throbber
Designing Menu Selection Systems
`
`Ben Shneiderman
`Department of Computer Science, University of Maryland, College Park,
`MD 20742
`
`Menu selection systems reduce training and memoriza-
`tion, simplify entry of choices, and structure the user’s
`tasks. However, the use of menu selection is no guaran-
`tee that novices or experts will be satisfied or able to
`carry out their work. This article focuses on the multiple
`design issues in creating successful menu selection sys-
`tems. These include the primary issue of semantic orga-
`nization and the host of secondary
`issues such as
`response time and display rates, shortcuts for frequent
`users, titles, phrasing of menu items, graphic layout, and
`selection mechanisms. Novel approaches such as pop
`up menus and embedded menus are covered. Experimen-
`tal results and design guidelines are presented.
`
`1. Introduction
`they can
`Menu selection systems are attractive because
`eliminate
`training
`and memorization
`of complex
`com-
`mand sequences. When
`the menu
`items are written using
`familiar
`terminology,
`users can select an item easily and
`indicate
`their choice either with one or two keypresses or
`through
`the use of a pointing device. This simplified
`inter-
`action
`style reduces
`the possibility
`of keying errors and
`structures
`the
`task
`to guide
`the novice and
`intermittent
`user. With
`careful design
`and high speed
`interaction,
`menu selection can become appealing
`to expert frequent
`users as well.
`is often contrasted with
`by menu selection
`Interaction
`interaction
`by command
`language,
`but
`the distinctions
`are sometimes blurred. Typically menu selection
`requires
`a single keystroke, whereas commands may be lengthy.
`However, how would you classify a menu
`in which the user
`has to type a six or eight letter
`item? Typically, menu se-
`lection presents
`the choices on the display, whereas com-
`mands must be memorized. However,
`how would you
`classify a menu
`that offered 4 numbered
`choices and ac-
`cepted 10 more generic
`choices
`that are not displayed?
`How would you classify a system
`that offers single letter
`
`Received April 10, 1985: revised May 10, 1985; accepted
`
`June 20, 198.5.
`
`0 1986 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
`
`two-dimensional menus
`prompts? What about graphical,
`where selection
`is made by pointing with a mouse or on a
`touchscreen?
`Finally, what category
`is voice synthesis/
`recognition menu
`interaction?
`to
`it is more useful
`Rather
`than debate
`terminology,
`the system offers on
`maintain
`an awareness of how much
`the display at the moment
`the selection
`is made,
`the form
`and content of item selection,
`and what problem domain
`knowledge
`is necessary
`for users to succeed. Menu selec-
`tion is especially effective when users have little training,
`are intermittent
`in using
`the system, are unfamiliar with
`the terminology,
`and need help in structuring
`their deci-
`sion-making
`process.
`it does
`However,
`if a designer employs menu selection,
`not guarantee
`that
`the system will be appealing
`and easy
`to use. Effective menu selection systems emerge only after
`careful consideration
`and
`testing of numerous
`design
`is-
`sues such as semantic organization, menu system struc-
`ture,
`the number
`and sequence
`of menu
`items,
`titling,
`prompting
`format, graphic
`layout and design, phrasing of
`menu
`items,
`display
`rates,
`response
`time,
`shortcuts
`through
`the menus
`for knowledgeable
`frequent
`users,
`availability
`of help, and
`the selection mechanism
`(key-
`board, pointing devices,
`touchscreen,
`voice, etc.).
`
`2. Semantic Organization
`is to create a sen-
`The primary
`task for menu designers
`sible, comprehensible,
`memorable,
`and convenient
`se-
`mantic
`organization.
`Some
`lessons can be
`learned
`by
`studying
`the semantic decomposition
`of a book into chap-
`ters, a program
`into modules,
`the animal kingdom
`into
`species, or a Sears catalog
`into sections. Hierarchical
`de-
`compositions,
`natural
`and comprehensible
`to most peo-
`ple, are appealing because every item belongs
`to a single
`category. Unfortunately,
`in some applications
`an
`item
`may be difficult
`to classify as belonging
`to one category,
`and
`the temptation
`to duplicate
`entries or create a net-
`work increases. Despite
`some limitations,
`the elegance of
`tree structures
`should be appreciated.
`soups, main
`Restaurant menus
`separate
`appetizers,
`dishes, desserts,
`and drinks
`to help customers organize
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE. 37(2):57-70; 1986
`
`CCC 0002.8231/86/020057-14$04.00
`
`

`

`fit logically into cate-
`items should
`their selections. Menu
`gories and have
`readily understood meanings.
`Restau-
`rateurs who list dishes with
`idiosyncratic
`names
`such as
`“Veal Monique,”
`generic
`terms
`such
`as
`“house
`dressing,”
`or unfamiliar
`jargon
`such as “Wor Shu Op”
`should expect
`that waiters will spend ample time explain-
`ing the alternatives
`or should anticipate
`that customers
`will be anxious because of their
`insecurity
`in ordering.
`the
`Similarly,
`for computer menu
`selection
`systems,
`so
`categories
`should be comprehensible
`and distinctive
`that
`the users are confident
`in making
`their selections.
`Users should have a clear idea of what will happen when
`they make a choice. Computer menu selection systems are
`more difficult
`to design
`than
`restaurant menus because
`computer
`screens
`typically
`allow
`less information
`to be
`displayed
`than do printed menus. Screen space is a scarce
`resource.
`In addition,
`the number of choices and the com-
`plexity
`is greater
`in many computer
`applications,
`and the
`computer user may not have a helpful waiter to turn
`to for
`an explanation.
`of menu
`organization
`of meaningful
`The
`importance
`items was demonstrated
`in a study with 48 novice users
`[ 11. Simple menu
`trees with 3 levels and 16 target
`items
`were constructed
`in meaningfully
`organized
`and disor-
`ganized
`forms. Error rates were nearly halved,
`and user
`think
`time (time from menu presentation
`to user’s selec-
`tion of an
`item) was reduced
`for the meaningfully
`or-
`ganized
`form.
`In a later menu search study, McDonald
`et
`al.
`[2] found
`that
`semantically meaningful
`categories,
`such as food, animals, minerals,
`and cities,
`led to shorter
`response
`times
`than did random or alphabetic
`organiza-
`tions. This experiment
`tested 109 novice users who worked
`through 10 blocks of 26 trials. The authors conclude
`that
`“these
`results demonstrate
`the superiority of a categorical
`menu organization
`over a pure alphabetical
`organization,
`particularly when
`there
`is some uncertainty
`about
`the
`larger menu
`terms.” With
`structures
`the effect
`is even
`more dramatic,
`as has been demonstrated
`by studies with
`extensive videotext databases
`[3,4].
`[S]
`model
`These
`results
`and
`the syntactic/semantic
`is to first
`suggest
`that
`the key to menu
`structure
`design
`consider
`the semantic organization.
`The number of items
`on the screen becomes a secondary
`issue.
`Menu
`selection
`applications
`range from trivial choices
`between
`two
`items
`to complex
`videotex
`systems with
`300,000 screens. The simplest
`applications
`consist of a
`single menu, but even with this limitation
`there are many
`variations.
`The second group of applications
`includes
`a
`linear sequence
`of menu
`selections;
`the progression
`of
`tree
`menus
`is independent
`of the user’s choice. Strict
`structures make up the
`third group, which
`is the most
`situation. Acyclic
`common
`(these
`are menus
`that
`are
`than one path) and cyclic (there are
`reachable
`by more
`that allow users to repeat menus) net-
`meaningful
`paths
`works constitute
`the fourth group. These groupings
`de-
`scribe
`the semantic
`organization;
`special
`traversal
`com-
`mands may enable users to jump around
`the branches of a
`
`to the previous menu, or to go to the be-
`tree, to go back
`ginning of a linear sequence.
`
`(Y,N)?
`
`A. Single Menus
`to ac-
`is sufficient
`a single menu
`In some situations,
`complish
`a task. Single menus may have
`two or more
`items, may require
`two or more screens, or may allow mul-
`tiple selections. Single menus may pop up on the current
`work area or may be permanently
`available
`(in a separate
`window or on a data
`tablet) while
`the main display
`is
`changed. Different guidelines
`apply for each situation.
`Binary Menus
`The simplest menu
`is a binary menu
`with yes/no or true/false
`choices such as is found
`in many
`home computer games:
`DO YOU WANT
`INSTRUCTIONS
`or in a medical-history
`taking
`interview
`YOU HAVE HAD SURGERY TO REMOVE YOUR
`APPENDIX
`1 - TRUE
`2 - FALSE
`MAKE YOUR SELECTION NOW:
`In the
`Even
`these simple examples
`can be improved.
`first case, a novice user might not understand
`the (Y,N)
`prompt-really
`an abbreviated
`form of the menu
`of
`choices. Secondly
`this common query leaves the user with-
`out a clear sense of what is going to happen next. Typing
`“Y” might produce many pages of instructions
`and
`the
`user might not know how to stop a lengthy output. Typing
`“N” is also anxiety producing because
`the user has no idea
`of what
`the program will do. Even
`in writing
`simple
`menus, clear and specific choices should be offered which
`give the user the sense of control:
`Your choices are:
`1 - Get 12 lines of brief instructions
`2 - Get 89 lines of complete
`instructions
`3 - Go on to playing
`the game
`Type a number
`and press RETURN:
`Since this version has three
`items,
`it is no longer a binary
`menu.
`It offers more specific items so the user knows what
`to expect, but it still has the problem
`that users must take
`instructions
`now or never. Another
`strategy might be:
`At any time you may type
`?-
`to get 12 lines of brief instructions
`?? -
`to get 89 lines of complete
`instructions
`Be sure to press RETURN
`after every command
`Ready for game playing commands:
`This example calls attention
`to the sometimes narrow dis-
`tinction
`between
`commands
`and menu
`selection:
`the
`menu choices have become more command-like
`since the
`user must now recall the ? or ?? syntax.
`The following examples
`illustrate
`additional
`
`issues in
`
`58
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE--March
`
`1986
`
`

`

`items can be identified by single let-
`binary menus. Menu
`ter mnemonics,
`as in this photo
`library
`retrieval system:
`Photos are indexed by film type
`B Black and white
`C Color
`Type the letter of your choice
`and press RETURN:
`service:
`or in a shop-by-computer
`PLEASE SELECT THE DESIRED METHOD
`OF BILLING:
`MASTERCARD.............................1
`VISA.......................................2
`KEY IN YOUR SELECTION AND PRESS ENTER:
`The mnemonic
`letters in the photo menu are often pre-
`ferred
`to the numbered
`choices
`in the shop-by-computer
`menu (see Section 7). The long line of periods can be a dis-
`traction,
`and the uppercase-only
`lettering may slow read-
`ing. The mnemonic
`letter approach
`requires
`additional
`caution
`in avoiding
`collision and
`increases
`the effort of
`translation
`to foreign
`languages,
`but
`their clarity and
`memorability
`are an advantage
`in many
`applications.
`These simple examples
`demonstrate
`alternative ways to
`identify menu
`items and convey
`instructions
`to the user.
`No optimal
`format
`for menus has emerged,
`but consis-
`tency across menus
`in a system is extremely
`important.
`Multiple Item Menus Single menus may have more
`than
`two items. Examples
`include online quizzes:
`Who invented
`the telephone?
`1 Thomas Edison
`2 Alexander Graham Bell
`3 Lee De Forest
`4 George Westinghouse
`Type the number
`and press RETURN:
`or the list of options
`in a document
`processing
`EXAMINE, PRINT, DROP, OR HOLD?
`The quiz
`example
`has distinctive,
`comprehensible
`items, but the document
`processing example shows an im-
`plied menu
`selection
`that could be confusing
`to novice
`users. There are no explicit
`instructions,
`and it is not ap-
`parent
`that
`single
`letter
`abbreviations
`are acceptable.
`Knowledgeable
`and frequent
`users may prefer
`this short
`form of a menu selection, usually called a prompt,
`for its
`speed and simplicity.
`Extended Menus Sometimes
`items
`list of menu
`the
`may require more
`than one screen but allow only one
`meaningful
`item to be chosen. One resolution
`is to create a
`tree-structured
`menu, but sometimes
`the desire
`to keep
`is very appealing. The
`the system to one conceptual menu
`first portion of the menu
`is displayed with an additional
`menu
`item that
`leads to the next screen
`in the extended
`menu sequence. A typical application
`is in word process-
`ing systems, where common choices are displayed
`first but
`
`system:
`
`infrequent
`screen:
`
`or advanced
`
`features
`
`are kept on the second
`
`MAIN MENU
`
`SUPERDUPERWRITER
`PAGE 1
`Edit a file
`1
`Copy a file
`2
`Create a file
`3
`Erase a file
`4
`Print a file
`5
`View the directory
`6
`P2 Go to PAGE 2
`Type the number of your choice
`and Press RETURN
`
`MAIN MENU
`
`SUPERDUPERWRITER
`PAGE 2
`7 Alter line width
`set
`8 Change character
`recovery of damaged file
`9 Attempt
`10 Reconstruct
`erased file
`Set cursor blink
`rate
`11
`12
`Set beep volume
`13 Run diagnostics
`to PAGE 1
`Pl Go back
`Type the number of your choice
`and Press RETURN
`the extended
`screen menu will go on for
`Sometimes
`many screens of command
`items or data items. More elab-
`orate scrolling capabilities may be needed.
`Pop-up Menus The
`or “pull down”
`term “pop-up”
`menus refers to the process of forcing a menu to appear on
`the screen
`in response
`to a click with a pointing
`device
`such as a mouse. The Xerox Star, Apple Lisa, and Apple
`Macintosh
`(Figure
`1) made
`these possibilities widely
`available. There
`is a great satisfaction on the part of most
`users in making
`the menu appear
`rapidly. Selection can
`be made by moving
`the pointing
`device over the menu
`items, which respond by highlighting
`(inverse video, a box
`surrounding
`the item, or color have been used).
`The contents of the pop-up menu may depend on the
`position of the cursor when the pointing device is clicked.
`Since
`the pop-up menu covers a portion of the screen,
`there
`is a strong motivation
`to keep the menu
`text small.
`Hierarchical
`sequences
`are also used in pop-up menus.
`Permanent Menus Since menus can be used for per-
`manently
`available
`commands
`that can be applied
`to a
`displayed object. For example,
`the Bank Street Writer, a
`word processor
`designed
`for children,
`always shows a
`fragment of the text and this menu:
`
`ERASE
`UNERASE
`
`MOVE
`MOVEBACK
`
`TRANSFER
`FIND
`REPLACE MENU
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE-March
`
`1986
`
`59
`
`

`

`f6
`
`File
`
`Font
`
`Fonts
`
`iu 1 FatBits
`
`Show Page
`Edit Pattern
`Brush Shape
`Brush Mirrors
`Introduction
`Short Cuts
`
`FIG.
`
`I.
`
`Pulldown menu on the Apple Macintosh
`
`appears when the user clicks down on the mouse button.
`
`the left and right arrow keys causes items to be
`Moving
`highlighted
`in reverse video. When
`the de-
`sequentially
`sired command
`is highlighted,
`pressing
`the RETURN key
`initiates
`the action.
`include Apple
`of permanent menus
`Other applications
`MacPaint,
`computer-assisted
`design
`(CAD)
`systems, or
`other graphics
`systems
`that display an elaborate menu of
`commands
`to the side of the object being manipulated.
`Price [6] describes
`a CAD system with 120 choices
`in an
`on-screen menu. Lightpen
`touches or other cursor-action
`devices allow the user to make selections without using the
`keyboard.
`Multiple Selection Menus A further variation on sin-
`gle menus
`is the capacity
`to make multiple
`selections
`from
`the choices offered. For example,
`this menu from a politi-
`cal interest
`survey allows multiple
`choices on one touch
`screen:
`
`ISSUES
`
`POLITICAL
`HIGH UNEMPLOYMENT
`AID TO ELDERLY
`NUCLEAR FREEZE
`HIGH DEFENSE BUDGET
`GOVERNMENT
`REGULATION
`FOREIGN AID
`PERSONAL TAXES
`CIVIL DEFENSE
`RIGHT TO ABORTION
`CRIME CONTROL
`DONE
`MINORITY RIGHTS
`ISSUES THAT YOU
`TOUCH UP TO THREE
`IMPORTANT,
`FEEL ARE THE MOST
`AND THEN TOUCH DONE:
`
`is nicely handled with a multiple selection
`This situation
`single menu;
`it would have been cumbersome
`to ask 11
`binary choices when the user could not scan the full list of
`issues. The system might highlight
`already selected
`items
`with a check mark or bold face.
`Summary Even
`the case of single menus provides a
`rich domain
`for designers and human
`factors researchers.
`Questions of wording,
`screen
`layout, and selection mech-
`anism all emerge even in the simple case of choosing from
`one set of items. Still more challenging
`questions
`emerge
`from designing
`sequences
`and trees of menus.
`
`B. Linear Sequence of Menus
`menus can be used to
`Often a series of interdependent
`guide the user through a series of choices in which the user
`sees the same sequence of menus no matter what choices
`are made. A document
`printing package might have this
`linear sequence of menus:
`
`printed at
`
`Do you want the document
`1 - your terminal
`line printer
`center
`2 -
`the computer
`laser printer
`center
`3 -
`the computer
`Type the number of your choice and press RETURN:
`
`Do you want
`1 -
`single spacing
`2 - double spacing
`Type the number
`of your choice and press RETURN:
`
`60
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE-March
`
`1986
`
`

`

`Do you want
`1 - no page numbering
`2 - page numbering
`on the top, right justified
`3 - page numbering
`at the bottom,
`centered
`Type the number of your choice and press RETURN:
`Another example would be an online examination
`that
`had a sequence of multiple
`choice
`test items, each made
`up in the form of a menu.
`Movement Through the Menus Linear
`sequences
`guide
`the user through
`a complex decision-making
`pro-
`cess by presenting
`one decision at a time. The document-
`printing
`example could be improved by offering
`the user a
`mechanism
`for going back to previous menus
`to review or
`change
`choices made
`earlier. A second
`improvement
`would be to display the results of previous choices, so users
`could see what decisions had been made. A third improve-
`ment might be to let the users know how many and which
`menus are yet to be seen.
`traversal,
`allowing backward
`The
`first
`improvement,
`could be handled
`easily by changing
`the instructions
`to:
`Type
`the number
`of your choice and press RETURN,
`or type “B” and press RETURN
`to go back to the pre-
`vious menu:
`the record of previ-
`showing
`improvement,
`The second
`ous menus, could be handled by displaying
`the choices al-
`ready made. The third
`improvement,
`showing upcoming
`choices, could be handled by displaying
`a descriptive
`term
`about
`the menus
`to follow, or simply an indication
`that
`this is the third of six menus. Unfortunately,
`as more im-
`provements
`are made
`there
`is a greater possibility of cre-
`ating cluttered
`displays.
`Judgments
`based on experience
`can resolve many decisions, but experimental
`tests with
`alternative
`formats
`and several classes of users may be
`useful to guide designers.
`Summary Linear
`are a simple
`of menus
`sequences
`the user through
`a deci-
`and effective means
`for guiding
`sion-making
`process. The user should be given a clear
`sense of progress or position within
`the sequence and the
`means
`for going backwards
`to earlier choices (and possi-
`bly to terminating
`or restarting
`the sequence).
`is of-
`Choosing
`the order of menus
`in a linear sequence
`ten straightforward,
`but care must be taken
`to match user
`expectations. One strategy
`is to place
`the easy decisions
`first to relieve users of some concerns,
`enabling
`them
`to
`concentrate
`on more difficult choices.
`
`C. Tree-Structured Menus
`When a collection of items grows and becomes difficult
`to maintain
`under
`intellectual
`control, people form cate-
`gories of similar
`items,
`creating
`a tree structure
`[7,8].
`Some collections
`can be easily partitioned
`into mutually
`exclusive groups with distinctive
`identifiers.
`Familiar
`ex-
`amples
`include:
`Male, female
`Animal, vegetable, mineral
`
`a
`
`is
`for
`
`autumn, winter
`Spring, summer,
`Sunday, Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday,
`Friday, Saturday
`Less than 10, between 10 and 25, greater
`Percussion,
`string, woodwind, brass
`lead to confu-
`Even
`these groupings may occasionally
`indexing
`is a
`sion or disagreement.
`Classification
`and
`is no perfect
`complex
`task, and
`in many situations
`there
`solution acceptable
`to everyone. The initial design can be
`improved as a function of feedback
`from users. Over time,
`as the structure
`is improved
`and as users gain familiarity
`with it, success rates will improve.
`menu systems
`Despite
`their problems,
`tree-structured
`have the power to make
`large collections of data available
`to novice or intermittent
`users. If each menu has 8 items,
`then a menu
`tree with 4 levels has the capacity
`to lead an
`untrained
`user
`to the right frame out of a collection of
`4096 frames.
`and compre-
`at each level are natural
`If the groupings
`hensible
`to the user, and if the user knows what he/she
`is
`looking for, then the menu
`traversal can be accomplished
`in a few seconds-more
`quickly
`than
`flipping
`through
`book. On the other hand,
`if the groupings
`are unfamiliar
`and
`the user has only a vague notion of what he/she
`looking for, it is possible
`to get lost in the tree menus
`hours [9].
`Depth Versus Breadth The depth
`(number of levels)
`of a menu
`tree depends,
`in part, on the breadth
`(number
`of items per level).
`If more
`items are put
`into the main
`menu,
`then the tree spreads out and has fewer levels. This
`is advantageous,
`but not if clarity is substantially
`compro-
`mised or if a slow display
`rate consumes
`the user’s pa-
`tience. Several authors have urged four to eight items per
`menu, but at the same time they urge no more than three
`to four levels. With
`large menu applications,
`oue or both
`of these guidelines must be compromised.
`in retriev-
`D. P. Miller [lo] studied user performance
`ing items from 4 versions of a tree-structured menu system
`containing
`64 target
`items. Menus had 2,4, 8, or 64 items
`in each screen, with corresponding
`depths of 6, 3, 2, and
`1. The 64 items were carefully
`chosen
`that “they
`form
`valid semantic
`hierarchies”
`in each of the 4 versions.
`Speed of performance was fastest with 4 or 8 items per
`menu, and the lowest error rate occurred with 8 items per
`menu. These results are useful, but there were two special
`conditions
`that may limit
`the applicability
`of this study:
`subjects became very familiar with the menus during
`the
`training
`and 128 trials, and
`the 64 items were chosen so
`that there were meaningful
`groupings
`in all 4 versions.
`Kiger [ 1 l] grouped 64 items in 5 menu
`tree forms:
`8-2: 8 items on each of 2 levels
`4-3: 4 items on each of 3 levels
`2-6: 2 items on each of 6 levels
`4-l + 16-1: A 4 item menu followed by a 16 item
`menu
`16-1 + 4-l : A 16 item menu
`menu
`
`than 25
`
`followed by a 4 item
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE-March
`
`1986
`
`61
`
`

`

`least
`the slowest,
`tree, 2-6, produced
`The deep narrow
`accurate,
`and least preferred version, whereas
`the 8-2 was
`among
`the best for speed, accuracy,
`and preference. The
`22 subjects performed
`16 searches on each of the 5 ver-
`sions.
`a one-level menu having 23
`Dray et al. [12] compared
`one-word
`target
`items arranged on 6 lines with a two-level
`menu having 6 items in the main menu. Selection was by
`cursor control arrow keys and an ENTER key. Subjects
`had 138 trials
`in each condition
`in this counterbalanced
`within subjects design. Although neither version emerged
`as superior,
`there was a significant
`order effect
`that
`the
`authors
`interpreted
`as evidence
`that
`the one-level menu
`was easier
`to learn.
`Informal
`reports
`from subjects
`sup-
`ported
`the conclusion
`that continuously
`seeing
`the full
`picture aided decision making.
`l-4096,
`the numbers
`When
`the menu
`tree contains
`time to locate a target number was found
`to increase with
`the breadth
`of the
`tree
`[13]. Search
`times were almost
`twice as long in a 12 level tree having 2 choices at each level
`(2-12) as opposed
`to a 3 level tree with 16 choices at each
`level (16-3). Although
`the 6 subjects made choices more
`rapidly
`in the shorter menus,
`the effort to work through
`the more numerous menus did slow them down substan-
`tially. Each subject did 12 trials with each of 4 widths.
`While
`the semantic
`structure of the items cannot be ig-
`nored,
`these studies suggest
`that fewer levels aid decision
`making. Of course,
`display
`rates,
`response
`time,
`and
`screen clutter must be considered
`in addition
`to the se-
`mantic organization.
`Semantic Grouping in Tree Structures Rules for se-
`mantic validity are hard
`to state, and there
`is always the
`danger
`that some users may not grasp
`the designer’s or-
`ganizational
`framework. Young and Hull [14] examined
`“cognitive mismatches”
`in the British Prestel viewdata
`system
`[ 151. Problems
`included
`overlapping
`categories,
`extraneous
`items, conflicting
`classifications
`in the same
`menu,
`unfamiliar
`jargon,
`and generic
`terms. Based on
`this set of problems,
`the rules
`for forming menu
`trees
`might be:
`
`1. Create groups of logically similar items. For exam-
`ple, a comprehensible menu would list countries at
`level one, states or provinces at level two, and cities
`at level three.
`that cover all possibilities. For exam-
`2. Form groups
`ple, a menu with age ranges O-9, 10-19, 20-29,
`and older
`than 30 makes
`it easy for the user
`to
`select an item.
`3. Make sure that items are non-overlapping.
`Lower-
`level items should be naturally
`associated with a
`single higher-level
`item. Young and Hull offered
`an example
`of a poorly designed
`screen with
`“Places
`in Britain”
`and “Regions of England”
`as
`overlapping
`items on the same menu.
`items
`that
`4. Use familiar
`terminology,
`but ensure
`from each other. Choosing
`are dtitinctive
`the right
`
`from sam-
`task; feedback
`is a difficult
`terminology
`ple users will be helpful during design and testing.
`Menu Maps As the depth of a menu
`tree grows, it be-
`comes
`increasingly
`difficult
`for the user
`to maintain
`a
`sense of position
`in the tree, and a sense of disorientation,
`or of “getting
`lost,” grows. To overcome
`this sense of dis-
`orientation,
`some menu systems come with a printed
`in-
`dex of terms
`that
`is easier to scan than a series of screen
`displays. The British Prestel system offers a detailed cross
`referenced
`index
`that
`in 1982 was 34 pages long and con-
`tained
`thousands
`of entries. The CompuServe
`Informa-
`tion Service’s November
`1984
`index contained
`almost
`1000 subjects.
`It included
`a diagram, or map, of the first 3
`levels of the tree structure, which contains 26 menus.
`The relative merits of a map and an index were studied
`in a small menu structure with 18 animals as target
`items
`[ 161. In this case, users who had the chance
`to study an in-
`dex did somewhat better
`than a control group that had no
`special navigation
`aids. The group with an overall map
`did substantially
`better
`than both
`the index and control
`groups:
`
`Number of subjects
`Mean time per search
`Mean choices per search
`
`Control
`
`10
`35.3
`12.3
`
`Index
`
`8
`30.7
`8.4
`
`Map
`
`8
`19.2
`4.7
`
`w as
`
`(3-3) menu
`
`the menus
`
`three-item
`for a three-level
`learning
`Menu
`studied with four forms of training
`[17]:
`Online exploration. Subjects could explore
`online.
`sequences.
`Command
`the 27 paths
`studied
`Subjects
`typed on paper, e.g., Plans Division, Concepts, Sys-
`tems Analyst
`Frames. Subjects
`Plans Division
`Concepts
`Designs
`Proposals
`Menu map. Subjects
`of the 13 frames.
`train-
`subjects had a 12 minute
`The 65 undergraduate
`ing period,
`followed by a 10 minute work period. The re-
`sults indicate
`a strong advantage
`for those who had the
`menu map:
`
`studied
`
`the 13 menu frames, such as
`
`studied
`
`a tree-structured
`
`layout
`
`Online
`Explora-
`tion
`
`8.2
`
`10.6
`10.1
`
`3.6
`
`Command
`Sequences
`Study
`4.7
`20.4
`
`8.4
`
`3.1
`
`Frames
`Study
`6.5
`19.6
`9.8
`
`2.8
`
`MCIlU
`Map
`Study
`
`8.5
`9.4
`16.7
`
`4.8
`
`Sig-
`nifi-
`cance
`
`N.S.
`p <
`.lO
`p < .05
`
`p <
`
`.Ol
`
`Targets found
`Average number of
`menus visited
`Recall of tree
`(max = 27)
`Satisfaction
`(best = 5)
`
`As the tree structure grows, users have greater difficulty
`in maintaining
`an overall understanding
`of the semantic
`
`62
`
`JOURNAL OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY FOR INFORMATION SCIENCE-March
`
`1986
`
`

`

`organization. Viewing the structure one menu at a time is
`like seeing the world through
`a cardboard
`tube:
`it’s hard
`to grasp
`the overall pattern
`and see relationships
`among
`categories. Offering a spatial map can help overcome
`this
`difficulty.
`Semantic Versus Alphabetic Organization Since the
`creation of a universally
`acceptable
`semantic decomposi-
`tion with several tree levels is a challenge,
`some designers
`have attempted
`an
`index
`strategy
`that provides
`a tree
`structure based on simple alphabetic
`organization
`of the
`target
`items.
`in the use of a
`tested
`There were 30 student volunteers
`tree-structured
`database of 470 index pages and an alpha-
`betic
`index of 453 terms
`[18]. A typical
`search of both
`forms proceeded
`as follows:
`
`User sees:
`
`User selects 2 and sees:
`
`Tree Search for Baseball Scores
`1. News
`2. sports
`3. Entertainment
`SPORTS
`1. Hockey
`2. Baseball
`3.
`. . .
`BASEBALL
`1. Scores
`2. Standings
`3.
`. . .
`BASEBALL SCORES
`
`User selects 2 and sees:
`
`User selects 1 and sees:
`
`User selects 1 and sees:
`
`Alphabetic Search for Baseball Scores
`1. A-B-C
`User sees:
`2. D-E
`3.
`. . .
`1. A
`2. B
`3. c
`DIRECTORY
`. . .
`. . . . . . . .22114
`Banks
`Baseball.
`. . . . .221313
`Books.
`. . . . . . . . .2516
`
`User selects 2 and sees:
`
`FOR B
`
`User keys 221313 and sees:
`
`User selects 1 and sees:
`
`BASEBALL
`1. Scores
`2. Standings
`3.
`. . .
`BASEBALL SCORES
`
`half
`design,
`this counterbalanced-within-subjects
`In
`the subjects began with one method and then tried the sec-
`ond method, while the other half of the subjects worked in
`the opposite order. Subjects performed
`20 searches, and
`no significant differences were found
`in mean search time,
`number of keypresses, or number of menus accessed. Un-
`der both conditions,
`subjects
`“required
`about
`twice the
`
`to find the informa-
`number of pages necessary
`minimum
`tion” and “they made one or more errors on 40% of the
`questions.” Users eventually
`succeeded
`in 98.7% of the
`questions,
`so that, although performance was far from op-
`timal,
`successful
`searching was possible with both meth-
`ods. Subjective evaluations
`did not favor one method over
`the other, but when one method
`required more pages for a
`specific question,
`the preference was for
`the
`shorter
`method.
`that offering
`[ 181 conjectured
`and McEwen
`Tombaugh
`subjects
`can
`both methods may be the best resolution:
`for each ques-
`choose the method
`that
`is most appealing
`leads to difficulty,
`then the users can
`tion.
`If one method
`try the other.
`Summary There
`that
`structure
`is no perfect menu
`do-
`matches every person’s knowledge of the application
`initial
`main. Designers must use good judgment
`for the
`implementation
`but
`then be receptive
`to suggested
`im-
`provements
`and empirical data. Users will gradually gain
`familiarity,
`even with extremely
`complex
`tree structures,
`and will be increasingly
`successful
`in locating
`required
`items.
`
`D. Acyclic and Cyclic Menu Networks
`some-
`Although
`tree structures
`are very appealing,
`For ex-
`times network
`structures
`are more appropriate.
`ample,
`it might make sense to provide access to banking
`information
`from both
`the financial
`and consumer parts
`of a tree structure. A second motivation
`for networks
`is
`that it may be desirable
`to permit paths between disparate
`sections of a tree rather
`than
`requiring
`users to begin a
`new search from the main menu. These and other condi-
`tions
`lead to network
`structures
`in the form of acyclic or
`even cyclic graphs. As users move from trees to acyclic net-
`works
`to cyclic networks,
`the potential
`for getting
`lost
`increases.
`the user can form a mental model
`With a tree structure,
`of the structure
`and
`the relationship
`among
`the menus.
`Developing
`this mental model may be more difficult with
`a network. With a tree structure,
`there
`is a single parent
`menu,
`so backward
`traversals
`toward
`the main menu are
`straightforward.
`In networ

This document is available on Docket Alarm but you must sign up to view it.


Or .

Accessing this document will incur an additional charge of $.

After purchase, you can access this document again without charge.

Accept $ Charge
throbber

Still Working On It

This document is taking longer than usual to download. This can happen if we need to contact the court directly to obtain the document and their servers are running slowly.

Give it another minute or two to complete, and then try the refresh button.

throbber

A few More Minutes ... Still Working

It can take up to 5 minutes for us to download a document if the court servers are running slowly.

Thank you for your continued patience.

This document could not be displayed.

We could not find this document within its docket. Please go back to the docket page and check the link. If that does not work, go back to the docket and refresh it to pull the newest information.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

You need a Paid Account to view this document. Click here to change your account type.

Your account does not support viewing this document.

Set your membership status to view this document.

With a Docket Alarm membership, you'll get a whole lot more, including:

  • Up-to-date information for this case.
  • Email alerts whenever there is an update.
  • Full text search for other cases.
  • Get email alerts whenever a new case matches your search.

Become a Member

One Moment Please

The filing “” is large (MB) and is being downloaded.

Please refresh this page in a few minutes to see if the filing has been downloaded. The filing will also be emailed to you when the download completes.

Your document is on its way!

If you do not receive the document in five minutes, contact support at support@docketalarm.com.

Sealed Document

We are unable to display this document, it may be under a court ordered seal.

If you have proper credentials to access the file, you may proceed directly to the court's system using your government issued username and password.


Access Government Site

We are redirecting you
to a mobile optimized page.





Document Unreadable or Corrupt

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket

We are unable to display this document.

Refresh this Document
Go to the Docket