment, 
`degree  of  family  or  social  support).  More  recent 
`studies  suggest  that  medication  concerns  are  of 
`prime importance. In a sample of 213 patients with 
`schizophrenia, Lambert et al (2004) found that those 
`presenting  with  side­effects  and  those  with  past 
`experience of side­effects had a significantly more 
`negative  general  attitude  toward  antipsychotics. 
`Loffler  et al  (2003)  conducted  a  study  in  which 
`307 people with schizophrenia were asked about 
`their reasons for antipsychotic adherence or non­
`adherence, using the Rating of Medication Influences 
`scale. A positive relationship with the therapist and 
`a  positive  attitude  of  significant  others  towards 
`antipsychotic treatment contributed to adherence. 
`Reasons for non­adherence were lack of acceptance 
`of the necessity for pharmacological treatment and 
`lack of insight.
`
`Drugs and alcohol
`
`Batel et al (2004) examined risk factors of early drop­
`out during induction of high­dose buprenorphine 
`substitution therapy in 1085 individuals addicted to 
`opiates. Younger age, lack of social support and partial 
`access  to care (lack of health insurance, previous 
`contact  with  the  prescriber)  were  significantly 
`associated with early drop­out. Herbeck et al (2005) 
`looked at predictors in those with dual diagnosis. 
`Patients with treatment adherence problems were 
`significantly more likely to have personality disorders, 
`lower Global Assessment of Functioning scores, and 
`medication side­effects than those who adhered to 
`treatment.  In  a  study  of  after­care  attendance  by 
`severely substance­dependent residential treatment 
`clients, Sannibale et al (2003) found that younger, 
`male, heroin­dependent clients with poly­drug use 
`who  had  refused  opioid  pharmacotherapy  were 
`more likely to drop out of treatment and to relapse 
`early following treatment.
`
`Discussion
`
`Rates of non­adherence with psychotropic medication 
`are  difficult  to  summarise  because  they  vary  by 
`setting, diagnosis and type of adherence difficulty. 
`The overall weighted mean rate of non­adherence, 
`calculated in a sample of 23 796 patients with psychosis 
`
`Advances in Psychiatric Treatment (2007), vol. 13. http://apt.rcpsych.org/
`
`341
`
`

`
`a quarter of those discontinuing antidepressants or 
`antipsychotics  cite  adverse  effects  as  the  reason 
`(Maddox et al, 1994). In other words, only a minority 
`of those experiencing significant adverse effects will 
`discontinue medication without advice. 
`A  substantial  minority  (about  a  quarter  to  a 
`half) do not tell their doctor after stopping or inter­
`rupting medication (Hogman & Sandamas, 2000; 
`Demyttenaere et al, 2001). In part this may be fear 
`of rejection or being disbelieved, or embarrassment 
`about  discussing  adverse  effects  such  as  weight 
`gain or sexual problems (Zimmermann et al, 2003; 
`Weiden et al, 2004a). A contributing factor may be 
`that the frequency of side­effects such as these is 
`greatly underestimated by doctors (Smith et al, 2002; 
`Roose, 2003). About half of people who stop their 
`medication after taking it for more than a month 
`do so intentionally (Barber et al, 2004). Even when 
`patients have discontinued medication unexpectedly, 
`studies in general medicine suggest that more than 
`half will offer some kind of rational explanation. 
`
`Box 3 Simple strategies to improve
`concordance
`
`Basic communication
`Establish  a  therapeutic  relationship  and 
`trust
`Identify the patient’s concerns
`Take into account the patient’s preferences
`Explain  the  benefits  and  hazards  of  treat­
`ment options
`Strategy-specific interventions 
`Adjusting  medication  timing  and  dosage 
`for least intrusion
`Minimise adverse effects
`Maximise effectiveness
`Provide 
`support,  encouragement  and 
`follow­up
`Reminders
`Consider adherence aids such as medication 
`boxes and alarms
`Consider  reminders  via  mail,  email  or 
`telephone
`Home visits, family support, counselling
`Evaluating adherence
`Ask about problems with medication
`Ask specifically about missed doses
`Ask about thoughts of discontinuation
`With  the  patient’s  consent,  consider  direct 
`methods: pill counting, measuring serum or 
`urine drug levels
`Liaise  with  general  practitioners  and 
`pharmacists regarding prescriptions
`
`• • • • • • •
`
`•
`
`• • • •
`
`• • • •
`
`Mitchell & Selmes
`
`from a systematic literature review was 25.78% (Nose 
`et al,  2003).  Complete  discontinuation of  medica­
`tion is thought to lead to about one in ten hospital 
`admissions and one in five nursing home admissions 
`(Sullivan et al, 1990). Fenton et al (1997) found that 
`non­adherent individuals with schizophrenia have 
`a  3.7­fold  greater  risk  of  relapse  than  those  who 
`are adherent over 6–24 months. Where medication 
`(or  appointments)  are  missed  for  predominantly 
`illness­related reasons such as lack of insight, there 
`is a particularly high risk of readmission. Yet illness 
`severity probably accounts for a minority of cases 
`of poor adherence in the community (Maddox et
`al, 1994). Further, the impact may be ameliorated 
`if patients who have further symptoms seek help. 
`Unfortunately, adverse experiences with medication 
`may prejudice willingness to attend in the future 
`(Gonzalez et al, 2005). 
`
`Potential solutions
`A  comprehensive  summary  of  interventions  that 
`may reduce poor adherence is beyond the scope of 
`this article. Advanced strategies such as adherence 
`therapy  and  depot  medication  are  reviewed  by 
`O’Ceallaigh & Fahy (2001) and Nadeem et al (2006). 
`Many  of  the  simpler  interventions  available  to 
`enhance adherence are listed in Box 3.
`Barriers to healthcare are a reversible cause of poor 
`medication and appointment adherence. Common 
`factors  that  reliably  influence  adherence  include 
`patient expectation and knowledge (perception of 
`benefits and hazards of therapy), involvement in 
`medical decisions, availability of social support, and 
`complexity and duration of the prescribed regime. 
`In the past it has been common to blame the indi­
`vidual for discontinuing medication or dropping out 
`of treatment against medical advice (Demyttenaere, 
`1998).  In  addition,  patients  who  do  not  follow 
`medical  advice  are  likely  to  be  discharged  early, 
`compounding the problem. In the future this may 
`be seen as inadequate care. Not too long ago it was 
`considered unusual to give the patient information 
`about their condition or, in some cases, even to reveal 
`the  truth

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket